A test of the trans-contextual model of motivation in Greek high school pupils.
Barkoukis, Vassilis ; Hagger, Martin S.
Research evidence has documented the positive effect of regular
vigorous physical activity on the physical (Gilliam & MacConnie,
1985) and mental health (Callas & Taylor, 1994) of young people.
However, there is also considerable body of research highlighting that
young people in industrialized societies do not engage in sufficient
physical activity thought to promote health (Centers for Disease Control
and Prevention, 2003). This has led to the publication of a number of
recommendations regarding desirable levels of physical activity children
and adolescents (Sallis & Patrick, 1994). Health professionals
involved in developing campaigns and curricula to promote physical
activity in young people have sought to develop effective interventions
to increase physical activity levels in young people. The use of
existing networks such as school physical education (PE) is considered
crucial in this regard (Fox & Biddle, 1989). PE is an ideal context
in which children and adolescents can learn more about healthy
lifestyles, particularly physical activity. Campaigns promoting physical
activity need to be oriented about theoretical research to identify the
salient predictors of physical activity participation in young people
(Biddle, Gorely, & Stensel, 2004). Current research is focused on
the theoretical approaches that describe the mechanisms that influence
motivation for participation in physical activity (Hagger &
Chatzisarantis, in press; Vallerand, 2007).
An important question raised from research concerns whether factors
affecting motivation in PE could increase motives for participation in
physical activities during leisure-time (Ntoumanis, 2005; Standage,
Duda, & Ntoumanis, 2003). The present study aims to address this
question by adopting an integrated theory of motivation, known as the
trans-contextual model (Hagger, Chatzisarantis, Culverhouse, &
Biddle, 2003). According to this model, school pupils' perceptions
that their PE teacher supports their autonomy during lessons are
positively related to their motivation in a PE context but also
motivation to engage in physical activities in their leisure-time
outside of school. In order to understand this complex motivational
sequence, the trans-contextual model integrates hypotheses from three
theories of motivation; self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan,
1985), the hierarchical model of intrinsic motivation (Vallerand, 2007),
and the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1985). The subsequent
sections will outline the major theories and then illustrate how they
can provide complimentary explanations of the proposed motivational
sequence.
Self-determination Theory
The central tenet of self-determination theory is the distinction
between self-determined or autonomous and non-serf-determined or
controlled forms of motivation (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2000). The
experience of autonomous is characterized by a sense of personal origin,
self-regulation, and a sense that one's actions are fully endorsed
by the self. Important outcomes of autonomous functioning are
perceptions of interest, enjoyment, satisfaction and a sense of choice.
Behaviors that people engage in for autonomous motives are those that
satisfy the basic needs for autonomy, competence and relatedness. On the
other hand, people that act for controlled reasons engage in behavior
but feel tension and conflict with their behavior and perceptions. This
typically arises because their actions are contingent on external
reinforcers such as obtaining rewards or avoiding punishment or internal
conflicts, such as perceptions of obligation or guilt. Importantly,
autonomous forms of motivation are associated with behavioral
persistence and adaptive outcomes while controlled motives are akin to
desistance, avoidance, and maladaptive outcomes. Several reviews and
recta-analyses have illustrated that autonomous forms of motivation are
associated with increased effort, persistence, and satisfaction among
individuals engaging in tasks in physical activity contexts
(Cbatzisarantis, Hagger, Biddle, Smith, & Wang, 2003; Hagger &
Chatzisarantis, 2008).
Hierarchical Model of Intrinsic Motivation
Vallerand (2007) adopted several aspects of the self-determination
theory and developed a hierarchical model of intrinsic motivation. This
model focuses on the role of environment and motivation at different
levels of generality to describe the consequences or outcomes of
motivated behavior (Vallerand, 2007). According to the model, global
motivation lies at the apex of the hierarchy. Global motives are
general, stable, represent motivational dispositions, and reflect the
basic needs for self-determination or autonomy. Global motives are
thought to influence motivation in many different contexts in a top-down
fashion (Guay, Mageau, & Vallerand, 2003).
Motivation at the contextual level is located at an intermediate
level in the hierarchy, subordinate to global motivation, and represents
motivation in specific contexts, such as the academic, social, and
physical contexts. These motives are less stable and can be influenced
by external factors (social and environmental) as top-down effects from
global level motives. An important prediction of the model for the
contextual level is the interaction between different contexts
(Vallerand, 2007). For example, intrinsic motives in an academic context
(i.e., motivation in PE) could be transferred as intrinsic motives in a
social or sport context (i.e., leisure motivation).
Situational motivation lies below contextual motivation in the
hierarchy (Vallerand, 2007). Situational motivation refers to the type
of motivation operating in specific situations. For example, the
motivational states operating in the physical context might include
motivation to participate in activities in a PE lesson or training at a
sports club. The type of motivation experienced by individuals in each
of these situations is partly determined by their contextual
motivational orientation as well as the environmental conditions
affecting motivation in that situation. The model presumes that
contextual motives mediate the top-down influence of global-level
motives on situational motives and vice versa (Guay et al., 2003).
The Importance of Autonomy Support
According to self-determination theory and the hierarchical model,
an environment that fosters autonomous motivation in an educational
context will lead to persistence with educational tasks and behaviors in
the absence of external reinforcement (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2000;
Reeve, 2002). The mechanism behind the positive influence of autonomy
support on behavior is ascribed to the positive influence that autonomy
support has on autonomous forms of motivation (Reeve, 2002). Autonomous
motives serve to mediate the effect of autonomy support on behaviors and
are therefore an important strategy to develop autonomous reasons for
acting (Hagger et al., 2003).
The Theory of Planned Behavior
The theory of planned behavior is a social-cognitive theory thought
to adequately explain intentional behavior (Ajzen, 1985). According to
the theory, intention to perform a behavior at a specific time point is
the strongest predictor of this behavior. Intention represents the plans
of the individual to engage in a target behavior in a specific context
and time. Intention is influenced by three social-cognitive variables.
