Are we underestimating the affective benefits of exercise? An experience sampling study of university aerobics participants.
Lutz, Rafer ; Lochbaum, Marc R. ; Carson, Tyler 等
There is considerable support for the ability of acute bouts of
exercise to influence affective states (e.g., Arent, Landers, &
Etnier, 2000; Landers & Petruzzello, 1994; Thayer, 1987a). Studies
have generally shown that exercise increases states of positive affect
or energetic arousal (Gauvin & Rejeski, 1993; Lutz, Lochbaum, &
Turnbow, 2003; Thayer, 1987a), and reduces anxiety, tension, or negative
affect (Breus & O'Connor, 1998; Kennedy & Newton, 1997;
Petruzzello, Jones, & Tate, 1997; Thayer, 1987a). Interestingly,
research has demonstrated that exercise may be similarly effective in
the treatment of depression compared with other commonly-employed
modalities such as selective-serotonin reuptake inhibitors (Dunn,
Trivedi, Kampert, Clark, Chambliss, 2005). A limitation exists, however,
in that most studies compare postexercise affect to preexercise affect
rather than daily affect. This is problematic because the magnitude of
the influence of exercise on affective states may be misrepresented if
baseline measures are influenced by the laboratory situation or
knowledge of impending exercise engagement, or if such baselines are
simply different than the affect individuals experience on a daily
basis. While several studies have examined affective states in
naturalistic settings (Gauvin, Rejeski, & Norris, 1996; Giacobbi,
Hausenblas, & Frye, 2005; Thayer, 1987a), only one known study
(Petruzzello, 1995) has yet combined the common pre-post exercise
methodology with a naturalistic assessment of affective states. The
present investigation, therefore, sought to examine how average daily
affect compares to affective states reported before, at the mid-point,
and after an exercise session.
Closely related to the present question of interest, Petruzzello
(1995) examined whether commonly-reported reductions in state anxiety
after exercise might be due to a "sense of relief" that
exercise is over. He recruited participants for a study to assess
"coping strategies and psychological control" and, upon
arrival in the laboratory, two resting baselines of heart rate and state
anxiety (10-item State Anxiety Inventory, SAI; Spielberger, 1983) were
taken. Next, participants were told that they would be required to run
on a treadmill for 15 minutes and two more resting heart rate and state
anxiety baselines were taken. There was no significant change in either
heart rate or state anxiety comparing the baselines taken before
revelation of the exercise requirement with those taken after this
revelation. Additionally, Petruzzello had participants take four
SAI's during the course of a 24-hr period outside the lab. The mean
levels of state anxiety taken outside the lab were in fact slightly
greater (Study 1 M = 16.3, SD = 5.0 & Study 2 M = 17.7, SD = 4.5)
than the mean of the baseline SAI's taken in the lab (Study 1 M =
15.4, SD = 3.99 & Study 2 M = 16.1, SD = 4.3), though these were not
significant differences. Based on these results, Petruzzello stated that
it is unlikely that anxiety reduction following exercise is an
artificial finding, due to a sense of relief that exercise is over.
At present, the "sense of relief" phenomenon investigated
by Petruzzello (1995) does not appear to be an important factor related
to the anxiolytic effects of exercise. One could make this case
considering Petruzzello's findings, the relatively long-lasting (up
to 2 hrs or more) anxiety-reducing effects of exercise (e.g., Raglin
& Wilson, 1996), and the fact that anxiety-reduction occurs after a
wide range of physical activities at low to moderate intensities (see
Ekkekakis & Petruzzello, 1999) which wouldn't be expected to
invoke a sense of relief upon completion. Still, research has not often
addressed the possibility of a sense-of-relief occurring after exercise
and it should not be ignored. Even if the sense-of-relief explanation
for anxiety reduction is not valid, it is important to consider how
exercise influences affect considering pre and postexercise affective
states and how they may differ from an individual's
"average" affective states. For example, subjects in
Petruzzello's research were slightly more anxious (though not
significantly) outside the laboratory than in the laboratory.
Considering the question regarding whether daily affect and
preexercise affect differ and Petruzzelio's (1995) study, it would
be of interest to examine this question using a broader set of affective
dimensions than just anxiety. Exercise appears to have somewhat
different effects on positive and negative affective states
(Bartholomew, 1999), so it would appear important to consider several
points on the affective "circumplex" (see Carroll, Yik,
Russell, & Barrett, 1999) as opposed to only one. Another potential
limitation of Petruzzello's study was that there were a very
limited number of measures of daily affect (four) taken, and it is
unclear if they occurred randomly throughout the day.
