Reconceptualizing indicants of sport perfectionism as a function of gender.
Anshel, Mark H. ; Kim, Jwa K. ; Henry, Ruth 等
Frost, Marten, Lahart, and Rosenblate (1990) define perfectionism
as "the setting of excessively high standards of performance in
conjunction with a tendency to make overly critical
self-evaluations" (p. 450). Perfectionism is considered a stable
pattern of thinking and behavior that changes relatively little over
time, as opposed to a measure of a person's state at any particular
moment (Anshel & Eom, 2002; Antony & Swinson, 1998).
Perfectionism may be viewed as both desirable (adaptive) and undesirable
(maladaptive, or neurotic). According to Enns and Cox (2002) and Flett
and Hewitt (2005), adaptive perfectionism reflects setting reasonably
challenging and achievable goals, high personal standards, a strong
desire to excel, self-forgiving of one's own mistakes, modifying
standards in accordance with situational factors, striving for success
in achievement settings, and enhanced levels of motivation. Maladaptive
perfectionism, on the other hand, describes the tendency to set
excessively high standards, be overly self-critical, and be extensively
influenced by the approval of significant others, such as parents,
teachers, and sports coaches. Individuals who are characterized as
maladaptive perfectionists tend to exhibit exaggerated expectations,
have a high fear of failure, and are less capable of coping with
challenging situations. Perhaps not surprisingly, then, perfectionism
has been traditionally perceived as negative, also referred to as
undesirable, abnormal, neurotic, maladaptive or dysfunctional (Anshel
& Mansouri, 2005; Blatt, 1995; Hamachek, 1978; Suddarth &
Slaney, 2001; Terry-Short, Owens, & Slade, 1995). Maladaptive
perfectionism "is instrumental in a variety of psychopathologies
and adjustment problems" (Flett, Sawatzsy, & Hewitt, 1995).
Some of these problems have implications for competitive sport.
The study of perfectionism in competitive sport has gained
increased popularity by researchers in recent years (e.g., Flett &
Hewitt, 2005; Stoeber & Eismann, 2007). These authors contend that
perfectionism can best be understood, measured, and interpreted for
clinical practice only when we understand the implications for
conceptualizing perfectionism in both positive and negative forms. Flett
and Hewitt view perfectionism as "primarily a negative factor that
contributes to maladaptive outcomes among athletes and exercisers"
(p. 14). Stoeber and Otto (2006) and Stober and Eismarm (2007) refer to
positive and negative dimensions of perfectionism in sport as
perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic concerns, respectively.
Perfectionistic strivings comprise characteristics of perfectionism
described as normal, healthy, or adaptive (e.g., setting high personal
standards, striving for perfection). Perfectionistic concerns, on the
other hand, reflect the negative, maladaptive, or neurotic components of
this construct (e.g., concern over mistakes, doubts about actions,
overly critical evaluations of one's behavior, negative reactions
to imperfections).
While numerous sport perfectionism studies in recent years have
been concerned with identifying components of perfectionism, examining
the adaptive and maladaptive dimensions, or perfectionistic strivings
and perfectionistic concerns, of this construct, has received limited
attention. For example, in a study of university level (elite) Canadian
athletes, failure to meet personal standards, regardless of extreme
expectations of both self and others, markedly contributed to increased
frustration and anger (Dunn, Gotwals, & Cosgrove Dunn, 2005). The
researchers speculated that fear, frustration and anxiety may lead to an
increase in critical self-evaluations and lower self-esteem. Along these
lines, Gotwals, Dunn, and Wayment (2003) found that low self-esteem
among athletes was related to maladaptive perfectionism, specifically
the dimensions concern over mistakes, parental criticism, and doubts
about actions. Self-criticism as a perfectionism sub-dimension was not
measured.
There are several unique attributes of skilled athletes that
directly relate to perfectionism in both positive and negative forms.
With respect to personal standards and self-criticism, for example, it
is well known that athletes develop challenging performance standards
and goals, and become self-critical when they do not meet those (often
lofty) goals (Anshel, 2003). However, when goals are prescribed by
others, or are set primarily to acquire approval from others, the
motivation to achieve is markedly reduced, and this may lead to
performance failure (Campbell & Di Paula, 2002).
In their sport perfectionism study, Anshel and Mansouri (2005)
asserted that individuals in a competitive environment have high
personal expectations, and that sport performance will more likely
reflect those expectations. Although typically classified as adaptive in
nature, the maladaptive effects of high personal standards may be
intensified when the level of ability is incongruent with the projected
goals (Flea & Hewitt, 2005; Hatzigeorgiadis, 2006). Negative
(maladaptive) perfectionists may project unreachable standards, in which
continued striving results in performance failure, feelings of
inadequacy, and the loss of motivation and confidence in one's
ability to succeed. These characteristics may be inherent in competitive
sport.
Researchers have focused on the negative, or maladaptive,
properties of perfectionism in competitive sport (e.g., Flett &
Hewitt, 2005; Haase & Prapavessis, 2004; Ommundsen, Roberts, Lemyre,
& Miller, 2005), and to a far lesser extent, in exercise settings
(Anshel & Seipel, 2006). For example, Ommundsen et al. (2005) found
that the combination of motivational climate, achievement goals, and
perfectionism was significantly related to peer acceptance and quality
of friendships among male and female soccer players. They speculated
that unrealistic and excessive parental criticism and parental
expectations might impel adolescent players to develop maladaptive
perfectionistic tendencies. More ominously, Haase and Prapavessis (2004)
and Hopkinson and Lock (2004) each found that perfectionism was
significantly related to disordered eating among female athletes and
dancers, respectively. Others (e.g., Flett & Hewitt, 2005; Frost
& Henderson, 1991; Gotwals et al., 2003; Hall, Kerr, & Matthews,
1998) have concluded that perfectionism in sport is associated with
severe anxiety among athletes, ostensibly due to excessive personal
standards and the high expectations of the athletes' parents and
coaches. In a study of Canadian ice hockey players, Vallance, Dunn, and
Causgrove Dunn (2006) found that highly perfectionistic athletes
anticipated significantly higher levels of anger following personal
mistakes than low and moderately perfectionistic athletes. Hinton and
Kubas (2005) found that perfectionism is more prevalent among athletes
than their non-athlete peers. This suggests that either sports
participation or level of competition may foster perfectionistic
thinking among competitive athletes, or that individuals with
perfectionistic tendencies are more likely to become higher skilled
athletes. Taken together, several sport studies have examined the
negative manifestations of perfectionism. One primary source of
perfectionism is the individual's parents.
