The development and validation of the golf self-efficacy scale.
Hayslip, Bert, Jr. ; Raab, Carlin M. ; Baczewski, Philip C. 等
**********
Self-efficacy reflects individuals' beliefs in their
capability to perform behaviors that will bring about specific outcomes
(Bandura, 1995, 1997). Several variables affect self-efficacy, including
one's social, environmental and temporal circumstances, perceived
task difficulty, and one's experiences with a specific task or set
of behaviors (Bandura, 1977). Moreover, an individual's
self-efficacy can influence many aspects of one's performance, such
as persistence, motivation, and effort, and can even predict future
performance (Bandura, 1997; Vealey, 1986). In fact, very few
psychological variables predict performance outcomes as well as
self-efficacy, particularly in the context of sport (Bandura, 1995,
1997; Barling & Abel, 1983 Christensen, 2000; Feltz, 1988; Gayton,
Matthews, & Burchstead, 1986; McAuley & Grill, 1983; Woolfolk,
Murphy, Gottesfeld, & Aitken, 1985; Ryckman, Robbins, & Thorton,
1982).
Golf is one sport in which self-efficacy has been studied
extensively, and has been found to be related to actual performance
outcomes, such as making an attempted putt (Pickens, Rotella, &
Gansneder, 1996), one's overall score (Christensen, 2000; Hassmen,
Raglin & Lundqvist, 2004), and overall level of skill (e.g.,
professional vs. amateur; Gagnon, 2002; Koczajowski, 1997).
Self-efficacy also relates to different psychological states, such as
flow (Catley & Duda, 1997; Stein et al., 1995), competitive anxiety
(Hassmen, Raglin & Lundqvist, 2004; Krane & Williams, 1992), and
self-handicapping (Kuczka & Treasure, 2005). In addition, golf
self-efficacy can be increased through instruction (Compton, 2003;
Edward, 1983; Ferguson, Lirgg, German, & Ting, 2005) and goal
setting (Kingston & Hardy, 1997), and moreover, its effects on
performance may be mediated by motivationally general-mastery imagery
(Beauchamp, Bray & Albinson, 2002).
Although these studies have shed light on the potential effects of
self-efficacy in sport, specifically golf, the issue of how
self-efficacy has been conceptualized and measured remains a problem and
affects the extent to which researchers can make useful comparisons
across studies and subpopulations of persons who play golf, e.g.
amateurs, professionals, college athletes. For example, in some studies,
self-efficacy has been represented by measures of state self-confidence
(e.g., Vealey, 1986; Kimball, 2001), whereas in other work, general
(non-sport specific) measures (e.g., Beauchamp et al., 2002) have been
utilized. Even in those instances where researchers have established a
scale's psychometric properties, instruments may be too long to be
of any practical use.
Importantly, while existing work has assessed golf self-efficacy in
a variety of ways, it has not done so utilizing a large and diverse
sample of amateur golfers, undermining the generalizability of findings
from (semi)professional players (e.g. McKay, Selig, Carlson, &
Morris, 1997) to amateurs, wherein the range of such scores might be
attenuated in samples of collegiate or professional players. Indeed,
such players might be expected to be more self-efficacious to begin
with. Such findings may not as well generalize to older golfers, who are
rapidly increasing in numbers not only among amateurs, but also among
more skilled players (see e.g., Chmiel & Morris, 2001), reflecting
the increase in the proportion of older persons relative to the general
population (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 2000). For older players, golf
self efficacy is likely to play a key role in allowing for the
maintenance of their skills over time, as well as serving as a buffer
against age-related declines in golf skills (Baltes & Baltes, 1990).
Indeed, one could argue that a positive attitude toward the game,
reflecting self-efficacious beliefs regarding one's skills, is an
equally important determinant of successful golf performance among older
persons, relative to the extent of their golf skills per se (Chmiel
& Morris, 2001; Rotella & Cullen, 1995).