The Importance of the Baseline
It is important to understand how affect may be influenced at the
baseline time point because preexercise affect appears to influence the
effects of exercise on postexercise affect. For example, O'Connor,
Petruzzello, Kubitz, and Robinson (1995) found that those with higher
preexercise anxiety levels exhibited larger reductions in anxiety
following maximal aerobic exercise testing (r's = .27 to .65).
Also, postexercise improvements in revitalization have been shown to
exist only if participants' baseline scores were low or moderate
(Rejeski et al., 1995). In fact, Rejeski and colleagues state that
"some of the confusion in the existing literature regarding effect
sizes from psychological research on exercise may have been caused by
differences in level of baseline functioning" (p. 357).
Experience Sampling Method (ESM)
A problem arises, however, in determining how to best measure
affective experience in participants' daily lives. One potential
solution is the use of the ESM (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1987;
Diener & Larsen, 1984), which provides a means of collecting
information about daily life in its natural setting by allowing
participants to respond to repeated assessments over time. In this
method, participants can be signaled with a pager or electronic device
at fixed or random intervals throughout the day to complete brief
self-report forms. The ESM reduces problems in research of daily life
due to memory recall and it is an effective means by which to measure
within-subject variance across situations and/or over time (Hektner
& Csikzentrnihalyi, 2002; Scollon, Kim-Prieto, & Diener, 2003).
A benefit of the ESM is that it allows for sampling of daily experience
without contamination by expectancy effects that might be due to prior
knowledge of the sampling period (Alliger & Williams, 1993). Thus,
employing the ESM in addition to a more commonly-used exercise protocol
may allow for an interesting comparison of daily vs. exercise-related
affective states.
Statement of the Problem
Raglin (1997) and Morgan (1997) have suggested that behavioral
artifacts (e.g., volunteerism, experimenter expectancy effects,
Hawthorne effect, and participant expectancies) may be influencing the
magnitude of the effects of exercise on affect. Yet, we have very little
information concerning how affective states measured pre and
postexercise relate to affective states experienced through the course
of daily life. Understanding whether any such differences occur between
daily and preexercise affect may be useful for better understanding
different magnitude effects demonstrated by individual studies and would
allow a more comprehensive understanding of the role exercise plays to
influence affective states. If differences were observed between daily
and preexercise affective states, it might indicate that artifacts such
as participant expectancy may be influencing the magnitude of affective
response to exercise. Therefore, the intent of the present study is to
compare affective states before, at the mid-point, and after a class
exercise session to daily affect reported over one week.
Method
Participants
College-aged males (n = 15) and females (n = 21) were recruited for
the present investigation. All participants were enrolled in one of
three separate sections of step aerobics led by one of two instructors
and were recruited by open invitation by one of the investigators during
one of their class periods. Of the total number of students in these
classes (n = 75), 48% agreed to participate. Participants' ages
ranged from 18 to 23 years (M = 20.26, SD = 1.15), and participants
reported a relatively frequent amount of physical activity as they
engaged in an average of 2.97 (SD = 1.48), 3.00 (SD = 1.83), and 3.34
(SD = 2.77) bouts of exercise per week at strenuous, moderate, and mild
intensities, respectively (Leisure-Time Exercise Questionnaire, LTEQ total score: M = 51.72, SD = 20.93; Godin & Shepherd, 1985).
Measures
Activation Deactivation Adjective Checklist (AD ACL). The AD ACL
short form (see Thayer, 1989, Appendix A, pp. 178-180) was used to
assess four dimensions of affective space--energy (unipolar scale items:
active, energetic, vigorous, lively, full-of-pep), tiredness (sleepy,
tired, drowsy, wide-awake, wakeful), calmness (placid, calm, at-rest,
still, quiet), and tension (jittery, intense, fearful, clutched-up,
tense). In all instances, participants were to respond to these items by
indicating how they felt "at this moment." The AD ACL assesses
two bipolar dimensions of arousal, energetic arousal and tense arousal,
considered somewhat analogous to positive affect and negative affect in
Watson and Tellegen's (1985) two-factor model. Yet, it has been
argued that affect is best represented in circumplex space (Carroll et
al., 1999), and Ekkekakis and Petruzzello (2002) have made a case for
the use of the circumplex model in the study of exercise, thus the four
unipolar dimensions of affect represented by the AD ACL (energy,
tiredness, calmness, and tension) were employed as the dependent
measures in the present investigation.