Children whose parents display affection only after their child
meets their parents' excessive expectations (i.e.,
"perfect" behavior patterns) are likely to develop
perfectionist behavioral tendencies or thought patterns (Flett, Hewitt,
Oliver, & Macdonald, 2002; Hamecheck, 1978; Missildine, 1963).
Negative perfectionism is more likely to occur when parental praise,
approval, and adoration is given only when the child behaves according
to parental expectations. Perhaps not surprisingly, then, children whose
parents have such high expectations are rarely satisfied with their
performance, even if they "succeed," or win, in competitive
conditions (Flett & Hewitt, 2005). In a rare study of parental
influences among competitive athletes, McArdle and Duda (2004) found
that the athlete's parents markedly contributed toward the
development of perfectionism among athletes by promoting the
athlete's disposition of ego-oriented goal attainment, that is,
motivation based on winning rather than enjoyment of sport
participation.
One source of expectations and perhaps subsequent perfectionist
tendencies, specific to sport and heretofore under-addressed in the
literature, is the coach. Coaches are instrumental in crafting and
sustaining the learning environment in a sport context (Anshel, 2003).
Previous studies of sport perfectionism (e.g., Anshel & Eom, 2002;
Dunn, Cosgrove Dunn, & Syrotuik, 2002, Dunn, Cosgrove Dunn, Gotwals,
Vallance, Craft, & Syrotuik, 2006a) have indicated an additional
dimension of perfectionism that may supplement the existing dimensions
as parental expectations and criticism in their
"socially-prescribed" context (Hewitt & Flett, 1991). In
sport, specifically, the perceived expectations of the coach can
markedly affect athletic performance. It is apparent that athletic
coaches jointly influence an athlete's expectations, perceptions,
and other thought patterns that mirror characteristics of perfectionism
(e.g., Anshel & Eom, 2002; Dunn et al., 2002; Dunn et al., 2005,
2006a).
Taken together, then, the collective findings of previous
perfectionism in sport studies (e.g., Anshel & Eom, 2001; Anshel
& Mansouri, 2005; Dunn et al., 2006a,b) suggest that perfectionism
has three sources: the athlete (e.g., personal standards, concern over
mistakes), the athlete's parents (e.g., perceived parental
pressure, parental criticism), and the athlete's coach (e.g.,
perceived coach pressure, coach criticism).
Several sport-related studies in recent years have focused on
generating valid measures of perfectionism among competitive athletes.
Anshel and Eom (2002), for instance, found that concern over mistakes,
personal standards, parental criticism, and coach criticism were the
dimensions that best described perfectionism among competitive athletes;
low correlations between the four dimensions indicated these dimensions
were independent. The dimensions doubts about actions and organization
were not represented.
Dunn et al. (2006a) attempted to establish construct validity for a
sport-related instrument named the Sport Multidimensional Perfectionism
Scale, or Sport-MPS, on four independent samples of Canadian athletes.
Participants included, respectively, 276 adolescent football players,
229 adolescent ice hockey players, 221 intercollegiate athletes from
various team sports, and 121 adolescent female figure skaters. Their
results confirmed evidence of four factors generalizable across samples
that define perfectionism in competitive sport: personal standards,
concern over mistakes, perceived parental pressure, and perceived coach
pressure. Of particular importance is that "parents and coaches are
central, yet separate, characters associated with perceived social
pressure" (p. 73). Clearly, then, as Dunn et al. concluded, parents
and coaches should be included in future measures of perfectionism,
"not only in sport, but in other social-evaluative contexts as
well" (p. 73). The authors did not examine gender differences, an
area of sport-related perfectionism that has been virtually ignored by
researchers.
Apparently, researchers have ignored the sub-dimension of
self-criticism as a component of perfectionism. Reflecting their review
of related literature, Flett and Hewitt (2005) assert that
perfectionism, in general, is maladaptive. One indicant of this is
excessive concern over mistakes leading to low self-esteem, low
perceived athletic competence, and dissatisfaction with their sports
performance. The authors claim "certain perfectionists are highly
concerned with presenting an image of perfection to other people.
Athletes with this tendency should be susceptive to a variety of
negative outcomes" (p. 16). Gotwals et al. (2003) found that
athletes who scored high on perfectionism tended to be concerned about
their mistakes, doubted their actions, and perceived their parents as
being critical of them. It is apparent, therefore, that one primary
outcome of perfectionistic thinking in competitive sport would include
self-criticism, particularly in response to the athlete's
perception of not meeting high personal standards and the expectations
of themselves and of others (e.g., parents, coaches, teammates,
spectators). Yet, self-criticism has been ignored in past related sport
psychology research and warrants further investigation.
The rationale for this study is based primarily on two issues.
First, several sport perfectionism studies have generated various
interpretations of this construct, with only a moderate degree of
consistency about its dimensions and the items that represent these
dimensions. Dunn, Gotwals, Causgrove Dunn, and Syrotuik (2006b) have
concluded "there is currently a lack of research examining the
potential influence of perfectionism upon athletes'
performance-related cognitions and affect in sport" (p. 10). Along
these lines, Flett and Hewitt (2002) contend, "there is a growing
concern ... that the term perfectionism has a variety of meanings and
that the same term is being used to refer to different concepts"
(p. 13). For example, the authors contend there is a difference between
the person's perfectionistic standards and the extent to which
those standards have been reached, that is, the failure to attain
perfection based on self-evaluation processes, at least among
nonathletes. The sport perfectionism literature has not addressed this
discrepancy. The second issue is that examining perfectionism in sport
as a function of gender has received only scant attention by
researchers.
