Testing the team identification-social psychological health model: examining non-marquee sports, seasonal differences, and multiple teams.
Wann, Daniel L. ; Keenan, Brian ; Page, Leslie 等
Within the last decade or so, the field of psychology has begun to
experience a paradigm shift. This new vision, referred to as positive
psychology, reflects the belief that the field of psychology has devoted
too much empirical and theoretical effort toward understanding human
maladjustments and shortcomings while investing too little time in
understanding the positive aspects of the human experience, such as
happiness and mental well-being (see Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith,
1999; Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000; Snyder & Lopez, 2001).
This tendency to focus primarily on the darker sides of human existence
also is reflected in the social scientific inquiry of sport fandom and
spectating. Historically, investigators have spent a majority of their
efforts researching the negative aspects of this pastime, choosing to
focus on such topics as spectator violence, the poor interpersonal
relationships of sport fans, and how sport can serve as an opiate to the
masses (many of these attacks against fans have little empirical
support, see Wann, Melnick, Russell, & Pease, 2001, for a review).
However, consistent with the positive psychology movement found in
general psychology, sport scientists have recently begun to explore the
positive side of sport fandom as well. One such line of research has
investigated the relationship between sport fandom and psychological
well-being. Research on this relationship has taken two directions.
First, research indicates that fans may improve their self-concept
(i.e., well-being) by basking in the reflected glory of a sport
team's successes (Boen, Vanbeselaere, & Feys, 2002; Cialdini et
al., 1976; Dalakas, Madrigal, & Anderson, 2004). Indeed, many fans
choose to follow historically successful teams to have easier access to
this route to enhanced well-being (End, Dietz-Uhler, Harrick, &
Jacquemotte, 2002).
The focus of the current investigation concerns a second route to
enhanced well-being through sport fandom. This path involves one's
level of team identification. Team identification is defined as the
extent to which a fan feels a psychological connection to his or her
team (Wann & Branscombe, 1993; Wann et al., 2001). When individuals
feel a strong sense of attachment to a group, they experience enhanced
well-being because their association with the group leads to a sense of
belongingness with others and a connection to society (Gibson, Willming,
& Holdnak, 2002; Mael & Ashforth, 2001). Thus, this route to
well-being is not dependent upon group or team success. Rather,
individuals may gain a sense of belonging (and hence, well-being) simply
through their group memberships. Recent work has confirmed this pattern
of effects with sport fans and their sense of identification with their
team. Specifically, high levels of identification with a local team have
been found to be related to higher levels of personal and collective
self-esteem, vigor, positive emotions, social life satisfaction,
openness, and extroversion, and lower levels of depression, alienation,
loneliness, and negative emotions (Branscombe &Wann, 1991; Lanter
& Blackburn, 2004; Wann, 1994; Wann, 2006b; Wann, Dimmock, &
Grove, 2003; Wann, Dunham, Byrd, & Keenan, 2004; Wann, Inman, Ensor,
Gates, & Caldwell, 1999; Wann & Pierce, 2005; Wann, Walker,
Cygan, Kawase, & Ryan, 2005).
Wann (2006a) recently developed a theoretical model to account for
the aforementioned effects. This model, termed the Team
Identification--Social Psychological Health Model, proposes that team
identification facilitates well-being by increasing social connections
for the fan. That is, by identifying with a team, a fan gains additional
associations with others, connections that can be important in
facilitating psychological well-being (Baumeister & Leary, 1995;
Linville, 1987). According to the model, two different forms of social
connections can be acquired through sport team identification: enduring
and temporary. Enduring social connections occur when a fan resides in
an environment in which other fans of the team are easily identifiable,
such as when someone supports a hometown (i.e., local) team. Temporary
social connections involve situations in which the fan does not live in
an enduring environment (e.g., displaced fans supporting distant teams),
but connections to other fans of the team are rendered temporarily
salient (such as when one attends a party frequented by similarly
displaced fans of a given team). The model contends further that the
relationship between identification (and the subsequent enduring and
temporary social connections) and well-being is mediated by threats to
social identity (e.g., poor team performance) and efforts to cope with
threats. The social connections resulting from team identification are
expected to impact both state (via increases in temporary social
connections) and trait well-being (via enduring connections). Finally,
because research indicates that group and team identification are more
closely related to social well-being than personal well-being (Rubin
& Hewstone, 1998; Wann et al., 2003), temporary and enduring social
connections are predicted to have their greatest impact on social
psychological health.