There are a wide variety of affective measures that have been
employed in past studies in this area and could have been chosen for the
present investigation. Certainly, there seems to be no shortage of
controversy regarding the choice of affective measures related to
exercise (see Ekkekakis & Petruzzello, 1999, 2002). Yet, the
underlying theory behind the development of the AD ACL is well suited to
studies of exercise engagement and resultant effects on affective
states. As physical exercise strongly influences physical activation, it
makes sense to examine affective dimensions that may be related to the
body's harnessing of resources for energy expenditure. The AD ACL
was originally developed to measure nondirective arousal states (Thayer,
1986), but eventually revealed the presence of two underlying arousal
dimensions--one related to energetic arousal which is related to
circadian rhythms but which can be temporarily influenced by thoughts
and experiences, and the other related to tense arousal which functions
as a warning system and is more strongly tied to environmental
experience (Thayer, 1989). Thus, this scale seems particularly
appropriate in regards to the study of exercise and affective states as
it should capture the complex dynamics related to changes in the
body's activation systems which should be expected upon engaging in
physical exercise. Additionally, the AD ACL's reliability and
construct validity are well established (Purcell, 1982; Thayer, 1986),
and Ekkekakis, Hall, and Petruzzello (2005) found that the AD ACL
possessed satisfactory circumplex structure for use in physical activity
contexts.
Ratings of perceived exertion (RPE). RPE was completed based on
Borg's (1998) 10-point, category-ratio scale. The scale ranges from
0 nothing at all to 10 very, very strong to a single point of maximal
exertion. While Borg's 15-point scale has been widely used, the
10-point category-ratio scale was designed using ratio properties to
avoid ceiling effects. This version, as other versions of Borg's
scales, has been shown to be a valid and reliable indicator of fatigue
and physical exertion (Chen, Fan, & Moe, 2002; Noble &
Robertson, 1996).
Procedure
After receiving permission from the class instructors to perform
research in their aerobics classes, students were approached within the
class periods (8:00am, 9:00am, and 12:30pm classes) and provided an
overview of the investigation. Interested individuals were then given
the informed consent form as approved by a University Human
Subject's Institutional Review Board to read and sign.
Before participants were recruited, the Experience Sampling Program
(ESP; Barrett & Feldman Barrett, 2003) was loaded onto 10 Palm PDA
devices (Palm Zire Handheld PDA with 2MB memory, Palm, Inc.), which was
designed for experience sampling studies using the Palm operating
system. These Palm recorders were programmed to cue participants to
complete the AD ACL five times per day over a 7 day period (35
recordings possible) using the signal-contingent experience sampling
method (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1987), which has been used in a
large number and variety of studies as a means of examining average
daily affect (e.g., Alliger & Williams, 1993; Emmons & King,
1989; Penner, Shiffman, Paty, & Fritzsche, 1994; Scollon, Diener,
Oishi, & Biswas-Diener, 2005; Swendsen, 1998; Van Eck, Nicolson,
& Berkhof, 1998). The ESP was programmed to sound the Palms'
warning signals at five random time points through the day between the
hours of 9:00am and 10:00pm. The warning signal sounded for 120 seconds,
during which time the participant was allowed to respond by tapping the
touch screen with the stylus to begin answering the AD ACL. The AD ACL
items were presented in the same order each time and participants were
allowed 120 seconds to respond to each scale item. If participants did
not tap the touch screen within the 120 second window, there was no
opportunity to complete the experience sampling recording, thus avoiding
the possibility that participants might complete affect scales at times
other than when they were signaled.
Students who gave consent were told that the study was about the
daily lives of college students and that they would be required to carry
the Palm unit for 1 week. They were encouraged to carry the Palm device
everywhere they went during waking hours (they were given a note to
share with their professors/others demonstrating their participation in
a research project that required them to carry a Palm device that may
sound during any activity and which would require their brief response)
and to return the device to their class the following week.
Participation was solely on a voluntary basis and those who participated
were not given extra credit in their aerobics class. Data collection
started on a Tuesday or Wednesday and progressed over the next 7 day
period. After volunteers signed the consent form, participants were
instructed how the Palm device would signal for their response through
the day, and before they left class that day participants completed a
trial experience sampling moment under the supervision of a research
assistant to ensure that they understood how to use the touch screen and
complete the AD ACL on the Palm device. Experience sampling data
collection occurred for the next 7 days.
