Pre-putt routines and putt outcomes of collegiate golfers.
Bell, Robert J. ; Cox, Kyle E. ; Finch, W. Holmes 等
The sport of golf presents researchers with various opportunities
to examine athletes' behaviors in specific situations. Golf also
provides an arena to test assumptions concerning motor control and
performance, due to the psychological and physiological demands of the
sport. The skill of putting is especially important within golf and
accounts for nearly 40% of golfers' total strokes (Pelz, 2000). The
acquisition of appropriate putting skills is easy for all ability
levels, yet the mastery of putting is difficult (Pelz, 2000). The
mechanics, motions, and mental approach differ from other aspects of
golf. Mechanically, the putting stroke is the shortest swing and results
in rolling the golf ball. Yet, the amount of control of a successful
putt appears unpredictable, especially since the PGA tour average of
making a 6-foot putt is around 55% (Diaz, 1989). The unpredictability of
successful putting may be due to several reasons. The target of the
putting stroke (i.e., the hole) requires greater precision than other
golfer's targets and if the direction of the putting stroke at
impact is off by just a fraction, the imperfection is magnified more
than any other swing in the game of golf(Pelz, 2000). Across courses and
tournaments, the speeds of the greens and types of grasses on the
putting surfaces vary, making it difficult for many golfers to adapt
(Pelz, 2000). Successfully navigating the contours of the green, while
properly judging the speed of the putt, requires a great deal of mental
skill (Pelz, 1991).
These aforementioned aspects of putting suggest that the skill of
putting is difficult to master (Pelz, 2000). Efforts of simplifying the
process include the development of numerous different types of putters,
and ways to actually hold the putter (Rotella, 2001). One facet of
putting similar to other sports is the preparation time prior to the
execution of a shot which is classified as a pre-performance routine
(Cohn, 1990). However, research specifically addressing the
effectiveness of pre-putt routines has yet to be explored to its limits.
Singer (2002) suggested that one important function of
pre-performance routines is to aid athletes in the self-regulation of
"arousal level, thoughts, performance expectancy, and attentional
focus" (p.359). Singer also suggested that self-regulation is
thought to be particularly important in self-paced events in which the
performer has the opportunity to prepare for the movement. Routines can
help an athlete mentally prepare for upcoming movements by enhancing
concentration. For instance, it has been argued that pre-performance
routines help athletes transfer their attention from task irrelevant
thoughts to task relevant thoughts (Weinberg & Gould, 2007).
Pre-performance routines increase the likelihood that individuals will
not be internally or externally distracted before and/or during
performance. They also noted that pre-performance routines help
structure the athlete's thought processes and emotional states,
keeping the focus of attention in the present and on task-related cues
(Weinberg & Gould, 2007).
Research has shown that individuals possess a limited attentional
capacity (Lewis & Linder, 1997). However, motor behavior research
also suggests that once a skill has been learned, the attentional
demands towards performance are decreased (Schmidt & Wrisberg,
2000). For instance, Backman and Molander (1991) found that expert
putters who attended to the technical aspects of the putting stroke
negatively affected their performance. Specifically, it is possible that
attention to skill execution may be counterproductive to performance and
may manifest with undue pre-performance routines (Beilock, Afremow,
Rabe, & Carr, 2001; Lewis & Linder, 1997). Due to the limited
attentional capacities, there may be a tendency of counterproductive
over-thinking, an increased susceptibility to distractions, and/or
heightened emotions (Krane & Williams, 1987). Golfers who focus on
the possible repercussions of poor performance are likely to experience
unsuitable levels of emotional anxiety (Beilock et al., 2001). Thus, the
implementation of suitable pre-performance routines should help lessen
the effects of negative stimuli and enhance the prospects of successful
performance (Cohn, Rotella & Lloyd, 1990).
Specific research has revealed results that suggest pre-performance
routines increase the likelihood of successful outcomes in basketball
free throw shooting (Czech, Ploszay, & Burke, 2004; Gayton,
Cielinski, Francis-Keniston, & Hearns, 1989; Lobmeyer &
Wasserman, 1986). For instance, Lobmeyer and Wasserman found that
free-throw pre-shot routines in basketball significantly contributed to
the accuracy of the shot. An experimental study by Gayton et al. (1989)
examined shooters' outcomes with alternating between implementing
pre-shot routines. Results revealed that the percentage of made free
throws was significantly higher during the pre-shot routine condition
than the control condition. However, the researchers manipulated
pre-shot routines by adding or removing dribbles, thus, introducing the
extraneous variable of practice which may have been a limitation to the
study. More recent research by Czech and colleagues (2004) also examined
the importance of pre-shot routines in basketball free-throw shooting.
