Marathon performance as a predictor of competitiveness and training in men and women.
Deaner, Robert O. ; Masters, Kevin S. ; Ogles, Benjamin M. 等
Gender differences in competitiveness have been frequently
reported. In general, males are more likely than females to compete for
status, whereas females are more likely to compete for attributions of
attractiveness or sexual exclusiveness (Buss, 2008; Campbell, 2004).
Furthermore, males are more likely to compete using direct means, such
as aggression or physical displays, while females more frequently
compete by indirect means, such as ostracizing rivals (Bjorkqvist, 1994;
Buss, 2008; Campbell, 2004; cf. Hawley, Little, & Card, 2008).
Because sports can be considered, at least in part, formalized
physical competitions for status (Miller, 2000; Williams, Park, &
Wieling, 2010), males can be expected to show greater sports
competitiveness. Although several studies have found this gender
difference (Cashdan, 1998; Gill, 1986; Weinberg, Tenenbaum, McKenzie,
Jackson, Anshel, Grove, & Fogarty, 2003), exceptions have been
reported (Longhurst & Spink, 1987). Furthermore, it remains unclear
what social and biological factors produce the gender difference.
Progress in understanding the generality of the gender difference
in competitiveness and the factors that produce it might be made if an
approach were developed to readily quantify competitiveness across
contexts. Deaner (2006a) hypothesized that performances in endurance
sports, specifically distance running, could serve as a basis for such
an approach. This hypothesis is predicated on the well established links
among performance, training, and motivation: greater competitiveness is
associated with larger training volumes (Masters, Ogles, & Jolton,
1993; Ogles & Masters, 2000; Ogles & Masters, 2003; Ogles,
Masters, & Richardson, 1995;); larger training volumes are
associated with faster performances (Hagan, Upton, Duncan, &
Gettman, 1987; Masters et al., 1993; Slovic, 1997); and faster
performances are associated with greater competitiveness (Masters et
al., 1993; Ogles & Masters, 2000; Ogles & Masters, 2003). Given
these relationships, if a difference in the occurrence of fast male and
female runners were found, it should estimate a gender difference in
motivation to compete and maintain large training volumes.
Deaner (2006a) noted that absolute comparisons of male and female
running performances are unwarranted because, all else being equal,
males are expected to run substantially faster than females due to
hormonally regulated differences in aerobic capacity and body fat
deposition (Shephard, 2000; Willmore & Costill, 2004). Nevertheless,
because gender differences in world-class performance have stabilized at
roughly 10 - 12% across all distances (Noakes, 2001; Seiler &
Sailer, 1997; Sparling, O'Donnell, & Snow, 1998)
gender-specific worldclass performances can be used as denominators in
making relative comparisons across genders. For example, if 20 men ran
within 2% of the male world record in a given event during one
particular year while 10 women ran within 2% of the female world record
that year, one could say that twice as many men ran relatively fast.
Using this approach, Deaner (2006a, 2006b) demonstrated a highly
robust gender difference: across virtually all distances, in matched
populations of elite, sub-elite, and recreational U.S. runners, two to
four times as many males as females ran relatively fast in 2003. Given
the relationships among performance, training, and motivation noted
above and the fact that male runners generally report greater
competitiveness (Callen, 1983; Johnsgard, 1985; Ogles & Masters,
2003) and larger training volumes (Callen, 1983; Clement, Taunton,
Smart, & McNicol, 1981; Ogles et al., 1995), Deaner (2006a, 2006b)
interpreted the gender difference in relative performance as
representing a gender difference in competitiveness and training
commitment. Although this interpretation is consistent with previous
research, alternative explanations for the pattern warrant attention.
One possibility is that males and females might show differential
responses to training. In particular, for any given level of training,
males might generally perform closer to gender-specific world-class
standards. Challenging this idea are several studies indicating that
when fitness-matched males and females undertake aerobic training
programs, they show highly similar physiological responses and relative
performance gains (Dolgener, Kolkhorst, & Whitsett, 1994; Skinner,
Jaskolski, Jaskolska, Krasnoff, Gagnon, Leon, Rao, Wilmore, &
Bouchard, 2001; Wilmore & Costill, 2004). Nevertheless, these
studies had modest sample sizes. In addition, they generally did not
express performance gains in terms of gender-specific world-class
running standards (cf. Dolgener et al., 1994). Thus, the hypothesis that
males and females might show differential responses to training requires
further investigation.
Similarly, although previous work indicates that greater
competitiveness is associated with faster running performances (Masters
et al., 1993; Ogles & Masters, 2000; Ogles & Masters, 2003), it
is possible that the relationships differ for males and females,
especially when relative measures of performance are employed. Thus,
although there is a gender difference in relative performance, this
might not automatically indicate a difference in male and female
competitiveness.