These variables are attitudes toward behavior, reflecting the
individual's positive and negative personal beliefs about the
target behavior; subjective norm; representing the social pressure that
significant others present to the actor regarding his/her performance of
the target behavior; and perceived behavioral control, which refers to
the individual's perception about his/her ability to perform the
behavior.
The basic hypotheses of the theory have been supported in a large
number of behavioral domains (Armitage & Conner, 2001), including
exercise and physical activity (Hagger, Chatzisarantis, & Biddle,
2002b). However, criticisms have been raised about the theory as it does
not account for all of the variance in intention and behavior (Conner
& Armitage, 1998), and has limited predictive utility for long-term
behaviors due to the boundary condition of stability (Chatzisarantis
& Biddle, 1998). The boundary condition stipulates that the
prediction of behavior is limited due to the inherent instability of the
situational constructs contained in the model. Such constructs are open
to change from the advent of additional information, compromising the
predictive utility of the model (Sheeran, Orbell, & Trafimow, 1999).
Researchers have suggested that this theory should be augmented to
incorporate more generalized constructs from self-determination theory
and the hierarchical model to explain the origins of its
situation-specific constructs (Chatzisarantis & Biddle, 1998; Deci
& Ryan, 1985; Hagger et al., 2003). Such theoretical modifications
have been justified by a number of authors who argue that the
decision-making process mapped by the theory of planned behavior
reflects situation-level motivation to engage in behaviors. Furthermore,
the direct antecedents of intentions, namely attitudes and perceived
behavioral control, reflect autonomous reasons for forming intentions or
plans to act (Sheeran, Norman, & Orbell, 1999). The theory of
planned behavior therefore, has potential to integrate with
Vallerand's (2007) hierarchical model as its constructs,
particularly attitudes, perceived behavioral control, and intentions,
reflect motivational orientations at the situational level.
Integration of the Theories: The Trans-Contextual Model
Self-determination theory and the theory of planned behavior may
offer complementary explanations of motivated behavior.
Self-determination theory provides explanations of the environmental and
interpersonal states that give rise to behavioral tendencies in given
contexts, Vallerand's (2007) hierarchical model suggests that
context level motivation influences motivation at the situational level,
and the theory of planned behavior represents the autonomous
decision-making process towards behavioral engagement at the situational
level. Recent evidence has supported these notions in studies of health
behavior including physical activity. For example, autonomous motives
affect attitudes, and attitudes, in turn, influence autonomous motives
on intention to perform a behavior (Hagger & Armitage, 2004; Hagger,
Chatzisarantis, & Biddle, 2002a). Hugger and colleagues (2002a)
employed this theoretical synthesis and provided empirical support for
the effect of autonomous motives on the theory of planned behavior
constructs. They found that attitudes and perceived behavioral control
mediated the effect of autonomous motives on behavioral intention and
physical activity behavior in accordance with Ajzen's (1985)
original conceptualization of the theory of planned behavior. There have
since been a number of studies adopting this integrated approach and a
meta-analysis of 12 studies has supported the integration of the theory
of planned behavior and self-determination theory (Hagger &
Chatzisarantis, 2007).
Extending these findings, Hagger et al. (2003) examined whether the
pattern of influence identified previously operated across contexts.
Specifically, the hypothesis that there could be interplay between
environmental contingencies and motivation across contexts as well
top-down from a contextual to a situational level was applied to the
question of whether motivation can be transferred across contexts. The
resulting model, known as the trans-contextual model, explicitly
illustrates how an environment perceived to be autonomy supportive can
promote autonomous motivation in a different context and whether that
motivation can be converted into intentions to act and actual behavior.
The model was tested in the PE and leisure-time contexts with physical
activity as the target behavior. According to the model, autonomy
support in PE lessons would have positive influence on the autonomous
motives in the same context (Ntoumanis, 2005; Standage et al., 2003;
Wilson & Rodgers, 2004). Furthermore, autonomy support in PE lessons
could affect autonomous motives in a non-educational
setting-leisure-time physical activity. This transference of
motivational orientations and perceived autonomy support is explicitly
stated to occur across contexts in Vallerand's (2007) hierarchical
model. There is interplay between motivational orientations across life
contexts at the contextual level as individuals use information
regarding their level of autonomy towards behaviors in related contexts
as a source of information when forming motivational orientations in
subsequent contexts.
In addition, as the top-down processes of the hierarchical model
imply, autonomous motives toward leisure-time physical activity were
hypothesized to affect motivation to act in specific situations. This
top-down process was represented by the effects of autonomous motives on
the situational variables from the theory of planned behavior,
particularly intentions. Furthermore, these motives are expected to
mediate the influence of perceived autonomy support on intention to
perform a behavior, as well as the behavior itself (Vallerand, 2007).
The preliminary investigation of the model supported the proposed
motivational sequence with no direct effects of perceived autonomy
support or autonomous motives on intention and behavior (Hagger et al.,
2003). Since then, a number of studies have supported the premises of
the trans-contextual model and a recent meta-analysis of eight tests of
the pattern of influence among the study constructs provided support for
the proposed sequence spanning from autonomy support in a PE setting to
regular physical activity in a leisure-time setting (Hagger &
Chatzisarantis, 2007). The model provides a comprehensive framework to
explain how autonomy support in an educational context is translated
into self-determined behavior in another context through its influence
on autonomous motives from self-determination theory and constructs from
the theory of planned behavior.