They observed the maintenance of routines that were classified as
behaviors repeated 90% of the time or more in between shots. Results
revealed that the group maintaining the same pre-shot routine for the
duration of the study shot a higher percentage than those not
maintaining the same pre-shot routine. Results of this study greatly
added to the external validity in that it is thought to be the first
study of pre-performance routines that observed athletes during actual
competition.
Research in the area of golf has also shown routines to be an
effective tool for minimizing distracting thoughts. Boutcher and Crews
(1987) studied the effects of attentional pre-shot routines on a golf
putting task. Twelve collegiate golfers were randomly assigned to four
groups, two control groups and two experimental pre-shot routine groups.
The purpose of the attentional training in the experimental group was an
attempt to prevent golfers from placing any unnecessary focus on a
specific aspect of the skill. The two control groups (male and female)
practiced the putting skill without the use of a pre-shot routine. It
was found that the routine group actually increased the time period
(i.e., took longer) between the addressing of the golf ball and the
impact of striking the ball. It was also found that the variability of
the putting task decreased, although only the female routine group
showed improved putting performance. The authors concluded that the
routines seemed to provide a means of effectively controlling the mental
and physiological states before the performance of closed-skills.
Although the golfers were instructed to go through a routine and only
one of the groups showed improvement, the introduction of a routine has
the potential to help golfers improve their putting accuracy (Cohn et
al., 1990).
McCann, Lavallee, and Lavallee (2001) also examined the efficacy of
implementation of pre-shot routines. The authors examined the effect of
pre-shot routines on wedge shots and observed the effects of novice
golfers' pre-shot routines on the accuracy of wedge shots from
distances of 40 meters, 50 meters, and 60 meters. Participants were
randomly assigned into two main groups" novice golfer and
non-golfer. They were then separated into three subgroups based on skill
level and intervention type (routine/no routine or
practice/non-practice). Those golfers in the cognitive-behavior
performance routine groups were issued a handout of a scripted
performance routine and shown two demonstrations. Results revealed the
non-golfer routine group significantly improved their shot dispersion following a 3-week long period of learning and implementing a
pre-performance routine, suggesting pre-performance routines have a
positive impact on golfers wedge shot accuracy to a given target.
The effects of consistency of routines have also been examined in
past literature. Boutcher and Zinsser (1990) conducted an observational
study of pre-putt routines of beginning and elite level golfers
performing six 4-feet and six 12-feet putts. Cardiac, respiratory, and
behavioral patterns of putting were measured. Researchers found that
elite golfers had longer, more meticulous pre-putt behaviors than those
of beginning golfers. The elite-level golfers demonstrated more
consistent behavior during their pre-putt routines by exhibiting their
dominant routine (i.e., one exhibited most often) on 62% of their putt
attempts. Beginning golfers did not show noticeably consistent behaviors
when performing their pre-putt routines as they exhibited their dominant
routine on only 35% of their putt attempts. Results indicate that expert
or near expert golfers who maintain consistency amongst their putting
routines will most likely have improved putting success.
Pre-shot routines and the effect of duration on outcome were
examined by Crews and Boutcher (1987) as they observed 12 players on the
Ladies Professional Golf Association (LPGA) tour. The authors analyzed
the pre-shot routines of both full-swing and putting strokes for 12
holes during tournament play. Observational results indicated that all
players were extremely consistent with regards to time and behaviors. It
was observed that the more successful golfers used longer time periods
for the full shot and putting routines, suggesting that the chances of
holing a putt are improved by taking longer during the pre-shot routine.
Yet, the authors did not account for difficulty of shots and it is not
possible to determine if this affected pre-performance routines.
Kingston and Hardy (2000) utilized a case-study design aimed at
developing more systematic and consistent pre-shot routines. The 10-week
intervention phase consisted of developing holistic goals and cue words
for an expert golfer. Results were significant regarding lessening the
variability of the timing of putting routines. However, results did not
correlate with improved putting performance. The authors utilized
self-report measures on the participant's perception of performance
and point out that self-reporting was problematic due to the inability
of controlling for extraneous factors such as difficulty of putts. As a
result, the collection of actual performance data and duration of
putting routines is still warranted.