Therefore, the goal of this cross-sectional study, based on
previously published data, is to test whether the relationships between
relative performance and training volume and relative performance and
competitiveness differ between male and female marathoners.
Method
Participants
Data used in this study is an amalgamation of information used in
previous studies (Masters & Ogles, 1998; Masters et al., 1993; Ogles
et al., 1995), raising the issue of previous/ duplicate publication
(American Psychological Association, 2010, pp. 13-14). However, none of
these previous studies computed relative performance or assessed the
relationships between relative performance and training volume or
relative performance and competitiveness. More importantly, none of
these previous studies addressed the central question of this paper,
whether these relationships differ among men and women
Participants in marathons in the Midwestern and Southeastern U.S.
were recruited during pre-race registration. While registering, they
were asked to take home and anonymously complete a demographic and
training questionnaire and, in some cases, the Motivations of
Marathoners Scales (MOMS; Masters et al., 1993). They were asked to
return these materials by mail. Across all of these studies, roughly 33%
of runners who were approached agreed to participate. Although the data
used in this study were published 12-17 years ago and are therefore
somewhat dated, we know of no reason why the fundamental relationships
among the variables of interest would have changed since then; we thus
believe our conclusions will still hold in the present.
The present study only included runners who reported a previous
marathon time, a weekly training volume, and three to 12 years of
running experience. The final sample included 694 men and 150 women; 518
men and 103 women completed the MOMS. Participants were predominantly
Caucasian (95%), and their ages ranged from 16 to 79 years. Their best
finishing times in previous marathons averaged three hours 23 minutes
(SD = 37 min) and ranged from two hours 15 minutes to seven hours four
minutes. Prior to the marathon, runners were training an average of 71.6
km/wk (SD = 25.4 km); 4% reported training less than 30 km/wk.
Instruments
The demographic and training questionnaire included many items, but
the relevant ones for this study are age, gender, ethnicity, number of
previous marathons attempted, best lifetime finishing time in a previous
marathon, mean finishing time in all previous marathons, and distance,
hours and days training per week during training for the upcoming
marathon. Because the study was completed anonymously, information was
not available on participants' performance in the marathon that
occurred the day after they received the questionnaires. To partially
overcome the problem that runners' best lifetime performances may
have occurred many years ago, we restricted the analysis to runners who
reported on their survey (completed in late 1980s to early 1990s) that
they had, at that time, run 12 years or less. (1) Because runners'
performances are expected to improve as they gain experience, we
attempted to minimize variation due to experience by only including
runners with three or more years of experience.
The MOMS consists of 56 items that are rated as to the degree to
which the runner considers them a reason for training and running in a
marathon, items represent nine internally consistent motivational
scales: affiliation, competition, health orientation, life meaning,
personal goal achievement, psychological coping, recognition,
self-esteem, and weight concern. Each item is rated on a one (not a
reason) to seven (a very important reason) scale. The score for each
scale is calculated by averaging the score for each item included in the
scale. Evidence for the internal consistency (Cronbach's alphas
range from .80 to .93), test-retest reliability (rs range from .71 to
.90), and factorial and construct validity of the scales has been
presented previously (Masters & Ogles, 1995; Masters et al., 1993;
Ogles et al., 1995). In this study, only the motivation for competition
scale was considered (alpha = .83; r = .90). This scale consists of the
following four items: (1) to compete with others; (2) to see how high I
can place in races; (3) to get a faster time than my friends; and (4) to
beat someone I've never beaten before.
Statistical Analysis
To assess performance, we used lifetime best, rather than mean,
marathon performance because only 94% of those reporting lifetime best
performance also reported mean performance. A second reason for using
lifetime best performance is that mean performance could be sensitive to
the occurrence of one or more unusually slow races due to injury or
sub-optimal race conditions. To make comparisons of relative performance
across males and females, we divided best performances by a
gender-specific world-class performance standard. Because current world
records could be biased against females (Seiler & Sailer, 1997), we
used the 10Fastest standard (Deaner 2006a), which is defined as the mean
best time of the 10 fastest performers in the world in that event (only
one performance included per individual). For initial analyses we used
the "all-time" 10-Fastest standard, which was 2:05:52 for
males and 2:19:50 for females as of April 1, 2005. (data from
MarathonGuide, 2010). We also repeated some analyses (see below), using
only the best times of 2004, which produced a 10-Fastest standard of
2:06:54 for males and 2:23:11 for females. (2)
To estimate runners' training volume (hereafter training
volume) in the months preceding their completion of the questionnaires,
we used distance run per week, rather than days or hours run. We used
distance because almost all (99%) marathoners in our original sample
reported training volume in distance, whereas fewer reported training in
duration (days: 48%; hours: 83%); we excluded the 1% of runners who did
not report training distance. In addition, training distance is known to
be highly correlated with other training indices, including the
frequency and intensity of training (Ogles & Masters, 2003; Slovic,
1977).