The Present Study
The present study aims to examine the motivational sequence
proposed by the transcontextual model in a sample of Greek high-school
pupils. Given that the trans-contextual model is a recently-developed
model of motivation, it is not known whether the proposed effects in the
model are unique to the culture in which it was developed or are
applicable across populations with different cultural backgrounds. There
is evidence to suggest that the processes highlighted by models of
motivation like the trans-contextual model may be universal and,
therefore, applicable to all adolescents in schools regardless of the
cultural values they endorse. Indeed, cross-cultural research adopting
self-determination theory and the theory of planned behavior have
supported the generalizability of the model structure and the pattern of
relationships in cultures that endorse different values. For example,
Chirkov and Ryan (2001) found that perceived autonomy support had a
significant influence on motivation and academic well-being in Russian
students, who tend to adopt a more collectivist cultural orientation. In
addition, Bagozzi, Lee, and Van Loo (2001) also found that the factor
structure and pattern of influence of the theory of planned behavior was
invariant across cultures that have predominantly individualist and
collectivist orientations. Given this evidence, and the premise put
forward by theorists that the processes mapped in motivational models
are universal, the present study aims to examine the trans-contextual
model in the Greek culture. This is a national group that endorses more
collectivist cultural values relative to the nations in which the model
was originally developed (United Kingdom) that typically endorses
individualist norms (Oyserman, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002). It is
hypothesized that the major predictions of the trans-contextual model
outlined in the previous section will be supported in this sample and
will extend the literature in this area by providing some initial
support for the generalizability of these processes across cultures.
Method
Sample
The sample of the study consisted of 183 adolescents (91 girls, 92
boys; M age = 13.93, SD = 0.78) from a co-educational high school in an
urban city of Northern Greece. According to the school director the
socioeconomic status of the adolescents could be described as middle
class. The majority of the participants were native Greek nationals,
while a small minority were immigrants from countries of the former
Soviet Union. These adolescents were second-generation immigrants with
Greek as their first language. The school director was informed on the
purpose of the study and gave permission for the data collection.
Consent from the participants' parents was also obtained via a
letter sent home with the participants outlining the study prior to data
collection. A pre-printed form was provided for parents to sign and
return to the class teacher if they did not want their child to
participate in the study. No forms were returned in any case. Children
were also informed that participation in the study was voluntary and
given the opportunity not to complete the questionnaires at each point
of data collection without prejudice. All children opted to complete the
questionnaire.
Measures
Perceived autonomy support in PE. Autonomy support in PE was
measured using an initial version of the Perceived Autonomy Support
Scale for Exercise Settings (PASSES; Hagger et al., 2007). The measure
asks respondents to rate the extent that significant authoritative
others support their autonomy in a given context. For the present study
the authoritative others were specified as the participants' PE
teachers. The scale has 15 items (e.g., "I feel that my PE teacher
makes sure I really understand the goals of the lesson and what I need
to do"). Responses were given on seven-point Likert scales ranging
from 1 ("strongly disagree") to 7 ("strongly
agree").
Autonomous motivation in PE and leisure-time. A modified version of
Ryan and Connell's (1989) perceived locus of causality (PLOC) scale
was used to measure autonomous motivation in PE. The scale measures four
types of motivation (1), each varying in the degree of autonomy on a
continuum ranging from high to low autonomy. Participants were presented
with a common stem: "Why do you participate in PE?", followed
by eight reasons, two from each regulation style from the perceived
locus of causality: intrinsic motivation (e.g., "because PE is
fun"), identified regulation (e.g., "because I value
PE"), introjected regulation (e.g., "because I will feel
ashamed if I do not"), and extrinsic regulation (e.g.,
"because important others want me to do it"). Responses were
measured on four-point Likert-typo scales ranging from 1 ("not true
at all") to 4 ("very true"). The construct and
discriminant validity of the PLOC measure in a PE context has been
reported in previous studies (Hagger et al., 2003).
Autonomous motivation in leisure-time physical activity was
measured using the Behavioral Regulations in Exercise Questionnaire
(BREQ, Mullan, Markland, & Ingledew, 1997). Participants were asked
"Why do you participate in active sports and/or vigorous physical
activities in your spare time?" followed by three reasons from each
regulation style. The items measuring intrinsic motivation (e.g.,
"I exercise because it is fun"), identified regulation (e.g.,
"I exercise because it is important to make the effort"),
introjected regulation (e.g., "I exercise because I will feel
guilty if I do not"), and external regulation (e.g., "I
exercise because others say I should") were assessed on seven-point
Likert-type scales ranging from 1 ("not true at all") to 7
("very true").
The theory of planned behavior questionnaire. Constructs from the
theory of planned behavior questionnaire were measured using
standardized items developed according to published guidelines (Ajzen,
2003). Three items measured behavioral intentions (e.g., "I intend
to do active sports and/or vigorous physical activities in the next 5
weeks ...") on seven-point Likert scales anchored by 1
("strongly agree") and 7 ("strongly disagree").
Attitudes were assessed in response to the following question:
"Participating in active sports and/or vigorous physical activities
during my leisure-time in the next five weeks is ..." Responses
were measured on four seven-point semantic differential scales with the
following bipolar adjectives: bad-good, harmful-beneficial,
unenjoyable-enjoyable, and useful-useless. Subjective norms were
measured by two items (e.g., "People important to me think that I
should do active sports and/or vigorous physical activities during my
leisure-time in the next 5 weeks") on seven-point scales with 1
("strongly disagree") and 7 ("strongly agree") as
endpoints. Perceived behavioral control was assessed through three items
(e.g., "I feel in complete control over whether I do active sports
and/or vigorous physical activities in my leisure-time in the next 5
weeks") measured on seven-point Likert scales ranging from 1
("no control") to 7 ("complete control").