Rationale for and Purpose of the Present Study
In this context, what appears to be missing and would be useful for
both researchers and practitioners is a short, reliable, and valid
measure of golf self-efficacy. Developing a measure of golf
self-efficacy (the Golf Self Efficacy Scale, GSES) and establishing its
psychometric properties, based upon data gathered from a large and
diverse sample of amateur players, were the purposes of this study.
In this respect, construct validity reflects the extent to which a
given measure correlates with other similar measures assessing a given
general construct, wherein we might expect such relationships to be
moderately high, but not so high as to suggest that the measure is
redundant (Anastasi & Urbina, 1997; Thorndike & Thorndike,
2009). To the extent that a measure adequately differentiates persons
utilizing an already available criterion, it possesses criterion-related
concurrent validity; if a measure predicts a criterion that is
temporally distinct (reflecting a criterion measure that has yet to be
collected), it possesses predictive criterion-related validity (Anastasi
& Urbina, 1997; Thorndike & Thorndike, 2009).
In particular, this present study sought to establish the
reliability and factorial composition of the GSES, as well as 1) the
criterion-related predictive validity of the GSES (regarding the extent
to which GSES scores predicted pre-round tension on the day of the
tournament as well as actual tournament performance), 2) the concurrent
criterion-related validity of the GSES (does the scale differentiate
persons based upon for example, age or skill level?), and 3) the
construct validity of the GSES (does the scale correlate with other
measures offer example, self confidence, anxiety, or perceptions of
one's golf skills?) (see Anastasi & Urbina, 1997; Thorndike
& Thorndike, 2009).
Given the above literature and definitions of concurrent,
construct, and predictive validity, we might expect that the GSES would
not only be reliable, but if the GSES possessed construct validity, it
would correlate with not only measures of generalized self efficacy but
also relate to measures of sport performance anxiety and multiple
indices of players' perceptions of their golf skills. Regarding
concurrent validity, we would expect that the GSES would differentiate
persons on the bases of age (important given the growing number of older
players) and golf skill (see e.g., Hassmen, Raglin, & Lundqvist,
2004). The GSES's predictive validity would be evidenced via the
scale's ability to predict not only tournament performance, but
also players' predicted first round scores and pre-round
tension/anxiety (see below).
Method
Participants
Two hundred and twenty male golfers from the Dupont World Amateur
Tournament in Myrtle Beach, South Carolina participated. On average,
participating golfers were 44.46 years old (SD = 8.47, Range = 23-64),
and were primarily Caucasian (n=203). Mean years of education was 14.76
(SD=2.48) for the present sample. Participants had been playing golf
from 2 to 50 years (M=18.44, SD = 10.36). Golfers also reported on their
practice habits, wherein persons reported practicing once (31%) or twice
(27%) a week, though some (4.5%) reported practicing every day; 39%
reported that when they did practice, it was for less than one hour.
Just over 73% were members of private golf clubs and 23.7% reported
having taken a golf lesson from a teaching professional in the last six
months.
Measures
Golf Self-Efficacy Scale (GSES). To assess golf self-efficacy, a
12-item measure, the Golf Self Efficacy Scale (GSES), was developed that
targeted key skills/behaviors associated with being a successful golfer.
Using an 11-point Likert Scale, ranging from 0, Not at all confident, to
50, Moderately confident, to 100, Completely confident, golfers
indicated their confidence in: consistency in 1) driving from the tee,
2) putting, 3) short irons, 4) long irons, and 5) fairway woods, as well
as 6) having good alignment or posture, 7) selecting the correct club
for a shot, 8) making necessary adjustments to one's grip or swing,
9) having good course management skills, 10) hitting trouble shots, 11)
playing out of the sand and 12) staying focused while playing. Each of
these skills is key to consistently good performance in golf (see Chmiel
& Morris, 2001; Nicklaus, Bowden, & McQueen, 2005; Palmer, 1983;
Rotella & Cullen, 1995; Watson, 1983).