We used linear regression, rather than correlation, to examine the
relationships between relative lifetime performance and each of the
variables of interest (training volume, motivation for competition).
This allowed us to subsequently employ homogeneity of slopes and
analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) models to test whether regression slopes
and intercepts differed between males and females. For all analyses, we
used two-tailed statistical tests and set [alpha] at 0.05. All analyses
were conducted with Statistica 6.1 (Statsofi Inc.,Tulsa, OK USA)
Results
Relative Performance, Running Volume and Motivational Variables
To assess the predictiveness of relative best lifetime performance
(hereafter relative performance), we first regressed upon it training
volume and motivation for competition (hereafter competitiveness). For
both males and females, faster relative performances (shorter marathon
durations) significantly predicted larger training volumes (Figure 1;
Table 1) and greater competitiveness (Figure 2; Table 1).
To test if the regression slopes differed significantly for males
and females, we used separate homogeneity of slopes models. Neither of
the gender by dependent variable interactions approached significance
(training volume: F(4,843) = 0.7,p = .39; competitiveness: F(4,617) =
0.2, p = .62), indicating that the relationships between relative
performance and training volume and relative performance and
competitiveness were not moderated by gender. We then used separate
ANCOVA models to test whether the intercepts of the regressions
differed. There was no significant main effect of gender for training
volume (F(3,844) = 1.5, p = .23; male adjusted mean = 72.1 km/wk, SD =
26.1; female adjusted mean = 69.7, SD = 22.5). Nevertheless, after
taking the effect of relative performance into account, males reported
significantly greater competitiveness (F(3,618) = 6.4, p = .01; male
adjusted mean = 3.2, SD = 1.5 ; female adjusted mean = 2.8, SD = 1.5;
see Figure 2).
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Age and Experience
Age and experience are known to be related to running performance
and motivation (Hagan et al., 1987; Masters & Ogles, 1995; Ogles
& Masters, 2000; Slovic, 1977).To test whether our results might be
sensitive to these potential confounds, we first performed separate
linear regressions for training volume and competitiveness on age and
two assays of experience--number of previous marathons attempted and
number of years running. We performed each of the six regressions
independently for males and females and found the following: age was a
significant predictor of training volume for males and competitiveness
for females, number of previous marathons was a significant predictor of
training volume and competitiveness for males, but number of years
running was not a significant predictor of either dependent variable,
for either males or females (Table 2).
We then returned to the homogeneity of slopes and ANCOVA models,
but this time added either age or number of previous marathons. (Number
of years running was not explored since it was not predictive.) When
age, relative performance, and gender were entered as independent
variables and training volume was entered as the dependent variable in a
homogeneity of slopes model, there were no significant interactions
involving gender (all ps >. 14); ANCOVA with these variables
indicated no gender difference in training volume (F(4,840) = 2.4, p =.
12; male adjusted mean = 72.3, SD = 26.0 ; female adjusted mean = 69.0,
SD= 22.5). With the same independent variables and competitiveness as
the dependent variable, there were again no significant interactions
involving gender (all ps > .62); however, ANCOVA with these variables
indicated greater competitiveness among males, just as was found above
in a model without age (F(4,616) = 6.8, p = .01; male adjusted mean =
3.2, SD = 1.5 ; female adjusted mean = 2.8, SD = 1.5; see Figure 2).
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
We then repeated these analyses using number of previous marathons,
rather than age, and obtained virtually identical results (training
volume: homogeneity of slopes, all ps > .52, ANCOVA (F(4,830) = 1.9,
p = .16; competitiveness: homogeneity of slopes, all ps > .59, ANCOVA
(F(4,608) = 6.0, p = .01, adjusted means and SDs as in previous ANCOVA).
In summary, age and experience did not moderate the relationships among
relative performance, gender, and either of the two dependent variables
of interest, training volume and competitiveness.
Alternative Performance Measures
We next explored whether our results were sensitive to the
particular relative performance measure that we employed. We did this by
repeating our analyses using the 2004 10-Fastest standard as the
denominator in relative performance calculations, rather than the
all-time 10-Fastest standard. For males, the 2004 10-Fastest standard is
1.5% greater in duration than the all-time 10-Fastest standard, while it
is 3% greater for females. Thus, using the 2004 10-Fastest standard
makes female performances somewhat "faster" compared to male
performances.