Self-reported physical activity behavior. Physical activity
behavior was assessed by an adapted version of Godin and Shephard's
(1985) Leisure-Time Exercise Questionnaire (LTEQ). Participants were
initially presented with the definition of vigorous physical activity
given by Godin and Shephard. Vigorous physical activity was defined as
all active sports and physical activities, but not low-level activities
or walking to school. Participants were requested to consider, on
average, how many times per week they engaged in vigorous physical
activity over the previous five weeks, recorded on a single seven-point
scale. Independent evaluations of the LTEQ have found it to be valid,
reliable, and easy to administer. It displays satisfactory concurrent
validity with gold-standard measures of physical activity such as
heart-rate monitoring (Jacobs, Ainsworth, Hartman, & Leon, 1993) and
this version has been found effective in estimating physical activity
levels in studies using social-cognitive models (Hagger et al., 2003;
Hagger, Chatzisarantis, & Harris, 2006).
Cultural orientation. A sub-sample of the participants recruited at
the first wave of data collection (N = 118) completed an abbreviated
version of Triandis' (1995) individualism-collectivism scale (2).
This nine-item measure was used to provide an estimate of the general
cultural orientation of the sub-sample. The scale comprises four items
measuring individualism (e.g., "I would rather make an important
decision by myself than discuss it with my friends") and five items
measuring collectivism (e.g., "I can count on my relatives for help
if I find myself in any kind of trouble"). Responses were given on
seven-point Likert scales with 1 ("not at all") and 7
("very much") as endpoints.
Translation of the Measures
A back translation technique was used to translate the measures of
the study into the Greek language (Brislin, 1986). In the first
instance, the study measures were translated from English to Greek by a
proficient translator. Two other proficient translators then translated
the Greek version back into English. These versions were then compared
with the original and the appropriate corrections were made to the Greek
version. Next, this modified version was back-translated again into
English and compared to the original version for further inconsistencies
and biases. This procedure was repeated until there were no
inconsistencies between the translated and original versions.
Research Design
A three-wave prospective design was implemented to allay common
method variance and for the prospective prediction of behavior in
accordance with the theory of planned behavior. At the first wave of
data collection, the autonomy support in PE (Deci & Ryan, 2003) and
autonomous motivation in PE (Ryan & Connell, 1989) measures were
administered. One week later at a second wave of data collection, the
measures of the theory of planned behavior components (Ajzen, 1985) and
autonomous motivation during leisure-time (Mullah et al., 1997) were
administered to the participants. The one-week delay ensured that the
measures of autonomous motivation in the two contexts were not
contaminated by common method variance associated with introducing
measures with similar themes simultaneously. The third wave took place
five weeks later and involved the administration of the self-report
measure of physical activity behavior. A five-week period was used
because it permits long-range prediction, a desirable outcome in
motivational models (Hagger et al., 2003).
Researchers conducted the data collection in quiet classroom
conditions. Participants were informed that they would be asked to
complete a series of short questionnaires over the coming weeks as part
of a survey on young people. Participants were separated so that they
could not copy or discuss responses. All of the questionnaires were
completed anonymously to preserve confidentiality. Measures across
time-points were matched using birth date and gender.
Results
Participants
Attrition of participants across the three waves due to absences
and incomplete questionnaires resulted in a final sample size of 93 high
school pupils across the three waves (57 girls, 36 boys; Mage = 13.99,
SD = 0.80). Scores on the Triandis (1995) abbreviated
individualism-collectivism scale indicated that participants'
average score on the collectivism scale was significantly higher than
their score on the individualism scale (t(117) = 2.19, p < .05). This
is consistent with cross-cultural data indicating that Greek nationals
tend to favor a collectivist orientation relative to other Western
European nations (Oyserman et al., 2002). This is in stark contrast to
the predominantly individualist cultural orientation prevalent in the
British culture where the trans-contextual model was developed (Hagger
et al., 2003).
Preliminary Analysis
In order to maximize parsimony in the hypothesised model and to
minimize the number of constructs in subsequent analyses, we calculated
a relative autonomy index (RAI) from the PLOC components in both
contexts, as recommended by other researchers (Guay et al., 2003).
Weights were assigned to each participant's intrinsic motivation
(+2), identified regulation (+1), introjected regulation (-1), and
extrinsic regulation (-2) score, and the RAI was the composite of these
weighted scores. The RAI therefore reflected participants' relative
level of autonomous motivation in the PE and leisure-time physical
activity contexts.
Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations among the
perceived autonomy support, RAI composites in both contexts, the theory
of planned behavior variables, and physical activity behavior are given
in Table 1. Correlations between perceived autonomy support and the RAI
composites in both contexts were significant and positive. Perceived
autonomy support was also significantly correlated with the theory of
planned behavior constructs, with the exception of the subjective norms
construct. The RAI for the leisure-time context was also significantly
related to the theory of planned behavior variables and physical
activity behavior, while the RAI for the PE context was not related to
any of the theory of planned behavior constructs or physical activity
behavior. Finally, measures displayed satisfactory levels of internal
consistency, with the exception of the subjective norms construct.
However, previous research has confirmed that measures of this construct
generally do not achieve satisfactory reliability despite adhering to
recommended guidelines for their development (Ajzen, 2003; Hagger,
Chatzisarantis, & Biddle, 2001). Therefore, the results pertaining subjective norms should be interpreted with caution.
Main Analysis
Study hypotheses were examined by path analysis using manifest
rather than latent variables and a simultaneous process with the EQS v.
6.0 computer program. Model fit was evaluated by means of the
goodness-of-fit Chi-square, which should be non-significant for a
well-fitting model, and other relative indexes of good fit, namely the
comparative fit index (CFI), which should exceed .95, and the root mean
square error of approximation (RMSEA) which should be less than .08 (Hu
& Bentler, 1999). In the model, the hypothesized pattern of
relationships among the trans-contextual model constructs proposed by
Hagger et al. (2002a) was tested by including unidirectional free
parameters (paths) mapping out the motivational sequence.