Past research has accentuated the value and importance of
pre-performance routines. A common theme has been that routines are most
effective when maintained across a series of given tasks (Boutcher &
Zinsser, 1990; Crews & Boutcher, 1987; Czech et al., 2004; Lidor
& Singer, 2000). Routines have been reported to organize thoughts
and focus attention before the execution of a sport-specific movement
(Boutcher & Crews, 1987). Nonetheless, the lack of applied research
within the area of putting and pre-performance routines still needs to
be addressed (Mack, 2001).
Pre-putt routines are individualized and vary across participants.
Putting has been reported by players as very "feel" oriented
and much of golfers' preparation for a putt attempt is based on
this notion (Penick & Shrake, 1992). Despite this, researchers have
yet to examine the effects of pre-performance routine duration on the
outcome of putting attempt in an applied setting. The aim of the current
study was to investigate the duration of pre-putt routines of highly
skilled golfers and the accuracy of putts.
Methodology
Participants
Direct observation occurred during two NCAA Division I golf
tournaments. One-hundred and sixty-seven male collegiate golfers from 30
Division I Universities across various conferences participated in the
tournaments combined and due to the team system within college
tournaments, were presumed to be experienced (< 5 handicap). Both
collegiate golf tournaments consisted of 54-holes of play with the
format consisting of 36-holes during day one and 18-holes of golf for
day two.
Setting
Data collection took place during the fall 2007 at two separate
collegiate golf tournaments in the Midwestern United States. The first
tournament for direct observation was coded as tournament A. The second
was coded as tournament B.
Design and Procedures
Prior to data collection, the researchers established a
hierarchical selection criteria based on the following: (1) A par-three
hole was observed to ensure similar approach shots from the same teeing
ground; (2) the length of the hole was as close to 175 yards as
possible, since the first hole used for observation determined the
length of subsequent distances; (3) the green of the hole was relatively
fiat, thus eliminating the variation of the difficulty of putts. Both
golf holes used for observation were par-three's measuring between
158 and 183 yards.
During tournament A, the ideal green for data collection that
satisfied the aforementioned selection criteria was a par-three
measuring 173 yards. The distance from tee to green was measured to the
center of the green and each day, the hole location was changed to a
different portion of the green. Thus, there were two distances for each
hole. The first day's length was 168 yards and the second
day's length measured 180 yards.
The green at the tournament B course used for data collection that
satisfied the selection criteria was a par-three hole similar in yardage measuring 175 yards. The first day's length measured 158 yards. The
second day's length measured 183 yards. Both holes featured a water
hazard bordering the right side of the green.
Speed of Green
To help ensure the consistency of measurements, the speeds of the
greens were measured using a Stimpmeter (Pelz, 2000). To obtain this
measurement, a ball was placed onto the Stimpmeter while it is flat on
the ground and then raised until the ball rolled onto the green. The
distance (in feet) that the ball rolled (green speed) was taken in the
morning before play and after play was finished. The speeds of the
greens were similar in that the tournament A course measured 10 in the
morning and 10.5 in the afternoon on both days. The green speed at the
tournament B course measured nine feet in the morning and 9.5 feet in
the afternoon both days. Golf literature states that a deviation of
speed in greens less than .5 feet is considered similar green speed
(Brede, 1990).
Weather
The weather for the tournament A was sunny with a temperature in
middle 80 degrees for both days. The wind was out of the North at
approximately 10 miles per hour both days. The weather for the
tournament B was overcast for much of the first day with temperatures in
the lower 50 degrees. The wind was out of the north/northwest at 10
miles per hour. The second day at tournament B consisted of sunny
conditions with temperatures in the lower 70 degrees. The wind speed and
direction was minimal for the second day of the tournament. Weather
conditions for each tournament day were retrieved from The Weather
Channel website (www.weather.com).
Data Collection
One-hundred and forty total putts from both collegiate golf
tournaments were directly observed and recorded. The primary variable of
duration of putting routines initiated when the player removed the
marker from behind the ball and timing finished at the beginning of the
execution of the putting stroke. These two criteria were selected due to
their consistency amongst the routines of each player. Data were not
recorded if a player did not complete either of these tasks. Observers
recorded data using a standardized stopwatch to ensure the consistency
of the measurement.