Self-Efficacy Scale (SES). The 23-item Self-Efficacy Scale (SES;
Sherer & Adams, 1983; Sherer, Maddux, Mercandante, Prentice-Dunn,
Jacobs, & Rogers, 1982) measures both general
(initiation/persistence in the face of adversity) and social (social
confidence) self-efficacy expectations. Each item is presented in a
5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly
disagree). Total scores are each subscale's average and higher
scores indicate
greater levels of self-efficacy. Cronbach alphas for the SES in the
current sample were .85 (General) and .65 (Social). Sherer et al. and
Sherer and Adams have each provided extensive information on the
scale's validity.
Golf Performance Survey (GPS). The Golf Performance Survey (GPS;
Thomas & Over, 1994) assesses the psychological and psychomotor
skills associated with golf performance and level of involvement in
golf. Each item is presented in a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Total scores on each subscale
are obtained by averaging the respective items; higher scores indicate
greater confidence in each area. Thomas and Over found a stable 3 factor
structure (Psychological Skills: Negative Emotions and Cognitions,
Mental Preparation, Conservative Approach, Concentration, Striving for
Maximum Distance; Golf Involvement: Commitment; Psychomotor Performance:
Automaticity, Putting Skill, Seeking Improvement) for the GPS, and as
well as finding GPS subscale scores to predict shot-making skills, based
upon data gathered from professional golfers in an Australian
tournament. Thomas and Over also found among amateurs GPS subscale
scores to be higher among more skilled players. Thomas and Fogarty
(1997) found GPS scores to be sensitive to the effects of a two-month
imagery/self taught training program among amateur golfers.
Because of the unique nature of the Commitment scale (see Thomas
& Over, 1994; Thomas & Fogarty, 1997) and to avoid unnecessary
overlap with other measures utilized here [Sport Anxiety Scale, measures
of physical and mental preparation, self efficacy, and areas of needed
improvement (see below)], only the GPS Automaticity, Putting Skill,
Seeking Improvement (all indexing Psychomotor Skills), and Commitment
subscales were used here (37 of 68 items in the GPS). While subsequent
analysis of these subscales necessitated the deletion of the Seeking
Improvement subscale due to its lower reliability (alpha = .56),
Cronbach alphas for the remaining GPS subscales in current sample were
.69 (Automaticity), .72 (Putting), and .79 (Commitment). A modified GPS
total score was created for purposes of the present study, reflecting
the sum of the Commitment, Putting, and Automaticity subscales.
Sport Anxiety Scale (SAS). The 18-item Sport Anxiety Scale (SAS;
Smith, Smoll, & Schultz, 1990) measures three types of
sport-specific trait anxiety: somatic (8 items), worry (7 items), and
concentration disruption (3 items). Participants rated each item on a
4-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (not at all how I typically feel
before or during a competition) to 4 (very much how I feel prior to or
during competition). Total scores for each factor are obtained by
averaging that scale's items; higher scores indicate more anxiety.
Cronbach alphas for the SAS in the current sample were .90 (Somatic),
.87 (Worry), and .68 (Concentration Disruption). Smith et al. have
provided extensive information about the scale's factor structure
through exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses, as well as
providing information regarding the construct validity of the SAS.
Golf-Specific Problem Areas. Participants indicated whether they
had experienced problems in the following areas of their play: driving
the ball straight (42%), short game (25.6%), concentration (25.6%),
putting (24.2%), driving the ball long (18.3%), sand play (17.8%), short
iron play (15,1%), feeling relaxed and ready to play (13.7%), long iron
play (11.9%) and trouble shots (6.8%). Cronbach's alpha for the
overall problem score, representing the extent to which golfers endorsed
the above areas as problematic for them personally, was .77 in the
present sample.