In the specified model, perceived autonomy support in PE was made
to predict autonomous motives in a leisure-time physical activity
context, as captured by the RAI, directly and indirectly via the
mediation of autonomous motives in a PE context. In addition, the model
proposed that autonomous motives in a PE context would predict attitudes
and perceived behavioral control via the mediation of autonomous motives
in a leisure-time context. In addition, paths from autonomous motives in
a leisure time context to physical activity intentions via the mediation
of attitudes and perceived behavioral control in a leisure-time context
were included. There was also an indirect path from autonomous motives
in a leisure-time context to physical activity intentions mediated by
subjective norms. Finally, attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived
behavioral control were made to predict physical activity behavior
through the mediation of intentions.
Examination of the fit indexes revealed that the hypothesized model
reproduced the observed covariance matrix satisfactorily ([chi square] =
6.71, df = 5; p = .24; CFI = .99; RMSEA = .06). The standardized path
coefficients for the final model are given in Figure 1. Turning first to
the effects of perceived autonomy support in PE in the model, as
expected, perceived autonomy support predicted autonomous motives in a
PE context ([beta] = .24, p < .01). In addition, there was also a
direct effect of perceived autonomy support on autonomous motives in a
leisure-time context ([beta] = .39, p < .01). Furthermore, the
anticipated effect of autonomous motives in a PE context on autonomous
motives was non-significant. So autonomous motives in PE did not mediate
the effect of perceived autonomy support in a leisure-time context, as
hypothesized. Furthermore, the non-significant direct path between the
autonomous motivation constructs across contexts indicated that
autonomous motivation in leisure time could not mediate the effect of
autonomous motivation in a PE context on attitudes, subjective norms,
and perceived behavioral control. There were no other direct effects of
perceived autonomy support or autonomous motives in PE in the model.
Next we focus on the effects of autonomous motives in leisure-time
on the theory of planned behavior constructs. Autonomous motives in
leisure-time significantly influenced attitudes ([beta] = .54, p <
.01), subjective norms ([beta] = .28, p < .01), and perceived
behavioral control ([beta] = .53, p < .01) as hypothesized. There
were no direct effects of autonomous motives on intention, which is
congruent with previous tests of the trans-contextual model (Hagger et
al., 2003) and the effects of self-determined motives on the theory of
planned behavior constructs (Hagger et al., 2002a; Wilson & Rodgers,
2004). However, there was a significant and positive zero-order
correlation between the autonomous motives construct and intentions (r =
.45, p < .01; see Table 1). This suggests that the attitude,
subjective norm, and perceived behavioral control constructs mediated
the impact of autonomous motives on intentions.
In order to test this premise formally, we adopted the criteria of
Baron and Kenny 0986). There are four criteria that require satisfaction
in order for mediation to be supported: (i) the dependent variable (in
this case, intention) should be correlated with the independent or
predictor variable (autonomous motives in leisure-time); (ii) the
mediator(s) (attitude/subjective norms/perceived behavioral control)
should be correlated with the independent variable; (iii) the
mediator(s) should have a significant unique effect on the dependent
variable when it is included alongside the independent variable in a
multivariate test of these relationships; and (iv) the effect of
independent variable on the dependent should be significantly attenuated or nullified when the mediator(s) is/are included as an independent
predictor(s) of the dependent variable (Baron & Kenny, 1986). In the
present study, intention was significantly correlated with the
autonomous motives construct in a leisure time context and the attitude,
subjective norm, and perceived behavioral control constructs were also
significantly correlated with autonomous motives, supporting the first
two criteria for mediation. Further, attitude and perceived behavioral
control had significant unique effects on intention, but subjective
norms did not. Therefore subjective norms had to be rejected as a
potential mediator, but the attitude and perceived behavioral control
constructs satisfied the third criterion for mediation. Finally,
removing the direct effect of attitude and perceived behavioral control
on intention independently in the path model resulted in an increase in
the direct effect of autonomous motives on intention in both cases,
satisfying the final criterion for mediation. For completion, a Sobel
(1982) test indicated that the indirect effect of autonomous motives on
intention via attitudes and perceived behavioral control was significant
([beta] = .40, p < .01).
Turning next to the relationships between the theory of planned
behavior variables, results showed that attitudes ([beta] = .40, p <
.01) and perceived behavioral control ([beta] = .35, p < .01) were
significant predictors of intention, as predicted. However, subjective
norms did not significantly predict intentions, so this hypothesis had
to be rejected. Further, intention significantly predicted leisure-time
physical activity behavior. There were no direct effects of attitude or
perceived behavioral control on behavior. It seemed that intentions
mediated the effects of attitude and perceived behavioral control on
behavior. A formal test of this was conducted using Baron and
Kenny's (1986) criteria for mediation. From Table 1 it can be seen
that attitude and perceived behavioral control are significantly
correlated with leisure-time physical activity behavior and attitude and
perceived behavioral control are significantly correlated with
intention. These relationships confirmed the first two criteria for
mediation. In order to confirm the final two criteria, we released free
parameters in an alternative model between attitude and behavior and
perceived behavioral control and behavior. Despite the inclusion of
these paths, the intention construct remained the only significant
predictor of behavior in the model, satisfying the third criterion for
mediation. Next, fixing the intention-behavior relationship to be zero
and re-estimating the model resulted in a restoration of the significant
direct effects of attitude and perceived behavioral control on
intention. Finally, Sobel tests confirmed the significant indirect
effects of attitude ([beta] = .32, p < .01) and perceived behavioral
control ([beta] = .28, p < .01) on behavior. Thus it can be concluded
that, in accordance with Ajzen's (1985) hypothesis, intention
mediated the influence of attitude and perceived behavioral control on
leisure-time physical activity behavior.