On the second day of the experiment, participants completed the AD
ACL immediately before, at the mid-point of, and 15 minutes after
engaging in a 20-min bout of step aerobic exercise in the context of a
class environment. To complete the AD ACL in the middle of the exercise
bout, a clock alarm signal sounded 10 minutes into the participation of
the aerobics class to cue participants to stop exercising and complete a
form which had been placed below their aerobics step at the beginning of
the class. Intensity of exercise was self-selected with RPE (asked
retrospectively on the mid-point AD ACL form to indicate at what level
they had been exercising) halfway through completion of exercise ranging
from 2.0 to 10 (M = 5.28, SD = 1.73).
Data Analysis
Preliminary analyses were conducted to examine compliance with
experience sampling procedures (i.e., percentage response rates) and to
examine potential influences of time of day of experience sampling
moments. To examine the effects of time of day on affective measures,
all experience sampling moments (n = 390) were treated as individual
cases and the following procedures were conducted: a) Frequencies of
experience sampling moments occurring at different times of the day were
examined; and b) scatterplots depicting the relationship between time of
recording and AD ACL subscale scores were examined. Also, because
individual differences in mood may introduce error into the latter
analyses, the mean of each participants' experience sampling points
within morning (9:00am to noon, n = 91), mid-day (12:01pm to 3:00pm, n =
104), late afternoon/early evening (3:01pm to 6:00pm, n = 96), and late
evening (6:01pm to 10:00pm, n = 99) time periods were computed. Only
participants who had two or more recordings in each time category (n =
23) were used for this analysis. Then, time category was treated as a
within-subjects variable in a one-way (Time Category) MANOVA using the
four AD ACL subscales as dependent variables.
Another set of preliminary analyses were conducted to examine
within-subjects variability in AD ACL subscale scores over the
experience sampling recordings. To examine such variability we used two
approaches. First, the standard deviations of participants' AD ACL
subscale scores were calculated to examine the dispersion of subscale
scores over recording moments. Second, we examined individual patterns
of AD ACL subscale scores over the experience sampling time points. In
the latter respect, one representative case was chosen as a means to
represent how AD ACL subscale scores varied over experience sampling
time points in addition to before, during, and after exercise.
The mean Of scores from all Palm-recording experience sampling time
points were computed to form a measure of average daily affect. Daily
affect was operationalized as the mean of experience sampling moments,
and was compared to affect scores before, at the mid-point of exercise,
and after exercise using one-way (Time) repeated measures MANOVA with
the four AD ACL subscales comprising the dependent variables.
Results
Compliance
Given the difficulty of experience sampling designs, participants
are expected to miss some signal prompts (see Scollon, Kim-Prieto, &
Diener, 2003). Of course, exactly what constitutes a problematic level
of noncompliance is unclear (Stone & Shiffman, 2002). Therefore, the
best alternative is to give an accurate and comprehensive picture of the
nature of compliance to experience sampling procedures (Stone &
Shiffman, 2002). In the present study, there was a wide range of
compliance. Of 35 possible experience sampling moments per subject,
compliance ranged from 1 (2.8%) to 25 (69.4%) responses, and the mean
response rate equaled 12.72 (35.3%). This is quite lower compliance than
found in other ESM studies (e.g., Updegraff, Gable, & Taylor, 2004,
56% mean response rate; Van Eck et al., 1998, 83% (1) mean response
rate; Yip, 2005, 70% mean response rate (2)), perhaps because we did not
provide sufficient financial or other incentive or perhaps because we
set a short time limit after the warning signal (120 seconds) for
participants to respond (see Feldman Barrett & Barrett, 2001;
Scollon et al., 2003).
Twenty-three (8 male & 15 female) participants exhibited
greater than 25% compliance (completed more than 8.75 AD ACL's over
the course of the week and missed no more than 2 days of recording) and
were included in subsequent analyses. This cut-off for compliance was
chosen based on a commonly-used level reported by Hektner and
Csikszenmihalyi (2002). (3) These participants were very active,
reporting exercising strenuously an average of 3.05 (SD = 1.72) times
per week. Only one participant reported engaging in no strenuous
exercise per week on average. For these participants, mean RPE at the
mid-point of exercise equaled 5.48 (SD = 1.75) and ranged from a minimum
of 3.0 to a maximum of 10.0. Of these remaining participants, 12
students were enrolled in the 9:00am class, seven were in the 12:30pm
class, and four were in the 8:00am class.