For both males and females, the regression slopes for all dependent
variables on relative performance based on the 2004 10-Fastest standard
were identical to those computed with the all-time 10-Fastest standard
(see Table 1). ANCOVA revealed that after controlling for performance
with this alternative standard, there was again no evidence of a gender
difference in training volume (F(3,844) = 2.9, p = .09; male adjusted
mean = 72.3, SD = 26.1; female adjusted mean = 68.9, SD = 22.5). As was
found above, however, ANCOVA showed that, after controlling for
performance with this alternative standard, males reported greater
competitiveness (F(3,618) = 7.6, p = .006; male adjusted mean = 3.2, SD
= 1.5 ; female adjusted mean = 2.7, SD = 1.5). We repeated these
analyses after separately entering age and number of previous marathons
into the ANCOVAs and found that the results did not change. These
results suggest that the kinds of analyses performed here are generally
insensitive to the particular world-class standard employed in deriving
relative measures.
To further assess the importance of employing relative performance
measures, we repeated these analyses with absolute best marathon times,
i.e., not using a relative standard, a procedure that makes female
performances substantially "slower" compared to male
performances. Regression slopes again were unaffected by using a new
performance measure. However, unlike the all of the analyses presented
above, ANCOVA revealed that after controlling for performance with this
alternative standard, females reported larger training volumes (F(3,844)
= 5.6, p = .02; male adjusted mean = 70.8, SD = 26.1; female adjusted
mean = 75.6, SD = 22.5). Also contrary to previous analyses, gender
difference in competitiveness no longer reached, or even approached,
significance (F(3,618) = 0.6, p = .42; male adjusted mean = 3.1, SD =
1.5 ; female adjusted mean = 3.0, SD = 1.5). We again repeated these
analyses after separately entering age and number of previous marathons
into the ANCOVAs and found that the results did not change. The key
point is that when assessing gender differences, the choice of using an
absolute or a relative performance measure can affect the results.
Discussion
This study demonstrates that relative (lifetime best) performance
does indeed predict competitiveness and training in a large sample of
male and female marathon runners. Because the regression slopes did not
differ between males and females, for either training volume or
competitiveness, these results suggest that relative performance can
serve as an estimator of gender differences for both variables of
interest, as Deaner (2006a) hypothesized. An additional and crucial
question for Deaner's hypothesis is whether the intercepts of the
regressions differ. Because Deaner had interpreted the finding that a
larger proportion of male runners run relatively fast as indicating that
more males are motivated by competition and maintain large training
volumes, the most damaging possibility for this hypothesis would be if
relative performance were found to systematically underestimate female
competitiveness and/ or training volume. In fact, the regression
intercepts for training volume did not differ for males and females
(Figure 1), although the intercepts differed for competitiveness, such
that relative performance somewhat underestimated male competitiveness
(Figure 2.) This poses no great difficulty for Deaner's hypothesis;
it merely suggests that a male bias in relative performance depth may
somewhat underestimate the male bias in competitiveness.
Absolute vs. Relative Performance
One of this study's pivotal claims is that relative
performance should be superior to absolute performance in terms of
providing an unbiased predictor of training volume and competitiveness
across genders. Our results clearly supported this claim for training
volume because, with either relative performance measure, relative
performance predicted training volume for males and females in an
unbiased fashion. By contrast, absolute performance significantly
underestimated female training volume. In other words, this result
implies that to achieve any given marathon finishing time, a typical
woman will have to train more than a typical man. This makes perfect
sense given males' physiological advantages for distance running
(Shephard, 2000; Wilmore & Costill, 2004) and their consequently
faster world-class performances (Noakes, 2001; Seiler & Sailer,
1997; Sparling et al., 1998).
By contrast, the fact that relative performance underestimated male
competitiveness, whereas absolute performance was an unbiased predictor,
does not support the use of relative performance measures. In addition,
this result raises the question of why a typical female can generally
run as relatively fast as a typical male with apparently less
competitive motivation. We cannot definitively answer this question but
can provide a plausible speculation.
To begin we note that for both males and females, competitiveness
was only modestly associated with relative performance (Table 1.)
Moreover, further analyses of our data set showed that, as expected
(Masters et al., 1993; Ogles & Masters, 2000; Ogles & Masters,
2003; Ogles et al., 1995), competitiveness was a significant predictor
of training volume, but that the strength of the relationship was quite
weak (males: [R.sup.2] = .06; females: [R.sup.2] = .02). (We also
analyzed other MOMS scales and found that some scales (e.g., personal
goal orientation, social recognition) also significantly predicted
training, but these associations were even weaker than for
competitiveness.)