Physical and Mental Preparation. Golfers also rated the extent (not
at all to extremely) to which they prepared themselves physically (2
items) and mentally (3 items) prior to practice, play, or competition,
based upon 5 such items (e.g., stretching, relaxing, thinking
positively, using mental imagery). Cronbach's alpha for this
measure of pre-round preparation was .86 in the present sample.
Factors Influencing Efforts at Improvement. Considering a list of
24 factors (e.g., relying upon new, game improvement equipment, taking
private lessons, playing with friends, practicing, controlling
one's emotions, playing with the best equipment, remaining fit,
playing easy or difficult courses), participants also rated the
importance (not at all important to extremely important) of each factor
in helping them to either maintain and/or improve their golf games. For
the measure of factors that helped to maintain/improve one's golf
performance, Cronbach's alpha in the present sample was. 81.
Pre-Round Performance Anxiety. Immediately prior to teeing off,
golfers indicated along a 5-point scale how physically and
psychologically anxious/tense they were at present, responding to 6
questions targeting such feelings (e.g., feeling nervous, tight, being
concerned about performing well, being a disappointment to others).
Cronbach's alpha for this measure of pre-round anxiety/tension was
.83.
Procedure and Analyses
Golfers who were scheduled to play at one of four courses were
identified by the tournament director as potential participants in the
study. Participation per se was not random; those who participated were
self-selected. However, as golfers were randomly assigned to courses,
this sample of male golfers represented both a random and a
representative sample of tournament participants; indeed, the vast
majority (over 80%) of tournament participants were males. At
registration, golfers were approached, the purpose of the study
explained (to survey their attitudes about their golf games) and their
participation solicited. Over 90% who were approached volunteered to
participate in the present study. Then, in small groups, they provided
written consent, and subsequently completed the above described
questionnaires.
Measures of pre-round anxiety/tension and predicted Round 1 scores
were taken the morning after the above discussed measures (GSES, GPS,
SES, SAS, preparation, problem areas, efforts at improvement) were
completed. Players' questionnaire packets were coded by number so
their scores during the four rounds of the tournament could be matched
to their responses. Following the tournament, the tournament director
provided the golfers' gross scores (i.e., uncorrected for handicap)
as well as their tournament verified handicaps.
Exploratory factor analysis (see below) was used to examine the
factor structure of the Golf Self-Efficacy Scale (GSES); once this
factor structure was determined, factor scores were calculated in
addition to internal consistency reliability. Pearson product-moment
correlations with the other measures were used to evaluate the
scale's construct, concurrent, and predictive validity. An ANOVA
examined the GSES's discriminant validity regarding the impact of
level of golf skill on GSES scores.
Results
Factor Structure and Reliability of the GSES
Principle axis factor analysis with squared multiple correlations
as communality estimates was used to explore the GSES's underlying
factor structure. A single factor explained 62.33% of the common
variance among items (Eigenvalue = 5.08); Cronbach's alpha for this
single factor score was .87. These findings suggest that the GSES is a
unidimensional and reliable measure of golfers' perceived efficacy.
Concurrent Validity
Correlational findings (see Table 1) speaking to the concurrent
validity of the GSES suggested that while golf self-efficacy was
unrelated to the number of years golfers had played and how often
persons practiced, GSES scores related to how much time they spent
practicing when they did practice. Importantly, golf self-efficacy was
negatively related to golfers' verified tournament handicaps (where
higher handicaps indexed less skill, see Table 1).
Construct Validity
As expected, and consistent with evidence supporting the
scale's construct validity, GSES scores were moderately and
positively associated with both general and social self-efficacy (see
Table 1). In addition, higher golf self-efficacy was related to lower
scores from each subscale of the Sport Anxiety Scale: somatic, worry,
concentration disruption, as well as to SAS total scores (see Table 1).
For golf specific skills, as expected, higher golf self-efficacy
was related to greater GPS automaticity, more skill in putting, and
greater commitment to the game of golf, as well as to higher GPS
modified total scores. In addition, those golfers whose GSES scores were
higher indicated that they had fewer problem areas in their games, were
more likely to prepare themselves prior to practicing and competing, and
endorsed to a greater extent a variety of factors that they thought had
helped them to improve their games (see Table 1).