Researchers have examined the role of gender in the development of
perfectionist traits in the processes of social development,
interpersonal relationships, friendship dynamics, and personality
correlates. For example, Hewitt and Flett (2002) suggest possible gender
differences in the way self-oriented perfectionism interacts with other
personality characteristics. In particular, the researchers found
self-oriented perfectionism among males was related to traits of
hostility and arrogance, while self-oriented women tended to be
agreeable and gregarious. In a study of 600 academically talented sixth
grade students, as compared to a group of 418 non-gifted sixth graders
from the same schools, Parker and Mills (1996) found that parental
expectations for non-gifted girls was significantly lower than for
non-gifted boys, whose results were statistically similar to all
academically talented students. Total perfectionism scores were similar
for gifted boys and girls, and were significantly higher than for
non-gifted girls. Perfectionism, then, appears to differ as a function
of gender, at least among non-athletes.
In a gender study of perfectionism and self-esteem and shame,
Ashby, Rice, and Martin (2006) found that self-esteem and shame each
mediated the association between perfectionism and depression for
females, but not for males. In particular, higher self-esteem and lower
feelings of shame were related to decreased perfectionism and
depression. For men, shame mediated the perfectionism-depression
relationship. The effect of perfectionism on self-esteem, however, did
not lead to higher depression, as it did with female participants. It
appears, then, that gender influences the association between
perfectionism and other cognitive conditions, and likely mediates the
development of perfectionism. Additional research on identifying the
sources (and related dimensions) of perfectionism as a function of
gender among competitive athletes is warranted. Unfortunately, any
attempt to examine the role of gender in determining or developing
perfectionism among competitive athletes must remain highly speculative
at this point because of the paucity of data in this area.
One area of perfectionism research strongly suggests that gender
differences are linked to body image. In their review of this
literature, for example, Antony and Swinson (1998) concluded that
perfectionism about one's (distorted) body image, clinically
referred to as body dysmorphic disorder, and consequent eating disorders
(e.g., anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa) were significantly more common
among females than males. Perfectionistic thinking, therefore, may be
strongly linked to body image problems. This suggests that genders may
differ on the components of perfectionism within the context of
competitive sport, and that recognizing individual differences between
male and female athletes in clinical treatment appears warranted. This
is not to suggest, however, that female athletes would necessarily score
higher for perfectionism than their male peers.
Perfectionism is more prevalent under conditions of heightened
emotion, such as anger (Vallance et al., 2006), and when a personally
meaningful goal is at stake (Berkowitz & Harmon-Jones, 2004),
conditions that are exacerbated by a person's dispositional
competitiveness, or competitive orientation (Gill, 2002). Based on her
review of related literature, Gill concluded "males typically
scored higher than females on competitiveness and win orientation"
(p. 362). Taken together, it would be plausible to speculate, therefore,
that male athletes would demonstrate a greater degree of perfectionism
in sport then their female counterparts, at least among non-elite
competitors.
One dimension that was generated in earlier non-sport research,
neatness/organization (Frost et al., 1990), has been ignored in sport
psychology, primarily because Frost et al. found poor reliability among
items in this sub-dimension. Second, relatively few sport psychology
researchers have distinguished between positive and negative aspects of
perfectionism, an important omission since each has markedly different
implications for clinical interventions. Finally, as indicated earlier,
there is a paucity of related research that has examined perfectionism
as a function of gender.
Thus, the rationale for this study is based on two issues. First,
several sport perfectionism studies have generated various
interpretations of this construct, with only a moderate degree of
consistency about its dimensions and the items that represent these
dimensions. Dunn et al. (2006b) have concluded "there is currently
a lack of research examining the potential influence of perfectionism
upon athletes' performance-related cognitions and affect in
sport" (p. 10). Along these lines, Flett and Hewitt (2002) contend,
"there is a growing concern ... that the term perfectionism has a
variety of meanings and that the same term is being used to refer to
different concepts" (p. 13). For example, the authors contend there
is a difference between the person's perfectionistic standards and
the extent to which those standards have been reached, that is, the
failure to attain perfection based on self-evaluation processes, at
least among nonathletes. The sport perfectionism literature has not
addressed this discrepancy. The second issue is that examining
perfectionism in sport as a function of gender has received only scant
attention by researchers. Given the increased proliferation of female
sports participants and the need to encourage females of all ages to
engage in sports activities to control obesity, determining evidence of
perfectionism between males and females is warranted.
The purposes of this study, then, were to identify dimensions of
perfectionism among skilled competitive athletes (who had competed on at
least one high school team), as determined by exploratory factor
analysis, and then to compare genders on each dimension. Despite the
virtual absence of existing perfectionism in sport research literature
on gender differences (Antony & Swinson, 1998), it was hypothesized
that male athletes would exhibit higher perfectionism scores than their
female counterparts. This is due, primarily, to previous findings that
males tend to be more competitive in sport settings than their female
counterparts, at least among nonelite level competitors (Gill, 2002).
This study was conducted in two stages. In the first stage,
coherent items were selected based on the results of an exploratory
factor analysis (EFA), along with item evaluation from a group of
experts, and selected items were evaluated with Cronbach's alpha
for internal consistency of each factor. The second stage consisted of
computing multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) to determine
differences between male and female athletes for each factor (dimension)
of perfectionism generated by the EFA.
Method
Participants
The data used in this study were collected from 322 college
students (142 males and 180 females), ranging in age from 18-31 years (M
= 22.5 yrs, SD = 6.32). The participants, all of whom volunteered for
this study, attended one of two universities, located 12 miles apart, in
the southeastern U.S., and met the criterion of having competed on at
least one high school sports team. Most of these participants were
physical education majors, and have continued to compete as intramural
athletes on their respective college campuses. Demographic data
indicated that the athletes had participated in both team (n = 267) and
individual sports (n = 55).
Inventory Development
The Likert-type inventory generated for this study, called the
Sport Perfectionism Scale (SPS), reflected the four dimensions of
perfectionism depicted in the previous sport psychology literature.