We believe that such modest associations should not be taken to
imply that competitive motivation, or motivation in general, is largely
irrelevant to training; instead we believe that a one-time-only,
self-report questionnaire, even if well designed, can only measure a
small portion of the many internal variables that govern
individuals' decisions about how they will or will not train over
the extended periods necessary for achieving fast running performances.
In fact, we suggest that, to a large extent, an individual's
training represents the sum total of his or her actual motivation to
compete and excel. We offer this argument as a parallel to the economics
concept of revealed preference, where instead of asking individuals how
much they value a good, behavioral economists measure how much they will
pay or work for it (Aharon, Etcoff, Ariely, Chabris, O'Connor,
& Breiter, 200l; Samuelson, 1938).
In our view, the fact that relative performance predicts training
volume in an unbiased fashion suggests that relative performance
actually does assess motivation in an unbiased fashion, even if, for
unknown reasons, males do tend to report greater competitive motivation
than would be expected with the MOMS questionnaire. From a practical
point of view, we believe our results support the idea that researchers
should use relative rather than absolute performance measures when
comparing males and females. At the very least, we suggest that they
make relative comparisons in addition to absolute ones.
Limitations
We found that age and experience did not affect this study's
main conclusions. Nevertheless, this study's cross-sectional design
requires that we interpret our findings cautiously, because it remains
possible that male and female marathoners differed in ways we could not
control. For instance, if the females in our study tended, for some
unknown reason, to be unusually responsive to aerobic training or have
exceptionally efficient biomechanics (i.e., they were, in some way,
exceptionally "talented"), then this could lead to unrealistic
expectations of how more typical females might have performed with
similar training. With respect to the possibility of random error, we
note that our sample contained relatively few women (103 in
competitiveness analysis; 150 in training volume analysis vs. 518,650
for men). Unfortunately, conducting fully controlled, prospective
studies with large samples of individuals engaging in months or years of
athletic training is difficult because there can be substantial biases
in recruitment and retention (Dolgener et al., 1994).
A second limitation of this study--noted in the Methods section--is
that we asked runners to report their training and motivation just prior
to an upcoming marathon and to also report their best lifetime marathon
performance. This means that some runners would have reported modest
training volumes and competitiveness because their goal in the upcoming
marathon was merely to complete the distance; however, in the years
prior to completing the survey, these runners may have trained more
rigorously and ran much faster than they were prepared to run when they
completed the survey. We attempted to limit this problem by excluding
individuals who reported more than 12 years of running experience, but
it's likely that this issue would have affected our results, at
least to some degree.
In fact, the presence of some runners whose current training and
motivation does not correspond with their lifetime best performance
might explain why the association between training and performance in
our study, although substantial (males: [R.sup.2] =.21; females:
[R.sup.2] = .15), was less than has been reported in studies with
smaller sample sizes that documented training for several months prior
to performance (Hagan et al., 1987; Slovic, 1977). Thus, the actual
predictiveness of marathon finishing times for training and
competitiveness is probably greater than is indicated in the present
study. The more general point, however, is that future studies
addressing the relationships among training, motivation, and performance
should focus on recent or current running performance, rather than
lifetime best.
Revisiting Deaner's Hypothesis
This study supports Deaner's (2006a, 2006b) claim that the
gender difference in the occurrence of relatively fast runners is at
least partly due to a gender difference in competitiveness and training
commitment. Nevertheless, alternative hypotheses for the gender
difference in relative performance require exploration.
One possibility is that there is indeed a gender difference in
training volume, but this reflects a gender difference in the
opportunity to train, not in the motivation to do so. One version of
this hypothesis is that females may be more susceptible to running
injuries and so enjoy fewer opportunities to train consistently and
reach high training volumes; however, this idea is not currently
supported (Deaner, 2006a; van Gent, Siem, van Middelkoop, van Os,
Bierma-Zeinstra, Koes, & Taunton, 2007). Another version of this
hypothesis is that females cannot train consistently because they are
constrained by pregnancy, child care, or similar constraints. Although
this hypothesis must be true in some cases, it does not seem able to
provide a general account for the gender difference in the occurrence of
relatively fast runners. The reason is that the gender difference is at
least as strong in high school runners as it is in road race populations
(Deaner, 2006a, 2006b), and pregnancy rates for U.S. high school females
are low, especially among athletes (Sabo, Miller, Farrell, Melnick,
& Barnes, 1999).