Time of Day of Experience Sampling Recordings
These participants' compliance was somewhat higher for the
first day of the study as indicated by number of experience sampling
moments (M = 3.22, SD = 1.31) compared to the second (M = 2.30, SD =
1.15), third (M = 2.00, SD = 1.41), fourth (M = 1.91, SD = 1.31), fifth
(M = 2.48, SD = 0.85), sixth (M = 2.70, SD = 1.26), and seventh (M =
2.35, SD = 1.15) days. For the entire week, these participants averaged
2.42 (SD = 0.70) experience sampling moments per day with the total
number experience sampling moments per participant ranging from 9 to 25
(M = 16.96, SD = 4.92). Thus, the mean response rate for participants
included in subsequent analyses equaled 48.46%.
Despite the use of a cut-off value related to participant
compliance with experience sampling procedures, there is still the
possibility that the mean of experience sampling points is not
representative of participants' typical affect. Of particular
concern, it is possible that experience sampling recordings
over-represent a certain portion of the day. Examining the frequency
distribution for experience sampling moments by time of day demonstrated
a somewhat leptokurtic distribution with a clustering of experience
sampling reports between 10am and noon (see Figure 1).
Examining the relationships between time of day and mood scores for
each of the AD ACL subscales using scatterplots and curve fitting procedures showed no apparent associations between time and the mood
dimensions considering linear, quadratic, or cubic trends for all
subscales ([R.sup.2] < .002; see Figure 2). The latter analysis, it
should be noted, ignores the possibility for individual differences in
sampling across the time of day. To better account for such differences
another analysis was conducted using mean AD ACL subscale scores for
each participant grouped morning, mid-day, late afternoon/early evening,
and late evening categories. Participants' mean values were
subjected to one-way (Time Category) repeated measures MANOVA on the
four AD ACL subscales. There was no significant multivariate main
effect, Wilks' Lambda = .87, F(12, 166.97) = 0.65, n.s.
Participants' experience sampling mean values by morning, mid-day,
late afternoon, and late evening categories, respectively, equaled 10.71
(SD = 2.84), 10.60 (SD = 2.99), 10.37 (SD = 3.55), and 10.36 (SD =
2.99), for energy; 7.05 (SD = 2.56), 6.92 (SD = 2.04), 6.96 (SD = 1.97),
and 7.28 (SD = 2.46), for tension; 14.08 (SD = 2.87), 13.28 (SD = 3.13),
13.55 (SD = 3.70), and 13.42 (SD = 2.76), for tiredness; and 11.11 (SD =
1.85), 11.48 (SD = 1.98), 11.11 (SD = 2.29), and 10.78 (SD = 2.46), for
calmness.
Within-Subject Variability of Experience Sampling Recordings
The impact of any differences exhibited between daily affect, as
calculated using the mean of experience sampling points over 7 days, and
exercise-related affective states would be lessened if there were
substantial within-subject variance for any of the AD ACL subscale
scores over experience sampling time points. The mean of the
within-subject standard deviations for the four subscales were 3.23 (SD
= 0.78), 2.66 (SD = 0.52), 1.87 (SD = 1.21), and 3.71 (SD = 1.11), for
the energy, calmness, tension, and tiredness subscales, respectively.
This would indicate that the greatest within-subject variance occurred
for the energy and tiredness subscales, while calmness and tension
demonstrated less within-subjects variability (see Figure 3) over the 1
week experience sampling period.
Another method for examining within-subject variability of
reporting across experience sampling time points is to examine
individual patterns of reporting. While this would be overly laborious to present here for 23 participants, one representative
participant's scores were plotted for all experience sampling time
points and is visually represented in Figure 4. In this 'case
study,' the participant clearly exhibited reduced tension before,
at the mid-point, and after exercise in comparison to his/her daily
recordings. Also, calmness, in this case was notably reduced at the
mid-point of exercise in relation to all other time points, and
tiredness was reduced at the exercise mid-point and after exercise in
comparison to before and during the week. Examining participants'
affective scores in this manner is clearly informative as it indicates
the magnitude of effects in relation to typical variability in the
course of a week.