Three related studies, Anshel and Eom (2002), Anshel and Mansouri
(2005), and Dunn et al. (2002), provided the primary sources of items
for generating the current sport related perfectionism inventory, each
with highly similar factors and items. As indicated earlier, Anshel and
Eom (2002) proposed four factors, each consisting of eight items. These
were concern over mistakes, personal standards, parental expectations
and criticism, and coach expectations and criticism. In their study of
perfectionism and goal orientation, Dunn et al. (2002) also found four
latent constructs, including concern over mistakes, personal standards,
perceived parental pressure, and perceived coach pressure. In addition,
selected items were adapted from the Frost et al. (1990) FMPS to fit a
sport framework and, as explained by Dunn et al. (2002), "modified
to make them contextually relevant" (p. 382). An example of this
feature was a factor heretofore ignored by previous researchers in this
area, neatness and organization.
Unlike items in previous related studies, we added items that
addressed a factor in the Frost et al. (1990) FMPS called neatness and
organization. Based on the clinical work of Loehr (1982, 1990) and a
review of related literature by Anshel (2003), it is apparent that
highly skilled athletes engage in pre-game and game rituals that require
planning, extensive practice, and time management/organization skills.
To Loehr, rituals help athletes manage unpleasant thoughts as an
integral part of both mental and physical preparation in meeting
performance demands. Rituals also reduce information processing load
that allow attentional focusing on the most relevant environmental
features. It is plausible to speculate that highly organized athletes
would also show high perfectionist tendencies.
Three researchers familiar with the perfectionism in sport
literature reviewed and reached 100 percent consensus for including each
item in the scale's final version. The scale consisted of 41 items
that described four factors obtained from the extant perfectionism in
sport literature. Items ranged from 1 (not at all like me) to 5 (very
much like me). Scale items are listed in the Appendix and results
section.
Procedure
Data were collected in classroom settings (with permission of the
class instructor) among undergraduate majors in physical education and
sport/exercise science at both universities. Only athletes who had
competed on at least one high school team, who were designated as
"skilled competitors," met the primary criterion for
participation in the study. They were informed that the questions
concerned their "attitudes about sport competition," that
their responses would be kept confidential, and that the inventory would
be anonymous.
Data Analyses
An exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was conducted on 41 items to
determine the initial factor structure. Factors were not predetermined.
Methods of analyzing the validity of these scales have focused on factor
analysis that identifies the underlying latent traits, a method used in
numerous previous studies in the perfectionism in sport research
literature (e.g., Anshel & Eom, 2002; Ashby & Rice, 2002; Dunn,
Dunn, & Syrotuik, 2002; Gotwals, Dunn, & Wayment, 2003; Haase
& Prapavessis, 2004; Harvey, Pallant, & Harvey, 2004). In the
present study, exploratory factor analysis provided a measure of item
reduction by suggesting for elimination items that did not load at .50
or above onto any of the factors (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). In
order to evaluate the internal consistency of each factor,
Cronbach's alpha was computed for each factor. The reduced items
forming each factor were then compared with a one-way multivariate
analysis of variance (MANOVA) to ascertain evidence of gender
differences for the perfectionism factors.
Results
Exploratory Factor Analysis and Cronbach's Alphas
The initial EFA yielded two choices in terms of the number of
factors; a nine-factor solution based the default eigenvalue of 1 (58%
of variance explained) and a four-factor solution with the eigenvalue of
2 along with a clear SCREE-elbow (43% of variance explained). The
four-factor solution was chosen for further interpretation. The
percentage of variance for each of the four factors was 19.02, 11.23,
7.72, and 5.14% respsectively. The factors were rotated through the
PROMAX (oblique) rotation method, with items numbered 1 through 41 based
on the EFA results. With the combination of a minimum factor loading
value of .50 and a careful examination of each item, items were selected
for each factor. This procedure eliminated 10 items resulting in a
four-factor solution with 31 items. The Appendix lists the final set of
31 items.
Factor 1 originally consisted of 12 items. Sample items were
"Mom or dad are happy only if my performance is outstanding,"
and "Mom or dad expect me to be perfect in sport." Item 11,
"Either parent always had very high expectations of my future
performance in sport," and item 12, "Mom or dad set very high
standards for me," were eliminated due to low factor loadings (.39
and .27, respectively). Thus, factor 1 was labeled Parental Expectations
and Criticism (PE/C). This factor is an extension of the previous
literature that links both expectations and criticism of the
athlete's parents within one factor.
Factor 2 included nine items. Sample items included "Even the
smallest mistake bothers me when I am competing," "I criticize
myself very hard after making an error," and "Even after I
win, I do not feel happy about my performance." Item 21 ("I
never feel I have trained enough") was deleted because its factor
loading (.36) fell below the criterion of .50. Thus, factor 2 was
labeled Self-Criticism (SC). Again, this factor adds a component not
previously addressed by researchers, the prominent role of
self-criticism, in addition to (or rather than) personal standards, as a
feature of perfectionism.
Factor 3 included five items. Sample items were "Neatness is
very important to me," "Being organized is very important to
me," and "I try to be a neat (tidy) person." Factor 3
revealed both clear logical unity and high factor loading values,
ranging from .74 to .80, and was labeled Neatness and Organization
(N/O). This factor has been absent from previous sport perfectionism
studies; however, it was strongly represented in the EFA with respect to
coherency of items and their respective factor loadings.
Factor 4 included eight items. Sample items were "My coach
would become angry with me or punish me if I performed below his
expectations," and "My coach rarely compliments me on my
performance." The rest of items in factor 4 were all related to
coach's expectations and criticism and showed high factor loading
values, ranging from .73 and .52. Thus, factor 4 was labeled Coach
Expectations/Criticism (CE/C). Not unlike factor 1 that describes
parental influences on perfectionism, factor 4 combines both
expectations and criticism by an important "significant other"
that is unique to competitive sport, the athlete's coach.
The Cronbach's alpha value was computed for each factor and
for the whole battery of items to ascertain internal consistency. It was
found that the complete perfectionism scale was relatively well
structured with a Cronbach's alpha value of .85. Cronbach alphas
for factors 1 through 4, respectively, were .85, .83, .87, and .79.
Group Comparisons
To determine gender differences among the dimensions of
perfectionism, a one-way MANOVA was computed to compare genders for the
linear combination of four dimensions (factors). The MANOVA results
indicated a significant gender effect for the linear combination of four
factors, F (4, 317) = 6.00, p< .0001, Wilk's Lambda = .930.