Discriminant Validity
An ANOVA of GSES scores broken down by 3 levels of skill, given the
distribution of verified handicaps in the sample, where cell sizes were
at least 39 (handicaps < 9.2; > 13.2 < 15.5; > 18.0),
suggested that level of skill reliably impacted GSES scores,
[F.sub.2,209] = 14.26,p < .01 (Ms = 70.04, 62.97, 62.06,
respectively). Post hoc Scheffe tests indicated that those who were the
most skilled had higher (p < .01) GSES scores than those whose skill
levels were lower; persons who handicaps placed them in the moderate and
least skilled levels of golf proficiency had similar GSES scores. GSES
scores were negatively, but nevertheless at best only moderately related
to players' ages.
Predictive Validity
Correlational findings indicated that GSES scores predicted
players' unadjusted tournament scores (where lower scores index
better performance, independent of handicap) in rounds 1, 3, and 4. Golf
self-efficacy was however, unrelated to round 2 scores. Higher GSES
scores were also related to better predicted fast round scores (see
Table 1), wherein such predicted scores were also strongly related to
Round 1 scores (r = .64, p < .01). Controlling for GSES scores
attenuated this relationship to an extent (partial r = .60). GSES scores
also predicted pre-round anxiety/tension scores (see Table 1), and the
latter were also related to predicted Round 1 scores (r =. 19, p <
.02), but not to Round 1 scores per se. Controlling for GSES scores
attenuated the pre-round anxiety/tension--predicted score relationship
(partial r =. 10, ns).
Discussion
Reliability, Factor Structure, Concurrent, and Construct Validity
of the GSES
In view of the GSES's high internal consistency reliability
and unidimensionality, and in light of its brevity, it has promise for
use in both research-related and teaching-related contexts. The findings
presented here speak to the concurrent validity of the GSES, wherein it
was principally related to players' handicaps, and importantly, to
the construct validity of the GSES, wherein golf self-efficacy was
related to, but nevertheless separate from generalized self-efficacy.
The partial but non-overlapping relationship between generalized and
golf-specific self efficacy found here is parallel to work exploring
relationships between generalized and domain-specific locus of control
(see Lachman, 1986; Wallston, Wallston, & DeVellis, 1978) as well as
to that examining relationships between generalized and domain-specific
hardiness (see Nowack, 1989, 1990). The GSES's relationship to SAS
and GPS scores also supports its construct validity.
Discriminant and Predictive Validity
The findings here also speak to the GSES's discriminant
validity (in differentiating persons by skill level), consistent with
previous research exploring other aspects of golfers' perceptions
of their skills (e.g., Thomas & Over, 1994). GSES scores were also
moderately related to age, reflecting the tendency for older players to
have less confidence in their skills (see Chmiel & Morris, 1997).
This might suggest that older players would especially benefit from
efforts to change their self efficacy beliefs, setting the stage for
improved performance among such persons. Regarding predictive
criterion-related validity, consistent with the above research on
self-efficacy and sport performance, these findings suggest that greater
golf self-efficacy predicts better performance during tournament play
(re: GSES scores predicted performance in 3 of 4 tournament rounds).
They also suggest that golfers with higher GSES scores were more
optimistic about their first round performance and were less anxious
prior to teeing off at the tournament's outset.
Implications for Research and Teaching
Viewed from a research perspective, the GSES findings here
underscore its utility in the efficient assessment of players'
beliefs about their golf skills. In this respect, GSES scores were
related to not only players' perceptions of the difficulties they
were having with their golf games, but also to the degree to which they
prepared mentally and physically prior to playing, to efforts they had
made to improve their golf performance, and to both predicted and actual
Round I performance. While it is entirely possible that greater golf
self efficacy makes it more likely that golfers will engage in more
preparatory or performance improvement-related behavior, greater
confidence in one's golf skills could also be an outcome of such
behavior. As these data are correlational, they cannot speak to issues
of causality, wherein cross lagged longitudinal analyses would be
necessary to answer such questions.