Putts between .91 meters (3 feet) and 3.04 meters (10 feet) in
length were recorded because these distances normally yield the highest
percentage of makeable putts (Pelz, 2000). The distances were then
separated into two categories for data analysis purposes: .91 meters (3
feet) to 1.82 meters (6 feet) in length and 1.82 meters (6 feet) to 3.04
meters (10 feet) in length. Distances were separated for the potential
effect of length on the outcome of the putt. Putts that could be
considered "tap-ins" (i.e. less than three feet from the hole)
were not included in the observation and data collection process. This
was due to the limited skill demands required for making shorter
distance putts, as well as the absence of a pre-putt routine (Cohn et
al., 1990). Lastly, putts outside of 3.04 meters (10 feet) were not
recorded due to the lower percentage of made putts (Cassidy, Morgan,
& Cherry, 2006; Pelz, 2000;).
Additional putting data were recorded for each participant's
analysis; grouping order, score, and putting order. Participants were
pre-organized into groups of three by the tournament committee and name
and school were recorded for identification purposes. Group number was
also recorded to examine if the time of day affected the duration of
putting routines or outcomes of putts. Putts for scores of birdie, par,
or bogey were also recorded. Putts for a score of double bogey (5) or
higher were recorded, but not used for analysis. Putting order was
recorded to determine if the order of putting affected the outcome of
the routine. Thus, each player who had a putt from a distance inside the
circle of measurement was assigned a number based on the proximity from
the hole. For instance, if all three players were eligible for
observation, the player furthest from the hole was number 1, the next
closest was number 2, and the closest was number 3. If a player was
attempting his first putt then he was labeled as X. 1 (X = number given
based on proximity). For example, the furthest player from the hole
attempting his first putt would be labeled 1.1.
Interscorer Reliability
The following steps were taken to ensure internal validity regarding recorded distances and minimize potential observation error
(Kazdin, 1998). First, maintaining rigid measurements was essential.
Prior to the beginning of each day's play, markers were placed at
.91 meters, 1.82 meters, and 3.04 meters at five different points around
the hole to establish the distance category circles. Second, three
separate observers recorded data during the study. At least two
observers' recorded separate measurements at all times and were
approximately 20 feet from the green. Data collection procedures were
unobtrusive, and participants were unaware of any data collection
methods performed by the researchers. Third, observers utilized standard
stopwatches to record time and to help ensure a standard use of
measurement, the fastest time recorded was utilized.
Lastly, observers conferred with each other after each group left
the putting green and determined the distance category for every putt
that may have been of a questionable length. During the tournament, any
distance not unanimously agreed upon was not recorded. During all
recorded observations, researchers were in agreement 95% of the time.
There was one incidence of disagreement and the data were not used.
These measures helped ensure inter-scorer reliability (Kazdin, 1998).
Data Analysis
To test the relationship between putting routine duration and putt
outcome, four separate logistic regression analysis using SPSS were
performed on the data from both tournaments (Tabachnik & Fidell,
2001). The outcome variable of interest was the outcome of the putt
(make/miss). Model building was conducted, in which an initial set of
independent variables (time) was included in the analysis, after which
those with large p-values were removed and the logistic regression
analysis was conducted again. Because this study was exploratory in
nature, it was felt that such model building was appropriate so as to
remove potential predictors that were clearly unrelated to the outcome
variable of making the putt. Three logistic models were developed, one
for the combined tournament data and one for each separate tournament.
The fourth logistic regression included participants in both tournaments
who had at least three recordable putts (a subset of the total sample),
thus a within-subjects logistic regression model was used. In order to
account for the repeated measurements on the same participants, general
estimating equations (GEE) were used for parameter estimation with this
latter model (Tabachnik & Fidell, 2001). By using GEE, the standard
error estimates for the parameters of interest were calculated properly,
accounting for correlations due to multiple occurrences in the data set
by the same golfers.
Results
Tournament A
Data were analyzed linking the duration of participants putting
routine to the outcome variable (making or missing the putt). Within the
recorded distances of .91 meters (3 feet) to 3.04 meters (10 feet) there
were a total of 46 made putts and 17 missed putts from tournament A (see
Table 1).