Comparison of Mean Daily Affect with Exercise-Related Affect
Examining the main question of interest when comparing the average
of experience sampling (daily affect) with exercise-related affective
states, there was a significant multivariate Time effect, Wilks'
Lambda = .18, F(12, 166.97) = 12.71, p < .001. Examination of
Mauchly's test results demonstrated that data for three (energy,
tiredness, & tension) of the AD ACL subscales failed to meet the
sphericity assumption. Thus, for all follow-up univariate analyses,
degrees of freedom were adjusted using the Greenhouse-Geisser correction
(epsilon = .71 for energy, .63 for tiredness, .69 for tension, and .85
for calmness). Follow-up analyses revealed significant Time effects for
energy, F(2.13, 46.77) = 30. 10, p < .001, tiredness, F(1.90, 41.70)
= 43.24, p < .001, tension, F(2.06, 45.21) = 3.85, p < .05, and
calmness, F(2.56, 56.25) = 13.38, p < .001, subscales. Descriptive
statistics and results of Bonferonni-corrected pairwise comparisons for
each of the AD ACL subscales are presented in Table 1. Energy was no
different before exercise than mean daily affect and was elevated both
at the mid-point and at 15-min post exercise. Tiredness was lower before
exercise than mean daily affect and dropped even lower at the mid-point
and postexercise. Tension was lower before exercise than mean daily
affect, but not compared to the mid-point or postexercise. Calmness was
no different before exercise than the daily mean, dropped at the
mid-point of exercise, and elevated slightly postexercise.
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]
Discussion
The results of the present investigation suggest that preexercise
baseline measures may not be reflective of daily affect. Specifically,
tension and tiredness were both at lower levels prior to exercise than
they were based on recordings from the week prior. Energy and calmness,
however, did not differ across these two time points.
Considering the affective response to exercise, the pattern of
results was mostly as that demonstrated by previous research. At the
mid-point of exercise, participants reported greater energy and reduced
tiredness and calmness, and 15-min after exercise participants'
energy remained elevated, tiredness remained reduced, and calmness had
risen but was still reduced from both baseline measures. This pattern
follows the expected pattern of results where exercise increased tension
during exercise, and resulted in positive affective changes (Biddle,
2000; Bixby, Spalding, 8,: Hatfield, 2001 ; Thayer, 1987a, 1987b;
Thayer, Peters, Takahashi, & Birkhead-Flight, 1993; Saklofske,
Blomme, & Kelly, 1992). However, the typical reduction in anxiety
(tension) was not evidenced. This may be due to the possibility of a
delayed anxiolytic effect that may take 15 minutes or longer
(Bartholomew, 1999), and that higher intensities may actually elevate
tension for a time period (Ekkekakis & Petruzzello, 1999) as the
mean intensity during exercise was relatively high. The failure to
obtain more time points postexercise is a weakness, yet the present
investigation was constrained by participants' class schedules,
thus only permitting a 15 minute window postexercise. In the
study's design, this was deemed an acceptable situation due to the
study's focus on comparison of daily affect and preexercise affect.
Certainly, more attention to the ability of exercise to reduce tension
as measured by the AD ACL is warranted as tension scores in the present
sample were very low (particularly preexercise) indicating potential
floor effects.
It is only possible to speculate why tension and tiredness are
reduced prior to exercise compared to daily affect. One possibility is
that the time of the day influenced affective scores. For most
participants in this study (n = 16) exercise occurred during the morning
(8:00am or 9:00am) when tiredness would be expected to be higher (Adan,
2005; Thayer 1978; Thayer, Takahashi, & Pauli, 1988). Our findings
contradict, therefore, what we might expect concerning our comparison
between preexercise tiredness and average tiredness at all points across
the day (preexercise tiredness should be higher based on time of day).
Also, examining the experience sampling scores by time did not suggest a
major impact in this regard for any of the subscales. There is, however,
an existing line of research that may explain why tiredness may be
reduced preexercise. In the present investigation, participants knew
they would soon be exercising and may have already begun harnessing
resources to engage in the bout. Decety, Jeannerod, Germain and Pastene
(1991) have demonstrated that motor imagery of actions such as exercise
increase heart and respiratory rates in a similar and proportional,
though reduced, manner to the action imagined. As participants knew they
would soon be exercising, it is logical that they engaged in some amount
of imagery of this activity either before or upon arriving at class.
Such imagery may have elevated physiological arousal, thus reducing
tiredness. It is also noteworthy that the present sample employed
experienced exercisers who may even have a conditioned response based on
repeated exercise at a specific time of day. As Watson (2000) points
out, predictable patterns in affective fluctuations may arise from
consistent lifestyle factors. Because this group of individuals were
enrolled in the aerobics classes for several weeks before this research
began, it is possible that their affective pattern through the day had
already been influenced. Though these ideas are speculative, they are
logically appealing and warrant further investigation.