As noted, research is quite supportive of Wann's (2006a)
model. For instance, although identification with a local team is
positively associated with social psychological well-being (Branscombe
& Wann, 1991; Wann et al., 2005), researchers have failed to find a
relationship between well-being and identification with distant teams or
sport fandom per se (Wann et al., 2004; Wann et al., 1999; Wann&
Pierce, 2005). Theoretically, this pattern of effects would be expected
because mere sport fandom and identifying with distant teams should not
readily lead to social connections (unless, in the case with distant
teams, temporary connections are made salient). However, while past work
has substantiated the model, there are still many unanswered questions
regarding the identification--well-being relationship.
First, Wann (2006b) noted that the past research had examined
identifications with popular and prominent local teams, such as a local
university's men's basketball team (e.g., Branscombe
&Wann, 1991; Wann& Pierce, 2005). Investigators had yet to
examine the relationship between identification with less popular,
non-revenue generating teams and well-being. According to the Team
Identification--Social Psychological Health Model, a team's
popularity should not impact one's ability to gain well-being
effects through the team. Rather, the model would predict that
identification with less popular teams and psychological health will be
positively related as long as societal connections can be generated
through associations with the team and these associations are apparent
to the fans. Thus, it was hypothesized that the positive correlation between team identification and well-being found for "marquee"
sports would be replicated for non-revenue generating sports that also
lead to enduring connections.
Second, according to Wann's (2006a) model, team identification
facilitates social well-being by leading to enduring and/or temporary
social connections. If this is indeed the case, one would expect that
the timing of assessments of identification and well-being, that is,
in-season or off-season, to have little impact on the relationship. For
instance, imagine a fan who is a supporter of her university's
football team. By living in the university community, other supporters
of this team are readily available whether the team is in-season or not.
Similarly, fans of professional sport teams such as the Boston Red Sox and Los Angeles Lakers living in their respective cities find themselves
immersed in a group of fellow fans year round (e.g., Red Sox fans
residing in Boston can be found wearing Red Sox apparel, discussing the
team, etc.). Thus, enduring social connections to others should be
readily available and salient regardless of the season. Consequently,
positive relationships should be found between identification with a
local team and social well-being whether the team is playing or not.
Such a pattern of effects was the focus of Hypothesis 2.
Third, it is clear that sport fans often maintain allegiances to
many different teams (Giulianotti, 2002). Knowing that high levels of
identification with a team can facilitate well-being, Wann (2006b)
wondered what happens in situations in which a fan has high levels of
identification with multiple teams. Is identification with one team
sufficient for generating the greatest effect or, rather, do fans with
multiple identifications (and the corresponding multiple social
connections) enjoy even greater well-being benefits? Researchers in
social psychology have examined the relationship between multiple group
memberships and well-being. For instance, Wann and Hamlet (1994) found
that individuals with a strong desire to join and maintain memberships
in diverse groups (referred to as "joiners") report positive
patterns of psychological well-being (see also Wann & Ensor, 2001;
Wann & Hamlet, 1996). Further, Roccas and Brewer (2002) noted that
high levels of social identity complexity (i.e., perceived overlap
between one's group memberships) are related to more successful
coping with threats to social identity. In fact, these authors state
that, "members of a discriminated group can enhance their
self-esteem by identifying with a successful sports team" (p. 104).