Descriptive data of duration of putting routines revealed the mean
for the made putts was 21.90 seconds SD = [+ or -] 6.69, while means for
the missed putts was 27.33 seconds SD = [+ or -] 8.76. For the entirety
of tournament A, the observed hole was .39 strokes over par. There were
20 birdies, 150 pars, 56 bogeys and 23 double bogeys.
The logistic regression revealed significant relationships with the
likelihood of making the putt were obtained for routine duration, [chi
square] (3, N= 63) [beta] =-0.098, p<.05. The negative slope value
([beta] = -0.098) for time reveals that players with longer duration
routines had lower probability of making the putt (see Table 2). A
significant relationship also existed regarding distance and outcome of
the putt during tournament A, [chi square] (3, N=63) [beta] =-1.768,
p<.05. The negative slope value ([beta] = - 1.768) for distance
reveals that players attempting putts from longer distances from the
hole (i.e., 3-6 feet, 6-10 feet) had a lower probability of making the
putt.
Tournament B
Within the recorded distances, there were 41 made putts and 36
missed putts. Descriptive data illustrates that the average time for the
made putts was 25.69 seconds SD = [+ or -] 8.55, while the average time
for the missed putts was 24.35 seconds SD = [+ or -] 7.97. During
tournament B, the observed hole played .23 strokes over par. There were
30 birdies, 183 pars, 80 bogeys and 18 double bogeys (see Table 3).
A logistical regression revealed no statistically significant
relationship between routine duration and the likelihood of making the
putt from tournament B, [chi square] (3, N=77) [beta] =.014, p<.05.
The only significant predictor in the logistic regression analysis was
distance, [chi square] (3, N=77) [beta] = - 1.016, p<.05. Putts
further away from the hole were less likely to be made (see Table 4).
Combined Tournaments
Combining both tournament data, the logistic regression revealed a
non-significant relationship between routine duration and outcome [chi
square] (3, N =1 40) [beta] =-0.037, p<.05 (see table 5). Similar to
both previous models, the predictor of distance was a significant factor
for this model [chi square] (3, N=140) [beta] =-1.286, p<.05
indicating that the further from the hole, the less likely the player is
to make the putt.
Within-Subjects Design
A total of 15 players participated in both tournaments and had
three or more recordable putts, and thus were included in the
within-subjects model. Within the recordable distances, there were 30
made putts and 13 missed putts. The logistic regression model using
General Estimating Equations (GEE) revealed that routine duration was a
significant factor [chi square] (3, N=43) [beta] =-0.009, p<.05
related to the probability of players making the putt (Table 6). Players
showing less deviation in their routine duration across the multiple
putt attempts were more likely to make the putt. Also, the further from
the hole, the less likely the player was to make the putt [chi square]
(3, N--43) [beta] =-0.516, p<.05.
Discussion
Pre-performance routines have been noted across a wide range of
sports (Cohn, 1990; Cornelius, 2002; Weinberg & Gould, 2007).
However gaps still exist in that pre-performance routines have not been
extensively analyzed in applied settings and golf in particular. To
date, research has yet to look at pre-performance routines and putting
in the specific manner of the present study.
In this study, an attempt was made to examine the duration of
pre-putt routines of elite level golfers and the accuracy of putt
attempts. There is a lack of research concerning the times of pre-putt
routines, but similar research (Boutcher & Zinsser, 1990) suggested
that longer duration of routines would yield more made putts. However,
results from this study are counter to previous studies (Boutcher &
Crews, 1987; Boutcher & Zinsser, 1990). Results from the analyses
lend support that the longer pre-performance routine duration, the lower
the probability of making the putt. Distance was also shown to be a
significant factor that the longer the putt, the less likely the player
was going to make the putt. Although, results from the current study are
not conclusive and whereas significance was shown for the
within-subjects design, the between subjects design revealed
significance for only tournament A.
Putting routines are individualized tasks and it raises the
question of whether the temporal consistency correlates with the
behavioral consistency of routines (Penick & Shrake, 1992). As a
result, the within-subjects design attempted to bridge the gap of
previous putting routine research (Wrisberg, Cassidy, Morgan, &
Cherry, 2009).