Perhaps the best way to further evaluate Deaner's (2006a)
hypothesis is to test its chief prediction, that more males do in fact
maintain large training volumes. Although several studies have reported
this pattern (Callen, 1983; Clement et al., 1981; Ogles et al., 1995),
the gender differences are usually modest, and interpreting such
findings is difficult. One problem is that the gender difference in
relative performance typically is pronounced only among the fastest 2-5%
of runners (Deaner, 2006b). If, as expected, the gender difference in
training commitment also only occurs among a small fraction of the
population, then an overall gender difference in training volume might
not be detectable when assessing the entire population. For example, in
this study's data set, which is fairly large, proportionally more
men (6.7%) than women (3.2%) reported running at least 140km/wk, yet
this difference did not reach significance (p = 0.12).
A second problem is that the studies indicating gender differences
in training volume in distance runners are generally based on surveys
from the 1980s and early 1990s. During this time, marathons and other
road races in the U.S. were comprised of roughly 75% males (Running USA,
2010). Since the so-called second running boom, however, beginning
roughly in the mid-1990s, females have begun participating in far
greater equal numbers; in 2007, females comprised 49% of participants in
U.S. road running events (Running USA, 2010). Moreover, median finishing
times of marathons and other road races have increased substantially
since the 1980s, indicating a general shift among runners to a more
participatory rather than competitive orientation (USA Track &
Field, 2004). In fact, despite a nearly threefold increase in overall
road running participation since 1987 (Running USA, 2010), the absolute
number of fast male and female marathoners in the U.S. has declined
slightly over this time period (Deaner, 2006a).
These points suggest that the gender difference in the percentage
of relatively fast runners is probably far greater now than it was in
the mid-1980s, although the absolute number of fast male and female
runners may not have changed substantially. For instance, in the present
study's sample, where there were more than four times as many males
as females, we found that the overall population distributions of male
and female training volumes and fast running performances were highly
similar. Most strikingly, 27% of male runners and 25% of female runners
reported best times within 150% of the all-time fast standard. Such a
pattern would seem to be at odds with the recent demonstration (Deaner,
2006b) that in twenty of the largest U.S. marathons and 5Ks in 2003, the
percentage of such relatively fast males was two to four times as great
as the percentage of relatively fast females. However, if we imagine
doubling or tripling the number of less competitive, low volume female
runners in the present data set, then the results would concur. The
bottom line is that a strong test of Deaner's (2006a) hypothesis
requires obtaining information on training and performance from large
numbers of distance runners at numerous events: if this hypothesis is
correct, then the gender difference in the proportion of high volume,
competitive runners should closely correspond with the gender difference
in the proportion of relatively fast runners.
Practical Applications and Conclusions
If the gender difference in relative performance provides a
reasonable estimate of the gender difference in competitiveness and
training commitment, then this should facilitate new lines of inquiry
into the factors that produce the gender difference in competitiveness.
For example, Deaner (2006a) showed that the absolute number of fast
female runners at elite and sub-elite levels in the U.S. has remained
remarkably stable since the mid-1980s, despite substantial increases in
participation and incentives for female runners. Deaner (2006a) thus
argued that the gender difference in distance running competitiveness
cannot be completely ascribed to sociocultural conditions favoring males
(Eagly & Wood, 1999) but instead partly reflects an evolved male
predisposition for competition (Campbell, 2004). Recent work has
suggested a more complicated picture, however. It turns out that the
gender difference in relative performance in U.S. swimmers has declined
and that there is no longer a gender difference in relative swimming
performance (Deaner, 2007). This finding shows that, although there
could be an intrinsic gender difference in sports competitiveness, its
expression depends crucially on sociocultural factors. Therefore, future
studies documenting how gender differences in relative performance vary
across sports, cultures, and time periods should provide further
insights into the expression of training commitment and athletic
competitiveness.
Author Note
Robert O. Deaner, Department of Psychology, Grand Valley State
University; Kevin S. Masters, Department of Psychology, Syracuse
University and Department of Psychology, University of Colorado Denver;
Benjamin M. Ogles, Department of Psychology, Ohio University; Rick A.
LaCaille, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota Duluth.
References
American Psychological Association. (2010).Publication Manual of
the American Psychological Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., O'Connor
E., & Breiter H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward
value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron, 8, 537-551.
Bjorkqvist, K. (1994). Sex differences in physical, verbal, and
indirect aggression: a review of recent research. Sex Roles, 30,
177-188.
Buss, D. M. (2004). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the
mind (3rd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Callen, K. E. (1983). Mental and emotional aspects of long-distance
running. Psychosomatics, 24, 133-151.
Campbell. A. (2004). Female competition: Causes, constraints,
content, and contexts. Journal of Sex Research, 41, 16-26.