Finally, it is important to examine the overall effects of the
distal autonomy support and autonomous motivation constructs on
intentions and behavior. Although there were no direct effects, there
was a significant total effect of perceived autonomy support in PE on
intentions ([beta] = .23, p < .01) and leisure-time physical activity
behavior ([beta] = .23,p < .01) in the model. In addition, there was
a significant total effect of autonomous motives in a leisure-time
physical activity context on intentions ([beta] = .45, p < .01) and
behavior ([beta] =. 19, p < .01). Overall, the model accounted for
22.21% of the variance in intentions and 44.76% of the variance in
leisure-time physical activity behavior.
Discussion
The present study tested a trans-contextual model of motivation
(Hagger et al., 2003; Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2007) in Greek high
school children. The model integrated three theories of motivation and a
motivational sequence was proposed in which perceived autonomy support
in a PE context would influence autonomous motivation, intentions, and
behavior in a leisure-time context. The motivational sequence was
generally supported in this sample of Greek children, with perceived
autonomy support in PE related to autonomous motivation in a leisure
time context. Autonomous motives in a leisure time context predicted
intentions, but only via the mediation of attitudes and perceived
behavioral control towards doing leisure-time physical activity.
Intentions predicted behavior and mediated the influence of attitudes
and perceived behavioral control in leisure-time physical activity
behavior. Only two hypothesized effects based on theory and previous
fundings with the trans-contextual model were not significant; the
effect of autonomous motives in PE on autonomous motives in leisure-time
and the effect of subjective norms on intention. The proposed
motivational sequence was therefore supported to a great extent and the
model explained a substantial proportion of the variance in leisure-time
physical activity behavior.
Results from the present study support the use of the
trans-contextual model in a different cultural group. One of the key
premises of self-determination theory is that the autonomous drive to
pursue behaviors that satisfy psychological needs are universal and
salient to all cultures, regardless of their orientation on cultural
values such as individualism and collectivism (Chirkov & Ryan,
2001). Results indicate that the trans-contextual model exhibits
satisfactory goodness-of-fit with the present data from a sample of
Greek students. The pattern of results in this test of the
trans-contextual model in a cultural group that tends to endorse more
collectivist cultural values, as illustrated by the abbreviated Triandis
(1995) individualism-collectivism scale, compares favorably with that
from a British culture (Hagger et al., 2003). These data suggest that
the processes identified by Hagger et al. (2003) might also exhibit
consistency across cultures and corroborated findings from the
meta-analysis of the trans contextual model (Hagger &
Chatzisarantis, 2007). Further cross-cultural replications and
comparisons of the model are required to corroborate current findings
(Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2007).
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
An important funding of the present study was that perceived
autonomy support in a PE context had a significant effect on
leisure-time physical activity behavior, but only via a motivational
sequence that included contextual-level motivation in a leisure-time
context and the theory of planned behavior constructs. Therefore,
adolescents in a PE context that perceive their teachers to provide
opportunities and choice for their activities and help them to set
personal goals are those more likely to report higher motivation to
participate in physical activities outside of school. In turn, they are
also more likely to form intentions to engage in future physical
activity behavior, and engage in actual physical activity behavior.
This motivational sequence provides some salient information about
the processes that underpin motivation and behavior from an educational
context to an extra-curricular context. It supports Deci and Ryan's
(1985, 2000) and Reeve's (2002) proposals that autonomy support is
influential in fostering intrinsic motivation. However, this model is
pioneering in that it supports this link across different contexts. The
model also supports Hagger and Chatzisarantis' (2007) and Wilson
and Rodgers' (2004) proposals that autonomous motives predict
intentions, which, in turn, predict behavior. This suggests that
autonomous motives compel people to form intentions to participate in
future physical activity, as outlined by the theory of planned behavior.
It also corroborates Vallerand's (2007) proposal that the effects
of distal, context-level autonomous motives toward physical activity
predict motivational constructs (i.e., intentions) at the proximal,
situational-level.
While the pattern of prediction and proposed motivational sequence
in the trans-contextual model was almost identical to the model reported
by Hagger et al. (2003), the proposed mediation of the effect of
perceived autonomy support in a PE context on autonomous motivation in a
leisure-time context by autonomous motivation in PE was not supported.
In fact, while perceived autonomy support directly influenced autonomous
motives in both contexts, autonomous motives in PE had no effects on any
of the variables in the model. One possible reason for this is that the
nature of autonomous motives in PE has no direct bearing on autonomous
motives in leisure-time in this sample of adolescents from a Greek high
school. To speculate, a possible reason for this may be the general lack
of endorsement for autonomous motives in school PE relative to
autonomous motives in a leisure time contexts. Therefore, while
perceived autonomy support seemed to explain variance in the level of
autonomous motivation in both contexts, autonomous motives in PE has no
motivational impact outside of that context.
The significant direct effect of perceived autonomy support on
leisure-time autonomous motives unmediated by autonomous motives in the
PE context may also imply that the link between the two contexts (i.e.,
PE and leisure) is initiated before the formation of motivation in PE.
For example, the satisfaction of the basic psychological needs in an
autonomy supportive environment may lead to the formation of autonomous
motives for leisure, overriding the PE motives. Or the satisfaction of
the needs in PE may influence the needs in leisure activities. So far,
there is some preliminary evidence regarding the top-down influence of
psychological needs on autonomous forms of motivation in leisure-time
contexts (Hagger et al., 2006), but this has not been corroborated in PE
or trans-contextual studies.
Conclusion, Recommendations for Future Research and Limitations of
the Study
The present study provided evidence to support the premises of
Hagger et al.'s (2003) trans-contextual model in a sample of
adolescents from a Greek high school. Importantly, the motivational
sequence in which perceived autonomy support in PE influences autonomous
motives, the decision-making constructs from the theory of planned
behavior, and physical activity behavior in a leisure-time context was
supported. This suggests that the fostering of autonomous motives in PE
may contribute young people's decisions to participate in physical
activity outside of school.