Significantly however in this respect, persons whose predicted
Round I scores were lower indeed played better on the first day of the
tournament, and each of these scores was related to golf self efficacy,
though controlling for GSES did not attenuate the predicted score-actual
performance relationship a great deal. However, the relationship between
pre-round anxiety and Round 1 performance was more strongly attenuated
when controlling for GSES scores. Generally speaking, this suggests golf
self efficacy to in part mediate the relationship between both pre-round
expectations/one's physical/mental state prior to playing and
actual tournament performance. Given the importance such persons attach
to doing well in a national tournament (for which they may have spend a
great deal of money and invested a great deal of preparation time),
attaining insight about the nature of their thoughts regarding their
game and the influence of such thoughts on one's golf score is
important in maximizing both tournament performance and the enjoyment of
the experience of competition.
These data also underscore the importance of golf self efficacy in
contributing positively to multiple aspects of the management of
one's game, such as being more involved in the sport (re: GPS
commitment scores), viewing one's putting as more adequate,
engaging in more physical and mental preparatory activities prior to
play, exhibiting more improvement-related behaviors, and thinking in a
more automatized fashion while playing. Such behavior is likely to
ultimately lead to better tournament performance.
Pedagogically, these findings highlight the importance of making
students aware of the fact that their beliefs about their skills can
indeed alternatively detract from or enhance their tournament
performance, apart from their play per se during the tournament itself.
From this perspective then, the GSES might be used to identify persons
who self efficacy beliefs and expectations about their ability to
improve might be counterproductive. As noted above, older players may
especially benefit from efforts to change their self efficacy beliefs.
For players whose GSES scores are lower, dysfunctional beliefs can be
isolated, targeted, and challenged, helping players to not only
understand that negative perceptions of their skills at present can
easily undermine their performance and detract from their enjoyment of
the game, but also learn to think in a more self efficacious manner as
it applies to their golf games.
Via the accurate assessment of their golf-self efficacy beliefs as
it pertains to specific areas of strength and weakness, such persons,
through instruction and self-assessment, could enhance their mental
approach to the game, enabling them to realistically match their skills
with their expectations (see Rotella & Cullen, 1995), yet allowing
them to remain optimistic about their ability to improve. For golfers
whose GSES scores are higher, who are more motivated to prepare mentally
and physically before playing, and who are more attuned to areas in
their games requiring improvement, teaching might focus on the
improvement of golf skills per se.
While the above data is relevant to only male golfers (see Bond,
Biddle, & Ntoumanis, 2001; Johnson, 1994), and in some cases, the
reported correlations only reflect a small proportion of variance, these
findings are nevertheless based upon a large and heterogeneous sample of
amateur players, to include older (50+) persons. The findings presented
here do suggest that the attitudes toward one's golf skills can be
reliably and accurately measured in an efficient manner, wherein such
beliefs contribute to better golf performance, leading to more enjoyment
of the game, permitting continued and active participation well into
one's adult years.
References
Anastasi, A., & Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological Testing (7th
Edition). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Baltes, B.P., & Baltes., M.M. (1990). Successful aging:
Perspectives from the behavioral Sciences. In P. B. Baltes (Ed.),
European Network on Longitudinal Studies on Individual Development, (pp.
114-131). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New
York: W.H. Freeman.
Bandura, A. (1995). Exercise of personal and collective efficacy in
changing societies. In A. Bandura (Ed.), Self-efficacy in changing
societies (pp 1-45). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of
behavioral change, Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.
Barling, J., & Abel, M. (1983). Self-efficacy beliefs and
tennis performance. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 7, 265-272.