Follow-up univariate ANOVAs indicated gender differences for factor 1
(Parental Expectations and Criticism), F (1,320) = 12.57, p=.0005;
effect size (Cohen's d) = .40. Female athletes scored (M=23.23)
significantly higher than male athletes (M=20.54) on factor 1. However,
male athletes scored significantly higher (19.78) than female athletes
(18.25) on Factor 3 (Neatness and Organization), F ( 1, 320) =
10.97,p=.001, d=.37. Factors 2 (Self-Criticism) and 4 (Coach
Expectations and Criticism) showed no significant gender differences.
Inter-factor correlations, listed in Table 2, indicated that the
dimensions of perfectionism were relatively independent. The
inter-factor correlation coefficients ranged from .003 through .21 in
the absolute value, except the correlation coefficients between Parental
Expectations & Criticism and Coach Expectations & Criticism (r =
.31), and between Self-criticism, and Coach Expectations & Criticism
(r = .40).
Discussion
The purposes of this study were to identify dimensions of
perfectionism among skilled competitive athletes (who had competed on at
least one of their high school teams), as determined by exploratory
factor analysis, and then to compare genders on each dimension. It was
predicted, based on the extant non-sport literature, that male athletes
would reflect a higher degree of perfectionism on all dimensions than
their female counterparts. The results indicated that the four factors
(dimensions) identified were parental expectations and criticism (PE/C,
factor 1), personal standards and self-criticism (SC, factor 2),
neat/organization (N/O, factor 3), and coach expectations and criticism
(CE/C, factor 4). Gender comparisons on each dimension indicated that
females demonstrated higher perfectionism for PE/C, while males scored
significantly higher for N/O. No gender differences were noted for SC
and CE/C.
The designation of perfectionism dimensions in this study formed a
clear extension of the existing sport-related literature. Specifically,
the concept of self-criticism, which was juxtaposed with personal
standards in this study, has not been previously labeled and examined in
the sport perfectionism research literature. This is surprising, since
self-criticism has received attention by non-sport researchers. For
instance, Dunkley, Zuroff, and Blankstein (:2003) found that
self-critical perfectionism was associated with maladaptive tendencies,
such as low perceived social support and hassles. The researchers also
found that self-critical perfectionists were emotionally reactive to
stressors that implied possible failure, loss of control, and criticism
from others, all of which are relevant to situations common among
competitive athletes.
Slaney, Franze, and Rice (2004) found that maladaptive
perfectionist scores were strongly correlated with self-critical
depression. Maladaptive perfectionism among athletes reflects two
characteristics that are relevant to the current study: setting overly
high standards and being self-critical if the individual fails to meet
these standards (Enns & Cox, 2002), or "failure associated with
harsh self-criticism" (p. 51). The present results indicated that
both genders were statistically similar on this (SC) dimension. Thus, it
appears that items that depict self-criticism warrant inclusion in
future perfectionism in sport research.
Future researchers might want to consider reexamining
self-criticism from a clinical perspective that reflects one source of
an athlete's perfectionistic cognitive and behavioral tendencies
(Enns & Cox, 2002; Flett & Hewitt, 2002). Self-criticism in
competitive sport, also called negative self-talk, self-blame, and
rumination, is deleterious to proper attentional focusing and
performance in sport settings (Anshel, 2003; Flett & Hewitt, 2005).
Practitioners could address individual differences on determining
sources of perfectionism and possible treatment (Flett & Hewitt,
2005). The consistent failure in meeting expectations, despite the fact
that expectations are often unattainable, may cause an individual to
increase self-blame and discontinue participation in activity (Dunn et
al., 2006). These outcomes are particularly important in competitive
sport because past studies have indicated they are detrimental to high
quality performance and may lead to dropping out (Anshel, 2003).
Apparently, this problem exists for both genders.
Another result of this study indicated no significant gender
differences for SC and CE/ C. These results are supported by previous
studies (see Gill, 2004, for a review) indicating that highly skilled
"female athletes possess more masculine personality characteristics
than do female nonathletes" (p. 482). Gill contends
"competitive athletics demands instrumental, assertive (certainly
competitive) behaviors" (p. 482). Perhaps the role of competitive
achievement orientation may be a manifestation of parental expectations
and criticism in fostering perfectionism among highly skilled female
athletes. This confirms previous gender in sport studies indicating
similar personality characteristics of skilled male and female athletes,
particularly with respect to goal orientation and need achievement
(Gill, 1993, 2002, 2004). Thus, perhaps it is not surprising that male
and female athletes are similarly susceptible to the high standards and
critical feedback of their respective coaches as a source of
perfectionistic thinking. This conclusion may carry over to
perfectionism in sport.
That females had significantly higher scores for parental
expectations and criticism may indicate that female athletes are even
more influenced by their parents than their male counterparts. According
to Parker (2002), parents' expectations and criticism reflect a
common experience among perfectionist children, in which parents push
their children to achieve at exceptional levels. It is important that
these adults create a balance between high, but attainable expectations
and standards, while offering both praise and critical feedback that
reflect performance quality, as well as performance outcome (Anshel,
2003). Maladaptive perfectionism in sport may result in low
participation satisfaction, and may lead to dropping out due to a dearth
of an athlete's perceived competence, gratification, and a sense of
fulfillment as a sport participant (Ashby & Rice, 2002; Dunn et al.,
2002).
In summary, the inclusion of new dimensions within the unique
context of competitive sport should be considered in future research.
One area of perfectionism that is unique to sport concerns the
athlete's coach, who apparently is a source of perfectionism among
athletes of both genders, as confirmed by the results of several
previous related studies (Anshel & Eom, 2002; Dunn, Dunn, &
Syrotuik, 2002; Vallance, Dunn, & Causgrove-Dunn, 2006). The role of
adult "significant others" (i.e., parents and coaches) in
developing perfectionistic thinking is very apparent in competitive
sport. Perhaps, not surprisingly, the role of parents and coaches of
perfectionist athletes is consistent with findings from the extant
related sport psychology literature (e.g., Anshel & Eom, 2002; Dunn
et al., 2002).