The study is not without its limitations, however. It is important
to note that some of the constructs such as subjective norms did not
exhibit sound psychometric integrity given the low Cronbach alpha
levels. While these have been reported in the same measures adopted
elsewhere, it does limit the inferences made using this construct in the
present study. In addition, the final sample size was relatively small.
Given the complex and time-demanding prospective design of the study,
such attrition over three waves is not surprising and is a feature of
such research. However, we also conducted a check using a bootstrap procedure (Efron, 1982) to support the robustness of the findings
presented here. The bootstrap resampling plan generates simulated
samples based on the original data and tests the hypothesized model
against these additional data sets. Such a plan has been used by
previous researchers using complex prospective designs to provide
additional support for their findings (Standage et al., 2003). Across
many replications (N = 999), the final model from the present study
exhibited robust goodness-of-fit statistics with an acceptable range (M
CFI = .95; 95% Confidence Interval Lower Bound = .91; 95% Confidence
Interval Upper Bound = 1.00). This analysis provides strong evidence
that the present model represents a robust description of the data in
this sample.
Present results have some important practical implications for PE
teachers and those devising school curricula. Such practitioners would
do well to adopt the autonomy-supportive behaviors identified by Reeve
(2002) to foster autonomy in PE. These include adopting a questioning
approach, providing a choice of activity, acknowledging conflict in
decision-making, setting personally-relevant goals, and providing a
clear rationale for the activity. These results also advance knowledge
in the field by replicating the trans-contextual model in a culture that
reports much higher collectivist values compared with the culture in
which the model was originally developed. It seems the pattern of
effects in the model may be universal as indicated by this and other
cross-cultural studies using the trans-contextual model (e.g., Hagger
& Chatzisarantis, 2007). However, future studies may focus on
establishing the generalizability of the model further by testing its
hypotheses in more diverse cultures, such as those from Asia, which are
typically considered to be predominantly collectivist in their cultural
orientation. Additionally, while the present study indicated that
autonomy support from one important source, that of PE teachers, seems
to affect motivation and physical activity behavior in a leisure time
context, the present study did not investigate autonomy support from
other salient sources such as parents and peers. Future studies will
need to account for these sources of autonomy support to provide a more
complete model of motivation. Finally, future studies should examine the
role of basic psychological needs in the formation of autonomous motives
across different contexts (see Hagger et al., 2006).
Authors' Note
The present study was part of a larger study into the
cross-cultural applicability of the trans-contextual model
References
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned
behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Action-control: From
cognition to behavior (pp. 11-39). Heidelberg: Springer.
Ajzen, I. (2003). Constructing a TPB questionnaire: Conceptual and
methodological considerations. Retrieved April 14, 2003, from University
of Massachusetts, Department of Psychology Web site:
http://www-unix.oit.umass.edu/~aizen: University of Massachusetts.
Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of
planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social
Psychology, 40, 471-499.
Bagozzi, R. P., Lee, H. M., & Van Loo, M. F. (2001). Decisions
to donate bone marrow: The role of attitudes and subjective norms across
cultures. Psychology and Health, 16, 29-56.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator
variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual,
strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.
Biddle, S. J. H., Gorely, T., & Stensel, D. J. (2004).
Health-enhancing physical activity and sedentary behaviour in children
and adolescents. Journal of Sport Sciences, 22, 679-701.
Brislin, R. W. (1986). The wording and translation of research
instruments. In W. J. Lonner & J. W. Berry (Eds.), Field methods in
educational research (pp. 137-164). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Calfas, C. J., & Taylor, W. C. (1994). Effects of physical
activity on psychological variables of children. Pediatric Exercise
Science, 6, 406-423.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2003). Physical
activity levels among children aged 9-13 years--United States, 2002.
Mortality and Morbidity Weekly, 52, 785-788.
Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Biddle, S. J. H. (1998). Functional
significance of psychological variables that are included in the theory
of planned behaviour: A self-determination theory approach to the study
of attitudes, subjective norms, perceptions of control and intentions.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 303-322.
Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Hagger, M. S., Biddle, S. J. H., Smith,
B., & Wang, J. C. K. (2003). A meta-analysis of perceived locus of
causality in exercise, sport, and physical education contexts. Journal
of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 25, 284-306.
Chirkov, V. I., & Ryan, R. M. (2001). Parent and teacher
autonomy support in Russian and U.S. adolescents: Common effects on
well-being and academic motivation. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 32, 618-635.
Conner, M., & Armitage, C. J. (1998). Extending the Theory of
Planned Behavior: A review and avenues for further research. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 28, 14291464.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and
self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The "What" and
"Why" of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination
of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11,227-268.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The Sport Climate
Questionnaire. Retrieved February 20, 2003, from Rochester University,
Self-Determination Theory Faculty web site:
http://www.psych.rochester.edu/SDT/.
Efron, B. (1982). The jackknife, bootstrap and other resampling
plans. Philadelphia: SIAM.
Fox, K. R., & Biddle, S. (1989). The child's perspective
in P.E. Part IV: Psychology and professional issues. British Journal of
Physical Education, 20, 35-38.
Gilliam, T. B., & MacConnie, S. E. (1985). Coronary heart
disease risk in children and their physical activity patterns. In R. A.
Boileau (Ed.), Advances in Pediatric Sports Sciences (pp. 171-187).
Champaign, II.: Human Kinetics.
Godin, G., & Shephard, R. J. (1985). A simple method to assess
exercise behavior in the community. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport
Science, 10, 141-146.
Guay, F., Mageau, G. A., & Vallerand, R. J. (2003). On the
hierarchical structure of self-determined motivation: A test of
top-down, bottom-up, reciprocal, and horizontal effects. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 992-1004.