Beauchamp, M. R., Bray, S. R., & Albinson, J. G. (2002).
Pre-competition imagery, self-efficacy and performance in collegiate
golfers. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20, 697-705.
Bond, K. A., Biddle, S. J. H., & Ntoumanis, N. (2001)
Self-efficacy and causal attribution in female golfers, International
Journal of Sport Psychology, 32, 243-256.
Catley, D., & Duda, J. (1997). Psychological antecedents of the
frequency and intensity of flow in golfers, International Journal of
Sport Psychology, 28, 309-322.
Chmiel, D., & Morris, K. (2001). Golf past 50. Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics Press.
Christensen, D. S. (2000). Self-efficacy, cognitive interference,
sport anxiety and psychological skills as predictors of performance in
intercollegiate golf Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Washington.
Compton, B. J. (2003). The effect of two golf training techniques,
a hologram ball and dry swings on performance and self-efficacy in
novice golfers. Unpublished Masters Thesis, Ball State University.
Edward, C. J. (I 983). Golf performance: Thinking, imaging, and
doing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Illinois Institute of
Technology.
Feltz, D.L. (1988). Self-confidence and sports performance,
Exercise and Sport Science Reviews, 16, 423-457.
Ferguson, R., Lirgg, C. D., Gorman, D., & Ting, L. (2005). The
effect of modified golf equipment and instruction on self-efficacy in
golf skills, Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 76, A-71.
Gagnon, M. (2002) Physical self-efficacy and amateurs'
performance on the golf course. Unpublished Masters Thesis. McGill
University.
Gayton, W.F., Matthews, G.R., & Burchstead, G.N. (1986). An
investigation of the validity of physical self-efficacy in predicting
marathon performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 63, 752-754.
Hassmen, P., Raglin, J. S., & Lundqvist, C. (2004).
Intra-individual variability in state anxiety and self-confidence in
elite golfers. Journal of Sport Behavior, 27, 277-290.
Johnson, D. R. (1994). Self-efficacy of male and female golfers at
differing ability levels. Microform Publications, Eugene, OR. University
of Oregon.
Kimball, K. L. (2001). Pre-competitive and post-competitive state
anxiety and performance of amateur golfers. Unpublished Masters Thesis,
Springfield College.
Kingston, K. M., & Hardy, L. (1997) Effects of different types
of goals on processes that support performance. The Sport Psychologist,
11, 277-293.
Koczajowski, D. L. (1997). State and trait sport-confidence and
physical self-efficacy of professional and amateur golfers. Microform
publication, Eugene, OR. International Institute for Sport & Human
Performance.
Krane, V., & Williams, J.M. (1992). Path analysis examining
relationships among cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, state
confidence, performance expectations and golf performance. Journal of
Sport Behavior, 15, 279-296.
Kuczka, K. K., & Treasure, D. C. (2005) Self-handicapping in
competitive sport: Influence of the motivational climate, self-efficacy
and perceived importance. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 6,
539-550.
Lachman, M. E. (1986). Locus of control in aging research: A case
for multidimensional and domain-specific assessment. Psychology and
Aging, 1, 34-40.
McAuley, E., & Grill, D.L. (1983). Reliability and validity of
the physical self-efficacy scale in a competitive sport setting. Journal
of Sport Psychology, 5, 410-418.
McKay, J., Selig, S., Carlson, J., & Morris, T. (1997).
Psychophysiological stress in elite golfers during practice and
competition. Australian Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 29,
55-61.
Nicklaus, J., Bowden, K., & McQueen, J. (2005). Golf my way:
The instructional classic, Revised and updated. New York: Simon &
Schuster.
Nowack, K. M. (1989). Cognitive style, cognitive hardiness, and
health status. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 12, 145-158.
Nowack, K.M. (1990). Initial development of an inventory to assess
stress and health risk. American Journal of Health Promotion, 4,
173-180.
Palmer, A. (1983). My game and yours. New York: Simon &
Schuster.