Applying the aforementioned work to the relationship between sport team
identification and well-being, one could predict that identifying with
multiple teams to be even more advantageous than forming a strong
attachment to a single team. However, simply having associations with a
variety of groups will not be enough to garner the well-being benefits
of group membership. For example, in the Wann and Hamlet (1994) work,
although there was a positive relationship between tendencies to be a
joiner and well-being, raw numbers of group memberships were not related
to psychological health. Wright and Forsyth (1997) report a similar
finding, concluding that, "Memberships in many groups, per se, was
not related to self-esteem" (p. 48). Rather, the group membership
needed to be satisfying to the individual (or prestigious) for the
effect to occur. Thus, simply being a casual fan of multiple teams
should not be beneficial. One would need to be a highly identified fan
of each team. Because the logic employed here is somewhat speculative
and Wann's (2006a) Team Identification--Social Psychological Health
Model does not address this issue, there were no hypotheses offered with
respect to the relationship between high levels of identification with
multiple teams and well-being. Rather, this topic was examined within
the framework of a research question.
Therefore, this research examined the following hypotheses and
research questions:
Hypothesis 1: Consistent with research on "marquee"
sports, there will be a positive correlation between team identification
and social psychological well-being for non-revenue generating sports
that lead to enduring connections.
Hypothesis 2: There will be positive relationships between
identification with a local team and social well-being both in season
and out of season.
Research Question l: Do persons with high levels of identification
with multiple local teams report more positive social well-being than
those with high levels of identification with only a single team?
The Current Investigation and Data Analyses
The aforementioned hypotheses and research question were examined
by assessing the team identification--well-being relationship for fans
of a college football team and a college baseball team at a university
where the men's basketball team has historically been the
preeminent team (i.e., the basketball team was a revenue sport while the
other two were not) (1). Hypothesis 1 was tested simply by examining
correlations among the variables for the lesser teams. Hypothesis 2 was
investigated by comparing correlations between identification and
well-being during the season with data acquired during the off-season.
Research question l was examined by categorizing participants into three
groups: those with high levels of identification with neither team,
those with high levels of identification with l team (either baseball or
football), and those with high levels of identification with both teams,
and then comparing the psychological well-being of persons in the three
groups.
Method
Participants
Participants were 272 university students. One hundred and
seventy-six of the participants (55 male, 121 female; M age = 20.52, SD
= 3.71) were tested during the fall semester (i.e., in-season for the
university's football team and off-season for the university's
baseball team) while 96 respondents (49 male, 47 female; Mage = 22.26,
SD = 4.94) participated during the spring semester (i.e., off-season for
the university's football team and in-season for the
university's baseball team).
Procedure
Upon entering the testing room and providing their consent to
participate, participants (tested in small groups) were asked to
complete a questionnaire packet (questionnaires contained in the packet
are described below). The packets were coded to indicate whether the
participant was tested during the fall or spring semester. After the
participants had completed the questionnaire packet, they returned it to
the researcher who handed them a debriefing statement. This statement
disclosed the purpose and hypotheses of the study and contained
information on contacting the author for a report of the research. Once
each participant had received the debriefing statement, the participants
were excused from the testing session. The sessions lasted approximately
15 minutes.
Materials
The questionnaire packet contained three sections. The first
section contained demographic items assessing age and gender. The second
section contained two versions of the Sport Spectator Identification
Scale (SSIS; Wann& Branscombe, 1993). The SSIS contains seven
Likert-scale items assessing identification with a sport team. Response
options to the SSIS ranged from 1 (low identification) to 8 (high
identification). A sample item from the SSIS reads, "How important
to you is it that (target team) wins?" Wann and Branscombe present
data indicating that the SSIS is a highly reliable and valid instrument
(Wann et al., 2001). Subjects targeted their university's baseball
team and their university's football team when completing the two
versions of the SSIS.