Results from the within-subjects design revealed that players who
maintained their preparation time (i.e., pre-putt routines) were more
likely to make the putt. It is thought that the consistency of pre-putt
routines would produce less conscious processing through the movement
resulting in more consistent behaviors and outcomes (Kingston &
Hardy, 2001). Juxtaposed is that deviation in temporal consistency
resulted in a decrease in putting performance. Although no specific
behavioral data were collected, it can be assumed that the near-expert
level of the participants (handicap <5) yielded established and near
"automatic" pre-shot routines (Boutcher & Crews, 1987). As
a result, changes in the duration of pre-putt routines and not
necessarily obvious behavioral changes caused the decrease in
performance. These results were similar to past research in basketball
free-throw shooting (Czech et al., 2004) suggesting that players who
repeated their dominant routine at least 90% of the time were more
likely to make the shot.
Due to the consistency of behavioral routines of elite level
golfers, perhaps the deviation in duration of putting routines was the
reflection of varied attentional processing. Past research has revealed
the importance of attention in self-paced skills and revealed that
conscious control over the movement results in decreasing performance
(Baumeister, 1984, Boutcher & Crews, 1987). Results of Beilock,
Bertenthal, McCoy, and Carr (2004) suggest that experienced golfers
(handicap < 8) performed better with less time compared to when they
were instructed to take as much time as they needed. Thus, it is
plausible in the within-subjects design that deviations in duration of
routines would lead to counterproductive over-thinking resulting in a
disruption of automaticity.
Due to the "real" world setting of the data collection,
statistical analyses helped control for threats to internal validity
(Kazdin, 1998). Putting order, distance, group number, and score were
not shown to be significant among any of the models thus ruling out
alternative hypotheses. First, no differences in duration of routines
were reported regarding putting order. Participants did not appear to
alter their routine with respect to whether they putted first, second,
or third. This aspect also reportedly filled in gaps presented by
similar research (Wrisberg et al., 2009).
Interestingly, although distance did reveal significance regarding
made/missed putts, it did not play a significant role in the duration of
the routines. Routine duration from the shorter distances of .91-1.82
meters did not significantly differ from observed routines in the longer
1.82-3.04 meter range. In addition, putting for a specific score
(birdie, par, or bogey) nor the time of day (group number),
significantly impacted the duration of the pre-putt routines. Although
past studies (Jackson & Backer, 2001) lend support that task
difficulty results in longer preparation times, results from the current
study did not appear to be the case.
In light of the current analyses and the idiosyncratic nature of
putting routines, assessing causal relationships between duration and
success, may warrant mixed methodologies. In the current study, the
variance of results from the between-subject design does not control for
intersubject error. Research has advocated intraindividual approaches of
data collection that that are sensitive to within-subject fluctuations
(Robazza, Bortoli, & Hanin, 2004). For instance, past state anxiety
research by Sonstroem and Bernardo (1982) and Burton (1988) both
employed repeated measures methodology to help control for interpersonal
differences. Thus, utilizing within-subject design methodologies for
naturalistic observation appears to offer greater advantages. Examining
specific participants across several tournaments consistent with the
current within-subject design may yield more meaningful results than
between-subject designs. Specifically, Gentner, McGraw, Gonzalez, and
Czech (2009) as well as Wrisberg and colleagues (2009) suggest that
prolonged observations across several tournaments that examine both
temporal patterns and behavioral tendencies may reveal more concrete
findings. Whereas, the present study looked at elite level golfers (<
5 handicap), within-subject observation could examine professional
golfers in natural settings across 72-hole tournaments as opposed to
54-holes. Combining the aforementioned methodologies along with
qualitative interviews may reveal further insight into the underlying
processes of pre-putt routines (see Wrisberg et al., 2009 for a detailed
discussion of naturalistic observation in sport settings).
Results of the between-subjects design are inconclusive, thus
suggesting the need for further applied research. Within the natural
settings of the current study, there may have been some extraneous
variables present with the data collection. First, whereas the
researchers attempted to satisfy all of the selection criteria for
greens observed, the continuity of the putting surfaces may have been a
limitation to this study. The first putting green at tournament A for
observation was extremely flat on all sides of the hole for both days
compared to the hole location at tournament B. During tournament B, the
back side of the second day hole location provided more slope and it is
possible routines differed according to the difficulty of this putt
encountered. However, it should be noted that location of putts were
observed and no participant putted from this vantage point. Nonetheless,
the processing time for the putting routines may still have varied.