Pickens, M. M., Rotella, R. J., & Gansneder, B. M. (1996). The
effect of putting-confidence on putting-performance, Journal of Sport
Behavior, 19, 148-162.
Rotella, B., & Cullen, B. (1995). Golf is not a game of
perfect. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Rotter, J.B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus
external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General
& Applied, 80, 1-28.
Ryckman, R.M., Robbins, M.A., & Thorton, B. (1982). Development
and validation of a physical self-efficacy scale. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 5, 891-900.
Sherer, M., & Adams, C. H. (1983). Construct validation of the
self-efficacy scale. Psychological Reports, 53, 899-902.
Sherer, M. Maddux, D., Mercandante, C., Prentice-Dunn, P., Jacobs,
R., & Rogers, S. (1982). The Self-efficacy Scale: Construction and
validation. Psychological Reports, 51, 663-671.
Smith, R.E., Smoll, F.L., & Schutz, S. (1990). Measurements and
correlates of sport-specific cognitive and somatic trait anxiety,
Anxiety Research, 2, 263-280.
Stein, G. L., Kimiecik, J. C., Daniels, J., & Jackson, S. A.
(1995) Psychological antecedents of flow in recreational sport, Society
for Personality & Social Psychology, 21, 125-135.
Thomas, P.R., & Fogarty, G.J. (1997). Mental skills training is
associated with performance improvements in golf. The Sport
Psychologist, 11, 86-106.
Thomas, P.R., & Over, R. (1994). Psychological and psychomotor
skills associated with performance in golf, The Sport Psychologist, 8,
73.
Thorndike, R.M., & Thorndike, T. (2009). Measurement and
evaluation in psychology and education (8th edition). Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
U.S. Bureau of the Census (2000). Current Population Reports.
Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Vealey, R.S. (1986). Conceptualization of sport confidence and
competitive orientation: Preliminary investigation and instrument
development, Journal of Sport Psychology, 8, 221-246.
Wallston, K., Wallston, B.S., & Devellis, R. (1978).
Development of the multidimensional health locus of control (MHLC)
scales. Health Education Monograpghs, 6, 160-170.
Watson, T. (1983). Getting up and down: How to save strokes from
forty yards and in. New
Woolfolk, R. L., Murphy, S. M., Gottesfeld, D., & Aitken, D.
(1985). Effects of mental practice of task motor activity and mental
depiction of task outcome on motor skill performance, Journal of Sport
Psychology, 7, 191-197.
Bert Hayslip Jr., Carlin M. Raab, Philip C. Baczewski, and Trent A.
Petrie
University of North Texas
Address Correspondence to: Bert Hayslip Jr., Department of
Psychology, 1155 Union Circle #311280, University of North Texas,
Denton, TX 76203-5017.
Table 1. Golf Self-Efcacy Correlation Matrix.
Golf Self Efficacy
Age -.22 **
Years Played .13
Problem (a) -.38 **
Preparation (b) .27 **
Improvement (c) .32 **
Often Played -.13
Often Practice -.07
Length Practice .14 *
Handicap -.35 **
General Self Efficacy .30 **
Social Self Efficacy .23 **
SAS somatic anxiety -.32 **
Pre-round
tension/anxiety -.26 **
SAS worry -.42 **
SAS Conc D is (d) -.31 **
SASTotal Score -.45 **
GPSAutomaticity .55 **
GPS Putting .38 **
GPSCommitment .24 **
GPS Total Score .60 **
Round 1 Score -.23 **
Round 2 Score .14
Round 3 Score -.23 **
Round 4 Score -.31 **
Predicted Score
Round 1 -.37 **
Note: (a.) Higher scores indicate more perceived problems in one's
game.
(b.) Higher scores reflect more preparation.
(c.) Higher scores reflect more involvement in improvement-related
behaviors
(d.) SAS Concentration Disruption
* p <.05 ** p <.01 N = 220