The dimension SC, which did not differ as a function of gender in
this study, often reflects the athlete's irrational need to achieve
at a level that may be unrealistic. Items in this factor reflect an
irrational need to achieve and an overly favorable self-evaluation
(Flett & Hewitt, 2005). If these irrational syndromes are
persistent, they are a clear manifestation of maladaptive or negative
perfectionism. Sources of maladaptive perfectionism, particularly based
on the SC dimension, include the athlete's significant others
(Blankstein & Dunkley, 2002), in this study, the athlete's
coaches and parents. This effect appears exacerbated if the adult's
mentoring style is authoritative (Flett, Hewitt, & Singer, 1995).
However, results of the present study, in which females demonstrated
higher perfectionism for PE/C than males, suggest that female athletes
may be more affected by autocratic coaching and parenting on developing
perfectionistic thinking then their male counterparts, at least for
higher-skilled, adult-aged competitors. Nevertheless, because the nature
of competitive sport inherently promotes interindividual and
intra-individual comparisons, the tendency to strive to succeed in
achievement settings and demonstrate perfectionistic thinking is
exacerbated for both males and females (Leonard, 1998). Clinical
research among competitive athletes is needed to confirm these
speculations and to examine the effectiveness of interventions to help
control it.
Finally, the factor, neatness/organization, has been absent from
previous perfectionism in sport research. Sample items included
"being organized is very important to me" and "neatness
is very important to me." Frost et al. (1990) deleted this factor
in their measure of total perfectionism because "the organization
subscale was the least highly correlated with the other subscales of the
MPS and the other measures of perfectionism" (p. 458). "For
this reason," they state, "these items were not used in the
calculation of the overall perfectionism score" in their initial
and subsequent research (p. 458). In a study of college student
exercisers, Anshel and Seipel (2006) found that individuals who created
a plan and were organized in theft approach to exercise exhibited
greater levels of adherence. Planning and organization were also
positively correlated with personal standards (achievement goals).
The results of the present study indicated that N/O was highly
cohesive, with extraordinarily high factor loadings. We speculate that
athletes who possess highly perfectionistic thought and behavioral
patterns are also characterized by high organization skills. This is
manifested in sport by developing routines that virtually assure optimal
mental and physical preparation. For example, elite athletes are
characterized by disciplined thought patterns under stress and
developing consistency by the use of mental checklists, even between
points in competitive tennis (Loehr, 1990).
An explanation of the finding that males scored significantly
higher than females on this factor can only be speculative. For the
athletes in this study, who shared the characteristic of having competed
on their high school team, perhaps male competitors were more likely to
have developed sport-related rituals than their female counterparts. It
is known, however, that nonelite male athletes are more competitive than
female athletes, although sex differences on characteristics of
perfectionism, particularly of this dimension, is apparently
non-existent, at least among elite athletes (Dunn et al., 2005, 2006;
Gill, 1992). This area warrants additional study.
One area of the extant perfectionism in sport literature that
remains controversial is whether perfectionism includes positive, or
desirable, properties. As indicated earlier, perfectionism has both
desirable (adaptive) and undesirable (maladaptive) properties, however,
researchers have not sufficiently explored the dichotomous nature of
this construct. In the context of competitive sport, positive
perfectionism would reflect maintaining high personal standards, as
indicated by setting challenging and achievable goals, a strong desire
to excel and improve, not being too self-critical, and altering
self-expectations in accordance with situational factors, particularly
in achievement settings (Enns & Cox, 2002). Maladaptive
perfectionism in sport, however, describes setting excessively high
standards, being overly self-critical, especially after mistakes, and
being extensively influenced by the approval of parents and coaches
(Anshel & Mansouri, 2005). Individuals characterized as maladaptive,
or neurotic, perfectionists tend to exhibit exaggerated expectations,
have a high fear of failure, and are less capable of coping with
challenging situations. In their study, Stoeber and Otto (2006)
concluded that "healthy perfectionists show higher levels of
positive characteristics compared to unhealthy perfectionists and
nonperfectionists" and that "self-oriented perfectionistic
strivings are positive if perfectionists are not overly concerned about
mistakes and negative evaluations by others" (p. 219). Enns and Cox
(2002) conclude, in their critique of the perfectionism self-report
research, that researchers need to clearly delineate the positive and
negative aspects of perfectionism to optimize risk assessment for
psychopathology and for improving our understanding of this construct.
Participants in the current study were a convenience sample of
former high school athletes, as opposed to current, college sports
competitors. The researchers acknowledge the inherent limitation of
retrospective (subjective) recall procedures. However, two procedures
provide appropriate psychometric validity of the data that enhance
recall accuracy. First, respondents in the current sample were asked to
recall actual, not simulated, events in their previous sport
experiences. As Schwarzer and Schwarzer (1996) contend, "nothing
can replace real-life events for ecological validity of the assessment
procedure" (p. 129). Second, perfectionism is commonly viewed as a
personality trait, not typically susceptible to inaccurate recall. To
Beehr and McGrath (1996), dispositional assessment (as opposed to
situational assessment) has stronger psychometric properties, partly
because the individual is asked to reflect general feelings about their
experiences as former athletes as opposed to specific situations that
are more susceptible to forgetting (e.g., "neatness is very
important to me," "My mom or dad treated me differently after
winning than after losing in sport"). Most studies that consist of
examining psychological dispositions have included a long-term
subjective recall procedure. Recall accuracy would likely be improved
through personal interview techniques (Enns & Cox, 2002). Further
research is warranted in developing a validated inventory that lends
further insights into the development and manifestation of both healthy
and unhealthy perfectionism in competitive sport.
Appendix
Perfection in Sport Scale (PSS) Items
Factor 1: Parental Expectations and Criticism (PE/C)
1. Mom or dad was happy only if my performance is outstanding.
2. Mom or dad expected me to be perfect in sport.
3. I never felt like I could meet my parents' standards.
4. Mom or dad never tended to criticize my performance, even after
complimenting me.
5. I never felt like I could meet my parents' expectations.
6. As a child, I was punished for doing things less than perfect.
7. Either parent always expected excellence from me as an athlete.
8. Mom or dad never tried to understand the reasons for my
mistakes.
9. My mom or dad treated me differently after winning than after
losing in sport.