Second, another limitation may have resulted from the participants
observed. Tournament B had more overall players than tournament A, which
may have led to more total observed putts. There were 77 putts, 41 made
and 36 missed, within the recordable distances for tournament B compared
to 63 putts, 46 made and 17 missed, for tournament A. Similarly, the
quality of the tournament field could have played a role in the lack of
significance for tournament B. Tournament B received a weaker tournament
ranking of 142 compared to the stronger ranking of 177 for tournament A
(www.golfstat.com). This may explain why the observed hole at tournament
A played somewhat easier (.23 strokes over par) compared to the hole at
tournament B (.39 strokes over par).
Last, weather conditions may have influenced data collection during
the first day of tournament B. The temperature was between 50-55
degrees, which was approximately 20 degrees colder than the second day
of the tournament. This could have affected the data by influencing
players to display a routine that was longer or shorter than their
normal duration. Again, future within-subject methodologies that include
qualitative explorations could help discern participants'
interpretations of putting routines in adverse or changing weather
patterns.
Summary
Whereas results from the current applied study reveal a possible
correlation between routine duration and outcome, conclusions should be
tempered with ideations of future research. The within-subject design
revealed that temporal consistencies are of importance to the likelihood
of making the putt. However, the between-subject design from tournament
A and B yielded varying results emphasizing the best way to assess
naturalistic observation is through intrasubject analysis. Identifying a
duration range that correlate with more made putts can potentially be
applied to various coaches and performers. Thus, it is important that
sport psychology consultants and other professionals are aware of and
continue naturalistic oberservation studies of pre-putt routines.
References
Backman, L., & Molander, B. (1991). Cognitive processes among
skilled miniature golf players: Effects of instruction on motor
performance, concentration time, and perceived difficulty. Scandinavian
Journal of Psychology, 32, 344-351.
Baumeister, R. (1984). Choking under pressure: Self-consciousness
and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. Journal
of Personality & Social Psychology, 46, 610-620.
Beilock, S., Afremow, J., Rabe, A., & Carr, T. (2001).
"Don't Miss!" The debilitating effects of suppressive imagery on golf putting performance. Journal of Sport & Exercise
Psychology, 23, 200-221.
Beilock, S., Bertenthal, B., McCoy, A., & Carr, T. (2004).
Haste does not always make waste: Expertise, direction of attention, and
speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 11, 373-379.
Boutcher, S., & Crews, D. (1987). The effect of a pre-shot
attentional routine on a well-learned skill. International Journal of
Sport Psychology, 18, 30-39
Boutcher, S., & Zinnser, N. (1990). Cardiac deceleration of
elite and beginning golfers during putting. Journal of Sport &
Exercise Psychology, 12, 37-47.
Brede, D. (1990). USGA Green Section Record, 28, 10-12.
Burton, D. (1988). Do anxious swimmers swim slower? Reexamining the
elusive anxiety--performance relationship. Journal of Sport &
Exercise Psychology, 10, 45-61.
Cassidy, C., Morgan, T., & Cherry, L. (2006). The relationship
of pre-putt routine duration and putt difficulty to performance outcome.
Unpublished manuscript, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN.
Cohn, P. (1990). Pre-performance routines in sport: Theoretical and
practical applications. The Sport Psychologist, 4, 301-312.
Cohn, P., Rotella, R., & Lloyd, J. (1990). Effects of a
cognitive-behavioral intervention on the pre-shot routine and
performance in golf. The Sport Psychologist, 4, 33-47.
Cornelius, A. (2002). Psychological foundations of sport. Boston,
MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Crews, D., & Boutcher, S. (1987). An observational analysis of
professional female golfers during tournament play. Journal of Sport
Behavior, 9, 51-58.
Czech, D., Ploszay, A., & Burke, K. (2004). An examination of
the maintenance of pre-shot routines in basketball flee-throw shooting.
Journal of Sport Behavior, 27, 323-329.
Diaz, J. (1989) Perils of putting, Sports Illustrated, 70, 76-79.
Gayton, W., Cielinski, K., Francis-Keniston, W., & Hearns, J.
(1989). Effects of a pre-shot routine on free-throw shooting. Perceptual
& Motor Skills, 68, 317-318.
Gentner, N., McGraw, T., Gonzalez, S., & Czech, D. (2008) An
examination of preservice routines of elite tennis players, The Sport
Journal, 12, 1.
Jackson, R., & Baker, J. (2001). Routines, rituals, and rugby:
Case study of a world class goal kicker. The Sport Psychologist, 15,
48-65.