Hagger, M. S., & Armitage, C. (2004). The influence of
perceived loci of control and causality in the theory of planned
behavior in a leisure-time exercise context. Journal of Applied
Biobehavioral Research, 9, 45-64.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N., & Biddle, S. J. H. (2001).
The influence of self-efficacy and past behaviour on the physical
activity intentions of young people. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19,
711-725.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N., & Biddle, S. J. H. (2002a).
The influence of autonomous and controlling motives on physical activity
intentions within the Theory of Planned Behaviour. British Journal of
Health Psychology, 7, 283-297.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N., & Biddle, S. J. H. (2002b).
A meta-analytic review of the theories of reasoned action and planned
behavior in physical activity: Predictive validity and the contribution
of additional variables. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24,
3-32.
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2007). The
trans-contextual model of motivation. In M. S. Hagger & N. L. D.
Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic Motivation and Self-determination in
Exercise and Sport (pp. 53-70). Champaign, Il: Human Kinetics.
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2008).
Self-determination theory and the psychology of exercise. International
Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1, 79-103.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Culverhouse, T., &
Biddle, S. J. H. (2003). The processes by which perceived autonomy
support in physical education promotes leisure-time physical activity
intentions and behavior: A trans-contextual model. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 95, 784--795.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Harris, J. (2006).
From psychological need satisfaction to intentional behavior: Testing a
motivational sequence in two behavioral contexts. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 32, 131-138.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit
indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus
new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55.
Jacobs, D. R. J., Ainsworth, B. E., Hartman, T. J., & Leon, A.
S. (1993). A simultaneous evaluation of 10 commonly used physical
activity questionnaires. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise,
25, 92-98.
Mullah, E., Markland, D., & Ingledew, D. K. (1997). A graded
conceptualisation of self-determination in the regulation of exercise
behaviour: Development of a measure using confirmatory factor analysis.
Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 745-752.
Ntoumanis, N. (2005). A prospective study of participation in
optional school physical education based on self-determination theory.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 444-453.
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking
individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions
and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3-72.
Reeve, J. (2002). Self-determination theory applied to educational
settings. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of
self-determination research (pp. 183-203). Rochester, NY: University of
Rochester Press.
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of
causality and internalization: Examining reasons for acting in two
domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 749-761.
Sallis, J. F., & Patrick, K. (1994). Physical activity
guidelines for adolescents: Consensus statement. Pediatric Exercise
Science, 6, 302-314.
Sheeran, P., Norman, P., & Orbell, S. (1999). Evidence that
intentions based on attitudes better predict behaviour than intentions
based on subjective norms. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29,
403-406.
Sheeran, P., Orbell, S., & Trafimow, D. (1999). Does the
temporal stability of behavioral intentions moderate intention-behavior
and past behavior-future behavior relations? Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 25, 721-730.
Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect
effects in structural equation models. In S. Leinhardt (Ed.),
Sociological Methodology (pp. 290-312). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Standage, M., Duda, J. L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2003). A model of
contextual motivation in physical education: Using constructs from
self-determination and achievement goal theories to predict physical
activity intentions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 97-110.
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder,
CO: Westview Press.
Vallerand, R. J. (2007). A hierarchical model of intrinsic and
extrinsic motivation for sport and physical activity. In M. S. Hagger
& N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic Motivation and
Self-Determination in Exercise and Sport (pp. 255-279). Champaign, I1:
Human Kinetics.
Wilson, P. M., & Rodgers, W. M. (2004). The relationship
between perceived autonomy support, exercise regulations and behavioral
intentions in women. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 5, 229-242.
Vassilis Barkoukis
Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece
Martin S. Hagger
University of Nottingham, UK
(1) According to Vallerand (2007) a further construct is located
beyond the external extreme on the PLOC continuum, amotivation, which
reflects the absence of motivation.
(2) Time constraints meant that only a sub-sample of participants
were able to complete the Triandis measure.
Address Correspondence To: Vassilis Barkoukis, Department of
Physical Education and Sport Science, Aristotle University of
Thessaloniki, 54124 Thessaloniki, Greece, Email: bark@phed.auth.gr.
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics and Intercorrelations Among the
Trans-Contextual Model Components
Variable Mean SD [alpha]
1. Perceived Autonomy Support (PE) 4.91 0.82 .82
2. Relative Autonomy Index (PE) 12.36 2.79 .67 (a)
3. Relative Autonomy Index (LT) 19.60 4.30 .65 (a)
4. Attitudes 5.54 1.39 .92
5. Subjective Norm 4.23 1.48 .48
6. Perceived Behavioral Control 5.10 1.27 .69
7. Intention 4.31 1.61 .73
8. Leisure Time Exercise Behavior 3.33 1.68 --
Variable 1 2 3
1. Perceived Autonomy Support (PE) --
2. Relative Autonomy Index (PE) .24 * --
3. Relative Autonomy Index (LT) .42 ** .24 * --
4. Attitudes .38 ** .17 .60 **
5. Subjective Norm .19 .13 .31 **
6. Perceived Behavioral Control .31 * .11 .56 **
7. Intention .23 ** .02 .45 **
8. Leisure Time Exercise Behavior .23 * -.01 .41 **
Variable 4 5 6 7
1. Perceived Autonomy Support (PE)
2. Relative Autonomy Index (PE)
3. Relative Autonomy Index (LT)
4. Attitudes --
5. Subjective Norm .22 * --
6. Perceived Behavioral Control .55 ** .29 ** --
7. Intention .60 ** .18 .58 ** --
8. Leisure Time Exercise Behavior .41 ** .19 .39 ** .45 **
Note. PE = PE Context; LT = Leisure Time Context (a) Coefficient
represents the median alpha coefficient for the intrinsic motivation,
identified regulation, introjected regulation, and external regulation
constructs that form the relative autonomy index composite
* p < .05. ** p < .01.