Considering the other observed difference between daily mean affect
and preexercise affect for the tension subscale, present results follow
the non-significant trend evidenced by Petruzzello (1995, Study 1 &
2) who found participants reported slightly higher levels of state
anxiety outside the laboratory than before exercise in the laboratory.
This finding, considering Pertruzzello's (1995), albeit weak
(non-significant), corroboration is interesting. The tension mood
dimension is little affected by time of day, allowing greater clarity of
interpretation than that for tiredness in the present investigation.
Nonetheless, tension is expected to be highest at mid-morning compared
to other times of the day (Thayer, 1987b). It is somewhat possible,
therefore, that because the majority of experience sampling points were
recorded near the midmorning (see Figure I) and the majority of
preexercise tension scores were recorded at 8:00am or 9:00am, that this
may account for some of the difference observed here. Previous
investigations examining changes in negative mood states through the
day, however, have generally shown relatively small variations over the
day (Watson, 2000), and it is probable that there are other reasons for
a difference of the magnitude observed here (ES = -0.62). Likely, it is
the case that people completed experience sampling moments at time
points when they were facing more significant challenge/threat than that
posed by a short aerobics class. Considering this possibility, it should
be noted that results may be quite different examining a less
experienced sample of exercisers (e.g., obese individuals, novice
exercisers) who may be more threatened by exercise participation than
the population represented by the present sample. While some may read
the present results and dismiss them as situational, we argue that this
is entirely the point. To get a true measure of the effect of exercise
on affect, it seems that we should consider participants' overall
affective patterns in addition to those exhibited before, during, and
after an exercise stimulus. In this manner, it should be possible to
better understand the nature of the effect of exercise on affective
states.
Considering the energy and calmness dimensions, though there were
no differences observed between daily mean affect and preexercise
affect, there were very clear benefits observed as a result of
participation in the bout of aerobic exercise. Energy was elevated at
the mid-point of exercise and remained elevated 15-min postexercise.
Calmness, on the other hand, was reduced at the mid-point of exercise
and then returned toward the preexercise baseline at 15-min
postexercise. This matches previous findings concerning exercise's
ability to enhance feelings of energy and temporarily influence feelings
of calmness (Ekkekakis et al., 2005; Thayer et al., 1993; Saklofske et
al., 1992).
While the present study suggests that preexercise affective states
are different from daily states, there are some important shortcomings
of the present study that should be noted. It is acknowledged that time
of day may influence affect, and this study did not account for diurnal effects. Next, while compliance to experience sampling procedures did
not appear to have a large impact on our findings (see footnote #3), our
compliance rate was low relative to other studies. While Hektner and
Csikszentmihalyi (2002) state that low response rates do not appear to
diminish ability of experience sampling to accurately depict content of
individuals' daily lives, future research may want to utilize more
experience sampling time points and take additional steps to ensure
compliance to experience sampling procedures. Finally, the findings of
the present research are limited in their generalizability as the
exercise session involved group activity and a healthy and active
university-aged sample. Findings may not be indicative of exercise
performed individually or in different populations.
The final limitation, that our sample included healthy and
experienced exercisers, is an important consideration for future
research. Ekkekakis and Petruzzello, in their 1999 review, suggest that
aerobic fitness may be an important moderator of the relationship
between exercise intensity and affective responses. Specifically, less
fit or less active individuals seems to exhibit a reduced affective
benefit to exercise at high intensities. Related to the present
investigation, it might be expected that unfit, inexperienced or obese
participants might experience greater levels of negative affect or less
positive affect before exercise in anticipation of the exercise bout
(thus, producing the opposite of the present findings) and the exercise
stimulus might heighten negative affect or reduce positive affect even
more during exercise. Yet, it is impossible to determine how such
expectancies may be influencing affective states for less experienced
exercisers as both the present research and Petruzzello (1995) used
healthy, university students to investigate the relation between daily
affect and exercise-related affect and to examine expectancy effects,
respectively. In Petruzzello's study, the exercise history and
fitness levels of participants is not thoroughly described, but we can
probably assume they were at least moderately fit and healthy as 20 male
in their early 20's served as subjects in this study (both Study 1
& 2 had similar samples).