10. After winning or performing successfully, at least one of my
parents pointed out something I did not do well during the competition.
Factor 2: Self-Criticism (SC)
11. Even the smallest mistake bothered me when I am competing.
12. I criticized myself very hard after making an error.
13. If I won the competition or generally performed well, I still
tended to criticize myself if I have made an error.
14. Making a mistake, even a "small" one, bothered me.
15. I felt very upset or angry if I made a physical or mental error
during the contest.
16. I always strive for perfection in my sport performance.*
17. When I evaluate myself as an athlete, I tend to think about my
weaknesses rather than about my strenths.
18. Even after I win, I do not feel happy about my performance.
Factor 3: (Neatness/Organization; N/O)
19. Neatness is very important to me.
20. I try to be a neat (tidy) person.
21. I am a neat (tidy) person.
22. Being organized is very important to me.
23. I am not an organized person.
Factor 4: Coach Expectations and Criticism (PE/C)
24. My coach would become angry with me or punish me if I performed
below his expectations.
25. My coach rarely compliments me on my performance.
26. My coach does not usually try to understand the reasons for my
mistakes.
27. My coach usually expects me to perform perfectly.
28. I can rarely meet my coach's expectations of me.
29. No matter how well I performed, my coach asks me to perform
better.
30. I would not refer to my coach as particularly sensitive.
31. Even after I perform successfully, my coach tends to point out
my mistakes during competition.
* Marginally associated with self-criticism
References
Anshel, M. H. (2003). Sport psychology. From theory to practice
(4th ed.). San Francisco: Benjamin-Cummings.
Anshel, M. H. (2004). Sources of disordered eating patterns between
ballet dancers and non-dancers. Journal of Sport Behavior, 27, 115-134.
Anshel, M. H., & Eom, H. J. (2002). Exploring the dimensions of
perfectionism in sport International Journal of Sport Psychology, 34,
255-271.
Anshel, M. H., & Mansouri, H. (2005). Influences of
perfectionism on motor performance, affect, and causal attribution in
response to critical information feedback. Journal of Sport Behavior,
28, 99-124.
Anshel, M. H., & Seipel, S. J. (2006). Relationships between
dimensions of perfectionism and exercise behavior among college
students. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 4,
46-59.
Antony, M. M., & Swinson, R. P. (1998). When perfect isn't
good enough: Strategies for coping with perfectionism. Oakland, CA: New
Harbinger Publications.
Ashby, J. S., & Rice, K. G. (2002). Perfectionism,
dysfunctional attitudes, and self-esteem: A structural equations
analysis. Journal of Counseling & Development, 80, 197-203.
Ashby, J. S., Rice, K. G., & Martin, J. L. (2006).
Perfectionism, shame, and depressive symptoms. Journal of Counseling
& Development, 84, 148-156.
Beehr, Y. A., & McGrath, J. E. (1996). The methodology of
research on coping: Conceptual, strategies, and operational-level
issues. In M. Zeidner & N. S. Endler (Eds.), Handbook of coping:
Theory, research, applications (pp. 65-82). New York: John Wiley &
Sons.
Berkowitz, L., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). Toward an
understanding of the determinants of anger. Emotion, 4, 107-130.
Blankstein, K. R., & Dunkley, D. M. (2002). Evaluative
concerns, self-critical, and personal standards perfectionism: A
structural equation modeling strategy. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt
(Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 285-315).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Blatt, S. J. (1995). The destructiveness of perfectionism:
implications for the treatment of depress ion. The American
Psychologist, 50, 1003-1020.
Campbell, J. D., & Di Paula, A. (2002). Perfectionistic
self-beliefs: Their relation to personality and goal pursuit. In G. L.
Flett & P. L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory, research, and
treatment (pp. 181-198). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied Multiple
Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd Ed.).
Hillsdale, N J: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Inc.
Dunkley, D. M., Zuroff, D. C., & Blankstein, K. R. (2003).
Self-critical perfectionism and daily affect: dispositional and
situational influences on stress and coping. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 84, 234-249.
Dunn, J. G. H., Causgrove Dunn, J. L., Gotwals, J. K., Vallance, J.
K. H., Craft, J. M., & Syrotuik, D. G. (2006a). Establishing
construct validity evidence for the Sport Multidimensional Perfectionism
Scale. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7, 57-79.
Dunn, J. G. H., Causgrove Dunn, J. C., & Syrotuik, D. G.
(2002). Relationship between multidimensional perfectionism and goal
orientation in sport. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24,
376-395.
Dunn, J. G. H., Gotwals, J. K., & Cosgrove Dunn, J. L. (2005).
An examination of the domain specificity of perfectionism among
intercollegiate student-athletes. Personality and Individual
Differences, 38, 1439-1448.
Dunn, J. G. H., Gotwals, J. K., & Cosgrove Dunn, J., Syrotuik,
D. G. (2006b). Examining the relationship between perfectionism and
trait anger in competitive sport. International Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 4, 7-24.
Enns, M. W., & Cox, B. J. (2002). The nature and assessment of
perfectionism: A critical analysis. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt
(Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 33-62).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2005). The perils of
perfectionism in sports and exercise. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 14, 14-18.
Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2002). Perfectionism and
maladjustment: An overview of theoretical, definitional, and treatment
issues. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory,
research, and treatment (pp. 5-32). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Oliver, J. M., & Macdonald, S.
(2002). Perfectionism in children and their parents: A developmental
analysis. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism:
Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 89-132). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L., & Singer, A. (1995).
Perfectionism and parental authority styles. Individual Psychology, 51,
50-60.
Flett, G. L., Sawatzsy, D. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (1995).
Dimensions of perfectionism and goal commitment: A further comparison of
two perfectionism measures. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
Assessment, 17, 11l-124.
Frost, R. O., & Henderson, K. J. (1991). Perfectionism and
reactions to athletic competition. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 13, 323-335.
Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Laharat, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990).
The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14,
449-468.
Gill, D. L. (2002). Gender and sport behavior. In T. S. Horn (Ed.),
Advances in sport psychology (pp. 355-375). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Gill, D. L. (1993). Competitiveness and competitive orientation in
sport. In R. N. Singer, M. Murphey, & L. K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook
of research on sport psychology (pp. 314-327). New York: Macmillan.