Cashdan, E. (1998). Are men more competitive than women? British
Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 213-229.
Clement, D. B., Taunton, J. E., Smart, G. W., & McNicol, K. L.
(1981). A survey of overuse running injuries. The Physician and
Sportsmedicine, 9, 47-58.
Deaner, R. O. (2006a). More males run fast: A stable sex difference
in competitiveness in U.S. distance runners. Evolution and Human
Behavior, 27, 63-84.
Deaner, R. O. (2006b). More males run relatively fast in U.S. road
races: further evidence of a sex difference in competitiveness.
Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 303-314.
Deaner, R. O. (2007, June). Different strokes." sex
differences in competitiveness have disappeared in swimming but not in
running. Poster presentation at the 19th Annual Meeting of the Human
Behavior and Evolution Society, Williamsburg, VA.
Dolgener, F.A., Kolkhorst, F. W., & Whitsett, D. A. (1994).
Long slow distance training in novice marathoners. Research Quarterly
for Exercise and Sport, 65, 339-346.
Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences
in human behavior: evolved dispositions versus social roles. American
Psychologist, 54, 408-423.
Gill, D. L. (1986). Competitiveness among females and males in
physical activity classes. Sex Roles, 15, 233-247.
Hagan, R. D., Upton, S. J., Duncan, J. J., & Gettman LR (1987).
Marathon performance in relation to maximal aerobic power and training
indices in female distance runners. British Journal of Sports Medicine,
21, 3-7.
Hawley, P. H., Little, T. D., & Card, N. A. (2008). The myth of
the alpha male: A new look at dominance-related beliefs and behaviors
among adolescent males and females. International Journal of Behavioral
Development, 32, 76-88.
Johnsgard, K. (1985). The motivation of the long distance runner:
I. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 25, 135-139.
Longhurst, K, & Spink, K. S. (1987). Participation motivation
of Australian children involved in organized sport. Canadian Journal of
Applied Sports Sciences, 12, 24-30.
MarathonGuide (2010). Marathon Records. Retrieved from
http://www.marathonguide.com/history/
Masters, K. S, & Ogles, B. M. (1995). An investigation of
different motivations of marathon runners with varying degrees of
experience. Journal of Sport Behavior, 18, 69-79.
Masters, K. S., & Ogles, B. M. (1998). Cognitive strategies
relate to injury, motivation, and performance among marathon runners:
Results from two studies. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 10,
281-296.
Masters, K. S., Ogles, B. M., & Jolton, J. A. (1993). The
development of an instrument to measure motivation for marathon running:
The motivation of marathoners scales (MOMS). Research Quarterly for
Exercise and Sport, 64, 134-143.
Miller, G. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the
evolution of human nature. New York, NY: Random House.
Noakes T. (2001). Lore of Running. 4th ed. Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Ogles, B. M., & Masters, K. S. (2000). Older vs. younger adult
male marathon runners: participative motives and training habits.
Journal of Sport Behavior, 23, 130-142.
Ogles, B. M., & Masters, K. S. (2003). A typology of marathon
runners based on cluster analysis of motivations. Journal of Sport
Behavior, 26, 69-85.
Ogles, B. M., Masters, K. S., & Richardson, S. A. (1995).
Obligatory running and gender: an analysis of participative motives and
training habits. International Journal of Sports Psychology, 26,
233-248. Running USA (2010). Statistics. Retrieved from
http://www.runningusa.org/statistics
Sabo, D. F., Miller, K. E., Farrell, M. P., Melnick, M. J., &
Barnes, G. M. (1999). High school athletic participation, sexual
behavior and adolescent pregnancy: a regional study. Journal of
Adolescent Health, 25, 207-216.
Samuelson, P. (1938). A note on the pure theory of consumers'
behaviour. Economica, 5, 61-71.
Seiler, S., & Sailer, S. (1997). The gender gap: elite women
are running further behind. Sportscience News, May-June. Retrieved from:
http://www.sportsci.org/news/news9705/gengap.html.
Shephard, R. J. (2000). Exercise and training in women, part I:
influence of gender on exercise and training responses. Canadian Journal
of Applied Physiology, 25, 19-34.
Skinner, J. S., Jaskolski, A, Jaskolska, A., Krasnoff, J., Gagnon,
J., Leon, A. S., Rao, D. C., Wilmore, J. H., & Bouchard, C. (2001).
Age, sex, race, initial fitness, and response to training: the HERITAGE
Family Study. Journal of Applied Physiology, 90, 1770-1776.
Slovic, P. (1977). Empirical study of training and performance in
the marathon. Research Quarterly, 48, 769-777.