The third section contained two reliable and valid instruments
designed to assess social psychological health: the 16-item Collective
Self-esteem Scale (Luhtanen & Crocker, 1991) and the 20-item UCLA Loneliness Scale (Russell, Peplau, & Cutrona, 1980). These measures
were scored so that higher numbers reflected better psychological health
(i.e., higher levels of collective self-esteem and lower levels of
loneliness). These two measures were selected for three reasons. First,
these measures had been successfully used in past research examining the
psychological health of sport fans (e.g., Wann, 2006b; Wann et al.,
2003; Wann,& Pierce, 2005). Second, these scales assess components
of social well-being. For example, operational definitions of social
well-being (Keys & Lopez, 2001; Luhtanen & Crocker, 1991;
Russell, Cutrona, Rose, & Yurko, 1984) have included social
integration (e.g., loneliness, belonging, a sense of community) and
social (i.e., collective) self-esteem as components of social
well-being. As noted by Wann (2006a) and others (e.g., Rubin &
Hewstone, 1998; Wann et al., 2003), group memberships and associations
with sport teams should be more closely related to social well-being
than personal well-being, and thus, research in this area should employ
measures of social psychological health. And third, these measures
assess trait levels of well-being (rather than state). Because we were
interested in the consequences of enduring (i.e., chronic) social
connections, assessments of trait well-being were more appropriate
(Warm, 2006a).
Results
Responses to the collective self-esteem scale and the loneliness
inventory were correlated (r = .54, p < .01). Thus, scores on these
scales were converted to z scores and combined to form a single index of
social psychological health (i.e., social psychological health index =
collective self-esteem scale z score + loneliness inventory z score).
Hypothesis 1 (there would be a positive relationship between team
identification and social psychological health for non-revenue sports)
and Hypothesis 2 (the positive relationship would be found during the
season and during the off-season) were examined using Pearson
correlations computed between the variables of interest. These analyses
revealed support for Hypothesis 1 as both level of identification with
the football team (in-season r =. 15, [r.sup.2] = .02; off-season r =.
18, [r.sup.2] = .03) and identification with the baseball team
(in-season r =. 19, [r.sup.2] = .04; off-season r =. 19, [r.sup.2]= .04)
were positively and significantly correlated (all p's < .05;
Cohen's d's ranged from .30 to .39). In support of Hypothesis
2, comparisons of the coefficients for in-season and off-season failed
to reveal a significant difference by season (all z's < 1.00,
all p's >. 10). Thus, all subsequent analyses were conducted
across season (combining the in-season and off-season data resulted in r
=. 16 for football ([r.sup.2] = .03) and r =. 19 for baseball
([r.sup.2]= .04) (both p's < .01).
To examine Research Question 1, participants' scores on both
versions of the SSIS were used to establish three groups: not highly
identified with either team (LOW BOTH), highly identified with only one
team (HIGH ONE), and highly identified with both teams (HIGH BOTH).
Participants scoring at least 32 on the SSIS (i.e., greater than 4.5 per
item and thus, at or above the midpoint on the 1 to 8 scale) were
considered to have high levels of identification. This resulted in 205
participants being placed into the LOW BOTH group, 56 were classified as
HIGH ONE, and 11 were classified as HIGH BOTH. The mean scores for
psychological health were then examined for these groups using a trend
analysis. This analysis revealed a linear term (unweighted), F(1, 269) =
4.64,p < .05, [[eta].sup.2] = .02. Participants in the LOW BOTH group
had the lowest levels of social psychological health (M = -. 12, SD =
1.80) while the well-being for those in the HIGH ONE was moderately
positive (M = .22, SD = 1.60). However, the psychological health of
those in the HIGH BOTH group was particularly high (M = 1.05, SD =
1.15). The quadratic term was not significant, F(1,269) = 0.46, p >
.40.
Discussion
The data described above provide support for both hypotheses as
well as the Team Identification--Social Psychological Health Model
(Wann, 2006a). As expected, there was a positive relationship between
identification with non-revenue (i.e., non-marquee) local sport teams
and social psychological health and these relationships were found both
in-season and during the off-season. Although the relationships are
modest in size (e.g., r's ranged from. 15. to. 19; [r.sup.2]s
ranged from .02 to .04), the magnitude of the effect is similar to
previous work investigating the relationship between well-being and
other factors, such as income, education, and religiosity (Diener &
Clifton, 2002; Diener et al., 1999). Thus, additional factors would be
needed to account for the variance not explained by levels of team
identification.