Gill, D. L. (2004). Gender and cultural diversity across the
lifespan. In M. R. Weiss (Ed.), Developmental sport and exercise
psychology: A developmental perspective. Morgantown, WV: Fitness
Information Technology.
Gotwals, J. K., Dunn, J. G. H., & Wayment, H. A. (2003). An
examination of perfectionism and self-esteem in intercollegiate
athletes. Journal of Sport Behavior, 26, 17-38.
Haase, A. M., & Prapavessis, H. (2004). Assessing the factor
structure and composition of the Positive and Negative Perfectionism
Scale in sport. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1725-1740.
Haase, A. M., Prapavessis, H., & Owens, R. G. (2002).
Perfectionism, social physique anxiety and disordered eating: A
comparison of male and female elite athletes. Psychology of Sport and
Exercise, 3, 209-222.
Hall, H. K., Kerr, A. W., & Matthews, J. (1998). Precompetitive
anxiety in sport: The contribution of achievement goals and
perfectionism. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 20, 194-217.
Hamachek, D. E. (1978). Psychodynamics of normal and neurotic
perfectionism. Psychology: A Journal of Human Behavior, 15, 27-33.
Harvey, B., Pallant, J., & Harvey, D. (2004). An evaluation of
the factor structure of the Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale.
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 64, 1007-1018.
Hatzigeorgiadis, A. (2006). Approach and avoidance coping during
task performance in young men: the role of goal attainment expectancies.
Journal of Sport Sciences, 24, 299-308.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, (3. L. (2002). Perfectionism and stress
processes in psychopathology. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt (Eds.),
Perfectionism: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 255-284).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self
and social contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with
psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60,
456-470.
Hinton, P.S., & Kubas, K.L. (2005). Psychosocial correlates of
disordered eating in female collegiate athletes: validation of the
ATHLETE questionnaire. Journal of American College Health, 54, 149-157.
Hopkinson, R. A., & Lock, J. (2004). Athletes, perfectionism,
and disordered eating. Eating and Weight Disorders, 9, 99-106.
Loehr, J. E. (1990). The mental game: Winning at pressure tennis.
New York: Penguin.
Loehr, J. E. (1982). Mental toughness training for sports:
Achieving athletic excellence. New York: Penguin.
Leonard, W. M. (1998). A sociological perspective of sport (5th
ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
McArdle, S., & Duda, J. L. (2004). Exploring social-contextual
correlates of perfectionism in adolescents: A multivariate perspective.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28, 765-788.
Missildine, W. H. (1963). Your inner child of the past. New York:
Simon and Schuster.
Ommundsen, Y., Roberts, G. C., Lemyre, P. N., & Miller, B. W.
(2005). Peer relationships in adolescent competitive soccer:
Associations to perceived motivational climate, achievement goals and
perfectionisms. Journal of Sports Sciences, 23, 977-989.
Parker, W. D. (2002). Perfectionism and adjustment in gifted
children. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism:
Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 133-148). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Parker, W. D., & Mills, C. J. (1996). The incidence of
perfectionism in gifted students. Gifted Child Quarterly, 40, 194-199.
Schwarzer, R., & Schwarzer, C. (1996). A critical survey of
coping instruments. In M. Zeidner & N. S. Endler (Eds.), Handbook of
coping: Theory, research, applications (pp. 107-132). New York: John
Wiley & Sons.
Slaney, R. B., Franze, S., & Rice, K. G. (2004).
Self-criticism, dependency, self-esteem, and grade point average
satisfaction among clusters of perfectionists and nonperfectionists.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51, 192-213.
Stoeber, J., & Eismann, U. (2007). Perfectionism in young
musicians: Relations with motivation, effort, achievement, and distress.
Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 2182-2192.
Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive perfectionism:
Conceptions, evidence, challenges. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 10, 219-319.
Stoeber, J., Otto, K., Pescheck, E., Becker, C., & Stoll, O.
(2007). Perfectionism and competitive anxiety in athletes:
Differentiating striving for perfection and negative reactions to
imperfections. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 959-969.
Suddarth, B. H. & Slaney, R. B. (2001). An investigation of the
dimensions of perfectionism in college students. Measurement and
Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34, 157-165.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate
statistics (5th ed.). New York: HarperCollins.
Terry-Short, L. A., Owens, G. R., & Slade, P. D. (1995).
Positive and negative perfectionism. Personality and Individual
Differences, 18, 663-668.
Vallance, J. H., Dunn, J. G. H., & Causgrove Dunn, J. L.
(2006). Perfectionism, anger, and situation criticality in competitive
youth ice hockey. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 28,
383-406.
Mark H. Anshel
Middle Tennessee State University
Jwa K. Kim
Middle Tennessee State University
Ruth Henry
Lipscomb University
Address Correspondence to: Mark H. Anshel, Ph.D., Department of
Health and Human Performance (joint appointment, Psychology Department),
Middle Tennesee State University, Murfreesboro, TN, 37132. Phone:
615-898-2812. E-mail: manshel@mtsu.edu.
Table 1. Rotated Factor Loadings after the PROMAX rotation
Items Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4
1 .74
2 .73
3 .70
4 .69
5 .67
6 .61
7 .61
8 .58
9 .56
10 .56
11 * .39
12 * .27
13 .81
14 .76
15 .74
16 .74
17 .73
18 .54
19 .51
20 .50
21 * .36
22 .80
23 .79
24 .77
25 .75
26 .74
27 .73
28 .67
29 .63
30 .63
31 .60
32 .57
33 .54
34 .52
35 * .32
36 * .49
37 * .42
38 * .35
39 * .40
40 * .38
41 * .40
* These items were deleted from the scale due to either low
factor loadings or lack of coherence.
Table 2. Inter factor correlation among four factors
Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4
Factor 1 1.00 .21 -.003 .31
Factor 2 1.00 .18 .40
Factor 3 1.00 .09
Factor 4 1.00
F1 = Parental Expectations and Criticism;
F2 = Self-criticism;
F3 = Neatness and Organization;
F4 = Coach Expectations and Criticism