Sparling, P. B., O'Donnell, E. M., & Snow, T. K. (1998).
The gender difference in distance running has plateaued: an analysis of
world rankings from 1980 to 1996. Medicine & Science in Sports &
Exercise, 30, 1725-1729.
USA Track & Field. (2004). 2004 Marathon Report. Retrieved from
http://www.usatf.org/news/specialReports/2004MarathonReport.asp
van Gent, R. N., Siem, D., van Middelkoop, M., van Os, A. G.,
Bierma-Zeinstra, S. M. A., Koes, B. W., & Taunton, J. E. (2007).
Incidence and determinants of lower extremity running injuries in long
distance runners: a systematic review. British Journal of Sports
Medicine, 41, 469-480.
Weinberg, R., Tenenbaum, G., McKenzie, A., Jackson, S., Anshel M.,
Grove, R., & Fogarty, J. (2003). Motivation for youth participation
in sport and physical activity: relationships to culture, self-reported
activity levels, and gender. International Journal of Sports Psychology,
31, 321-346.
Williams, K. M., Park, J. H., & Wieling, M. B. (2010). The face
reveals athletic flair: Better National Football League quarterbacks are
better looking Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 112-116
Wilmore, J. H., & Costill, D. L. (2004). Physiology of Sport
and Exercise (3rd ed). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Footnotes
(1) We chose 12 years as cut-off after considering a "years
running experience" histogram categorized by sex. The histogram
showed that for both men and women, the distributions were approximately
normal, with a peak of about 9.5 years of experience. On the one hand,
if we had chosen six (or eight) years as a cut-off we would have reduced
our sample size by more than half, so this was not a viable option. On
the other hand, if we had chosen 18 or 20 years as a cut-off, our sample
would have been roughly 10-15% larger, but this would have introduced a
potential bias because the vast proportion of such highly-experienced
runners were men. Thus 12 years seemed like the optimal choice. We did
explore whether our results differed substantially if we used slightly
different cut-offs, e.g. 10 or 15 years; they did not.
(2) We used data from 2004 and 2005 because we initially conducted
these analyses in 2005. However, it can be argued that the more relevant
time period for our participants was when they completed the
questionnaires, i.e., the late 1980s and early 1990s. We therefore
computed "all-time" 10-Fastest standards based only on
performances from 1990 and earlier. These standards were 2:23:11 and
2:07:31, a percentage difference of 12.2%. This falls between the
percentage difference of the 2005 "all-time" 10-Fastest
standard (11.1%) and the best 2004 10-fastest standard (12.8%). Thus, if
our analyses were repeated using world-class times that would have been
familiar to the participants (i.e., 1990 and earlier), our conclusions
would not change.
Address Correspondence to: Robert O. Deaner, Department of
Psychology, Allendale, M149401. E-mail: deanerr@gvsu.edu,'
Robert O. Deaner
Grand Valley State University
Kevin S. Masters
Syracuse University and University of Colorado Denver
Benjamin M. Ogles
Ohio University
Rick A. LaCaille
University of Minnesota Duluth
Table 1. Relative performance as a predictor of training volume
and competitiveness
Dependent Gender n [beta] [R.sup.2] F p
Variable
Training volume M 697 -0.46 0.21 184.2 <.0001
Training volume F 150 -0.39 0.15 26.5 <.0001
Competitiveness M 518 -0.27 0.07 39.6 <.0001
Competitiveness F 103 -0.29 0.08 8.9 0.003
Table 2. Age and experience as predictors of training volume and
competitiveness
Independent Dependent Gender n [Beta]
Variable Variable
Age Training volume M 694 -0.17
Age Training volume F 150 -0.12
Age Competitiveness M 517 -0.06
Age Competitiveness F 103 -0.22
Previous Marathons Training volume M 682 0.26
Previous Marathons Training volume F 149 0.06
Previous Marathons Competitiveness M 510 0.14
Previous Marathons Competitiveness F 102 0.15
Years running Training volume M 697 0.01
Years running Training volume F 150 0.04
Years running Competitiveness M 518 0.001
Years running Competitiveness F 103 0.06
Independent [R.sup.2] F p
Variable
Age 0.03 19.6 <.0001
Age 0.02 2.2 0.14
Age 0.0 2.1 0.15
Age 0.05 5.3 0.02
Previous Marathons 0.07 49.8 <.0001
Previous Marathons 0.0 0.58 0.45
Previous Marathons 0.02 10.7 0.001
Previous Marathons 0.02 2.4 0.22
Years running 0.0 0.3 0.86
Years running 0.0 0.2 0.66
Years running 0.0 0.0 0.92
Years running 0.01 0.4 0.52