With respect to the first hypothesis, it is important to note that
the teams examined in the current investigation were not only less
marquee than were teams examined in previous efforts, but were less
successful as well. For instance, several studies have looked at
identification with the men's basketball teams from the University
of Kansas and Murray State University. Each of these teams had had a
long history of success, particularly in recent years (e.g., each team
had won multiple conference regular season and/or tournament titles, had
regularly qualified for the NCAA tournament, etc.). Conversely, the
baseball and football teams examined in this investigation had not been
as successful. In fact, the twelve years prior to this investigation,
the baseball team had a combined winning percentage of .428 (247 wins,
330 losses) while the football team's winning percentage was .515
(69 wins, 65 losses). Thus, as suggested by the Team
Identification--Psychological Health Model, team success is not
necessary for sport fans to gain a sense of well-being through the
pastime. Certainly, identifying with successful teams can lead to a
positive sense of self as one basks in the team's accomplishments
(Cialdini et al., 1976; Dalakas et al., 2004). However, fans also may
reach a heightened sense of well-being through a second route as a
consequence of the social connections accrued through the associations
with other fans. For this route, the team need not be a historically
successful one. Rather, fans of teams with long histories of poor
performance (e.g., the Chicago Cubs Major League baseball team) also may
gain a sense of well-being through their allegiance to the team,
provided that they gain social connections through their involvement.
As for the second hypothesis and the lack of difference between the
in-season and off-season results, it is important to note that,
according to the Team Identification--Social Psychological Health Model
(Wann, 2006a), one would not expect to find a significant relationship
between team identification and well-being if the identification does
not result in increased enduring or temporary social capital. Thus, the
significant correlation between identification and well-being during the
off-season reported above suggests that, for these fans, the social
connections gained via their team support were salient (i.e., easily
identifiable) during the off-season as well. Given that the participants
were university students tested in a university setting, it is quite
likely that their associations with other fans were salient during the
off-season. That is, these persons were still able to communicate with
other fans, experience camaraderie with them, as so forth, even though
the team wasn't playing. Similarly, fans of many professional sport
teams really do not experience an "off-season" as a fan. For
instance, fans of the St. Louis Cardinals Major League Baseball team who
live in or around St. Louis have little difficulty finding other fans,
even during the winter months, because the Cardinals remain a frequent
topic of conversation for fans and the media alike and fans continue to
demonstrate their allegiance to the team by wearing apparel announcing
their allegiance. Such is precisely the benefit of residing in a
situation characterized by enduring connections (see Wann, 2006a); the
connections are readily available and visible simply by being present in
the environment. However, in situations where the social connections are
rendered non-salient during the off-season, one would not expect a
significant relationship between team identification and social
psychological health.
With respect to the research question, the data indicated that fans
with high levels of identification with two local (and thus, salient)
teams reported more positive levels of well-being than persons who were
highly identified with only one team. Individuals with low levels of
identification with both teams reported the poorest levels of social
psychological health. Evidently, the increased social connections gained
through multiple allegiances to sport teams lead to even greater
well-being benefits.
The finding that fans with multiple team associations (i.e., high
levels of identification with both teams) had particularly high levels
of social psychological health warrants special discussion. First, it
should be noted that this was the first empirical endeavor investigating
the impact of multiple team allegiances on fan well-being. Thus, it is
important to replicate this effect to ensure its viability. This need
for replication is particularly important given that the effect size for
this analysis was quite small. Second, although the current data set
suggests that there are additional social psychological health
advantages to identifying with multiple sport teams, it seems likely
that such an additive effect will have a limit. For instance, consider
work on the conformity effect. Research in this area suggests that,
although group size and amount of conformity are positively correlated,
the impact of increased group size reaches a peak at about five persons
(Asch, 1956; Gerard, Wilhelmy, & Conolley, 1968; but also see Bond
& Smith, 1996). That is, adding additional group members will
increase pressures to conform but only up to approximately five
individuals, at which point the effect levels off. A similar effect may
well be found with multiple sport team allegiances and well-being. For
instance, it seems unlikely that having a high level of identification
with 14 teams will yield significantly greater well-being benefits
compared to "only" identifying with 13 teams. Instead, it
seems reasonable to suggest that the benefits gained through multiple
sport team allegiances have a limit. Of course, the precise point when
this limit is reached is purely speculation at this point. However, such
a number could be estimated by testing a large sample from an
environment where there are many teams to which one could form an
allegiance (e.g., New York City, Chicago, Los Angeles). By examining the
relationship between social psychological health and the number of
strong allegiances (i.e., the number of teams through which one gains
connections to others), one could not only replicate the findings
reported here, but also examine any potential limits to the benefits
gained through multiple allegiances.