Kazdin, A. (1998). Research design in clinical psychology. (3rd
ed.) Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Kingston, K., & Hardy, L. (2001) Preperformance routine
training using holistic process goals. (Ed.). Patrick R. Thomas.
Optimising Performance in Golf. Australian Academic Press.
Krane, V., & Williams, J. (1987). Performance and somatic
anxiety, cognitive anxiety, and confidence changes prior to competition.
Journal of Sport Behavior, 10, 47-56.
Lewis, B., & Linder, D. (1997). Thinking about choking?
Attentional processes and paradoxical performance. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 937-944.
Lidor, R., & Singer, R. (2000). Teaching pre-performance
routines to beginners. Journal of Physical Education, Recreation &
Dance, 71, 34-36.
Lobmeyer, D., & Wasserman, E. (1986). Preliminaries to free
throw shooting: Superstitious behavior? Journal of Sports Behavior, 9,
70-78.
Mack, M. (2001). Effects of time and movements of the preshot
routine on free throw shooting. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 93,
567-573.
McCann, P., Lavallee, D., & Lavallee, R. (2001). The effect of
preshot routines on golf wedge shot performance. European Journal of
Sport Science, 5, 1-9.
Rotella, B. (2001). Putting out of your mind. New York, Simon &
Schuster.
Pelz, D. (1991). Putt like the pros. New York: Collins.
Pelz, D. (2000). Dave Pelz's putting bible. New York:
Doubleday.
Penick, H., & Shrake, B. (1992). Harvey Penick's little
red book. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Robazza, C., Bortoli, L., & Hanin, Y. (2004). Precompetition
emotions, bodily symptoms, and task-specific qualities as predictors of
performance in high-level karate athletes. Journal of Applied Sport
Psychology, 16, 151-165.
Schmidt, R., & Wrisberg, C. (2000). Motor learning and
performance (2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Singer, R. (2002). Pre-performance states, routines, and
automaticity: What does it take to realize expertise in self-paced
events? Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24, 359-375.
Sonstroem, R., & Bernardo, P. (1982). Intraindividual pregame
state anxiety and basketball performance: A re-examination of the
inverted-U curve. Journal of Sport Psychology, 4, 235-245.
Tabachnik, B., & Fidell, L. (2001). Using multivariate
statistics (4th ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Weinberg, R., Gould, D. (2007). Foundations of sport and exercise
psychology. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Wrisberg, C., Cassidy, C., Morgan, T., and Cherry, L. (2009).
Naturalistic observations in sport and physical activity settings.
Journal of Behavior Analysis in Health, Sports, Fitness, and Medicine,
1, 117-129.
Robert J. Bell, Kyle E. Cox, and W. Holmes Finch
Ball State University
Address Correspondence to: Robert J. Bell, Ph.D, HP 222-E, Ball
State University, Muncie, IN, 47306. Email: robbell@bsu.edu. Phone:
765-285-3286.
Table 1. Tournament A Putting Routine Duration
Made/Missed Mean N Std.Dev
Made 21.9 46 6.69
Miss 27.33 17 8.76
Table 2. Tournament A Results
Predictor B SE B [e.sup.B] Sig
Time -0.098 .048 .906 .038 *
Distance -1.768 .704 .171 .012 *
Putt # 1.146 .782 3.146 .143
Note: * P <.05
Table 3. Tournament B Putting Routine Duration
Made/Missed Mean N Std. Dev
Made 25.67 41 8.55
Miss 24.35 36 7.97
Table 4. Tournament B Results
Predictor [beta] SE [beta] [e.sup.B] Sig
Time .014 .032 1.014 .662
Distance -1.016 .504 .362 .044 *
Putt # .541 .364 1.717 .137
Note: * P<.05
Table 5. Combined Tournament Results
Predictor [beta] S E [beta] [e.sup.B] Sig
Time -.037 .027 .963 .162
Distance -1.286 .437 8.672 .003 *
Putt # 0.541 .341 1.718 .112
Note: * P <.05
Table 6. Within Subjects Design: Generalized Estimating Equation
[chi L 95% U 95%
Parameter [beta] S. E. square] C.I. C.I. Sig
(Intercept) .901 .178 25.480 .551 1.250 .000 *
Distance -.516 .132 15.170 -.775 -.256 .000 *
Time -.009 .005 4.019 -.018 .000 .045 *
Note: * P<.05