In conclusion, limitations of the current study warrant mention.
First, as noted earlier, the effect sizes detected were rather small.
Thus, replication of the current effects is warranted to insure their
validity. Second, the current research was limited to two different team
sports (baseball and football) both of which were at the same level of
competition (NCAA Division I). Additional researchers should attempt to
replicate the current investigation with other team sports such as
basketball and volleyball as well as individual sports (e.g., auto
racing, golf) at different levels of competition such as interscholastic and professional. And finally, because the participants targeted only
two teams, the ceiling effect for the additive advantages of multiple
identifications is not know. As discussed earlier, future endeavors
should examine larger numbers of teams to detect possible limits to the
additive effect of multiple team identifications.
References
Asch, S. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: A minority
of one against unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs, 70 (Whole
No. 416).
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. P. (1995). The need to belong:
Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation.
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529.
Boen, F., Vanbeselaere, N., & Feys, J. (2002). Behavioral
consequences of fluctuating group success: An Internet study of
soccer-team fans. The Journal of Social Psychology, 142, 769-781.
Bond, R., & Smith, P. B. (1996). Culture and conformity: A
meta-analysis of studies using Asch' s (1952b, 1956) line judgment
task. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 111-137.
Branscombe, N. R., & Wann, D. L. (1991). The positive social
and self-concept consquences of sports team identification. Journal of
Sport & Social Issues, 15, 115-127.
Cialdini, R. B., Borden, R. J., Thome, A., Walker, M. R., Freeman,
S., & Sloan, L. R. (1976). Basking in reflected glory: Three
(football) field studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
34, 366-375.
Dalakas, V., Madrigal, R., & Anderson, K. L. (2004). "We
are number one!" The phenomenon of basking-in-reflected-glory and
its implications for sport marketing. In L. R. Kahle & C. Riley
(Eds.), Sports marketing and the psychology of marketing communication
(pp. 67-79). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Diener, E., & Clifton, D. (2002). Life satisfaction and
religiosity in broad probability samples. Psychological Inquiry, 13,
206-209.
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999).
Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological
Bulletin, 125, 276-302.
End, C. M., Dietz-Uhler, B., Harrick, E. A., & Jacquemotte, L.
(2002). Identifying with winners: A reexamination of sport fans'
tendency to BIRG. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 1017-1030.
Gerard, H. B., Wilhelmy, R. A., & Conolley, E. S. (1968).
Conformity and group size. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
8, 79-82.
Gibson, H., Willming, C., & Holdnak, A. (2002).
"We're Gators ... not just Gator fans": Serious leisure
and University of Florida football. Journal of Leisure Research, 34,
397-425.
Giulianotti, R. (2002). Supporters, followers, fans, and flaneurs:
A taxonomy of spectator identities in football. Journal of Sport &
Social Issues, 26, 25-46.
Keyes, C. L. M., & Lopez, S. J. (2001). Toward a science of
mental health: Positive directions in diagnosis and interventions. In C.
R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp.
45-59). New York: Oxford.
Lanter, J. R., & Blackburn, J. Z. (2004, September). The
championship effect on college students' identification and
university affiliation. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
Association for the Advancement of Applied Sport Psychology,
Minneapolis, MN.
Linville, P. W. (1987). Self-complexity as a cognitive buffer
against stress related illness and depression. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 52, 663-676.
Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. (1991). Self-esteem and intergroup comparison: Toward a theory of collective self-esteem. In J. Suls &
T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social comparisons: Contemporary theory and research
(pp. 211-234). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mael, F. A., & Ashforth, B. E. (2001). Identification in work,
war, sports, and religion: Contrasting the benefits and risks. Journal
for the Theory of Social Behavior, 31, 198-222.
Roccas, S., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). Social identity complexity.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 88-106.
Rubin, M., & Hewstone, M. (1998). Social identity theory's
self-esteem hypothesis: A review and some suggestions for clarification.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 40-62.
Russell, D., Cutrona, C. E., Rose, J., & Yurko, K. (1984).
Social and emotional loneliness: An examination of Weiss's typology of loneliness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46,
1313-1321.
Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. E. (I 980). The
revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 472-480.
Seligman, M. E., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive
psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55, 5-14.
Snyder, C. R., & Lopez, S. J. (Eds.). (2001). Handbook of
positive psychology. New York: Oxford.
Wann, D. L. (1994). The "noble" sports fan: The
relationships between team identification, self-esteem, and aggression.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78, 864-866.
Wann, D. L. (2006a). Understanding the positive social
psychological benefits of sport team identification: The Team
Identification--Social Psychological Health Model. Group Dynamics:
Theory, Research, and Practice, 10, 272-296.
Wann, D. L. (2006b). Examining the potential causal relationship
between sport team identification and psychological well-being. Journal
of Sport Behavior, 29, 79-95.
Wann, D. L., & Branscombe, N. R. (1993). Sports fans: Measuring
degree of identification with the team. International Journal of Sport
Psychology, 24, 1-17.
Wann, D. L., Dimmock, J. A., & Grove, J. R. (2003).
Generalizing the Team Identification-Psychological Health Model to a
Different Sport and Culture: The Case of Australian Rules Football.
Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 7, 289-296.
Wann, D. L., Dunham, M. D., Byrd, M. L., & Keenan, B. L.
(2004). The five-factor model of personality and the psychological
health of highly identified sport fans. International Sports Journal,
8(2), 28-36.
Wann, D. L., & Ensor, C. L. (2001). The psychological health of
"joiners." Psychological Reports, 89, 122.
Wann, D. L., & Hamlet, M. A. (1994). The Joiners Scale:
Validation of a measure of social-complexity. Psychological Reports, 74,
1027-1034.
Wann, D. L., & Hamlet, M. A. (1996). Being a "joiner"
and psychological well-being. Psychological Reports, 79, 1186.
Wann, D. L., Inman, S., Ensor, C. L., Gates, R. D., & Caldwell,
D. S. (1999). Assessing the psychological well-being of sport fans using
the Profile of Mood States: The importance of team identification.
International Sports Journal, 3, 81-90.
Wann, D. L., Melnick, M. J., Russell, G. W., & Pease, D. G.
(2001). Sport fans: The psychology and social impact of spectators. New
York: Routledge Press.
Wann, D. L., & Pierce, S. (2005). The relationship between
sport team identification and social well-being: Additional evidence
supporting the Team Identification--Social Psychological Health Model.
North American Journal of Psychology, 7, 117-124.
Wann, D. L., Walker, R. G., Cygan, J., Kawase, I., & Ryan, J.
(2005). Further replication of the relationship between team
identification and psychological well-being: Examining non-classroom
settings. North American Journal of Psychology, 7, 361-366.
Wright, S. S., & Forsyth, D. R. (1997). Group membership and
collective identity: Consequences for self-esteem. Journal of Clinical
and Social Psychology, 16, 43-56.
Footnotes
(1) Of course, if the team were so unpopular as to have almost no
fans, there would be no connections to others to be gained. Although the
teams examined in the current investigation were not "marquee"
(i.e., revenue generating) sports, they did have a following.
Daniel L. Wann, Brian Keenan, and Leslie Page
Murray State University
Address Correspondence To: Daniel L. Wann, Department of
Psychology, Murray State University, Murray, KY 42071, E-mail:
dan.wann@murraystate.edu.