In conclusion, this research highlights an important point in the
study of exercise and affect. Namely, researchers should carefully
consider what constitutes an effective "baseline" measure of
affect. Time of day, expectancies, and participants' fitness levels
are among variables that deserve greater attention when making these
judgments. While this research raises many questions, it does
nonetheless point out that affective responses to exercise, as measured
in many studies, may give only a portion of the picture reflecting how
exercise impacts how we feel.
References
Adan, A. (2005). Mood rhythmicity and individual differences. In A.
V. Clark (Ed.), Causes, role and influence of mood states (pp. 33-56).
Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.
Alliger, G. M., & Williams, K. J. (1993). Using
signal-contingent experience sampling methodology to study work in the
field: A discussion and illustration examining task perceptions and
mood. Personnel Psychology, 46, 525-549.
Arent, S. M., Landers, D. M., & Etnier, J. L. (2000). The
effects of exercise on mood in the elderly: A meta-analytic review.
Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 8, 407-430.
Barrett, D. J., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2003). The experience
sampling program [computer software and manual]. Retrieved from
http://www2.bc.edu/~barretli/esp/index.html.
Bartholomew, J. B. (1999). The effect of resistance exercise on
manipulated preexercise mood states for male exercisers. Journal of
Sport and Exercise Psychology, 21, 39-51.
Biddle, S. J. H. (2000). Emotion, mood and physical activity. In S.
J. H. Biddle, K. R., Fox, & S. H. Boutcher (Eds.), Physical activity
and psychological well-being (pp. 63-88). Routledge: London, UK.
Bixby, W. R., Spalding, T. W., & Hatfield, B. D. (2001).
Temporal dynamics and dimensional specificity of the affective response
to exercise of varying intensity: Differing pathways to a common
outcome. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 23, 171-190.
Borg, G. (1998). Borg's Perceived Exertion and Pain Scales.
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Publishers.
Breus, M. J., & O'Connor, E J. (1998). Exercise-induced
anxiolysis: A test of the time-out hypothesis in high-anxious females.
Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 30, 1107-1112.
Carroll, J. M., Yik, M. S. M., Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. E
(1999). On the psychometric principles of affect. Review of General
Psychology, 3, 14-22.
Chen, M. J., Fan, X., & Moe, S. J. (2002). Criterion-related
validity of the Borg ratings of perceived exertion scale in healthy
individuals: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20, 873-899.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and
reliability of the experience sampling method. Journal of Nervous and
Mental Disease, 174, 526-536.
Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., Germain, M., & Pastene, J. (1991).
Vegetative response during imagined movement is proportional to mental
effort. Behavioral Brain Research, 42, 1-5.
Diener, E., & Larsen, R. J. (1984). Temporal stability and
cross-situational consistency of affective, behavioral, and cognitive
responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 871-883.
Dunn A. L., Trivedi, M. H., Kampert, J.B., Clark, C. G., &
Chambliss, H.O. (2005). Exercise treatment for depression: Efficacy and
dose response. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 28, 1-8.
Ekkekakis, P., Hall, E. E., & Petruzzello, S. J. (2005).
Evaluation of the circumplex structure of the Activation Deactivation
Adjective Check List before and after a short walk. Psychology of Sport
& Exercise, 6, 83-101.
Ekkekakis, P., & Petruzzello, S. J. (1999). Acute aerobic
exercise and affect: Current status, problems, and prospects regarding
dose response. Sports Medicine, 28(5), 337-374.
Ekkekakis, P., & Petruzzello, S. J. (2002). Analysis of the
affect measurement conundrum in exercise psychology: IV. A conceptual
case for the affect circumplex. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 3,
35-63.
Emmons, R. A., & King, L. A. (1989). Personal striving
differentiation and affective reactivity. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 56, 478-484.
Feldman Barrett, L., & Barrett, D. J. (2001). An introduction
to computerized experience sampling in psychology. Social Science
Computer Review, 19, 175-185.
Gauvin, L., & Rejeski, W. J. (1993). The Exercise-Induced
Feeling inventory: Development and initial validation. Journal of Sport
and Exercise Psychology, 15, 403-423.
Gauvin, L., Rejeski, W. J., & Norris, J. L. (1996). The impact
of acute physical activity on feeling states and affect of women in a
naturalistic setting. Health Psychology, 15, 391-397.
Giacobbi, P. R., Jr., Hausenblas, H. A., & Frye, N. E. (2005).
A naturalistic assessment of the relationship between personality, daily
life events, leisure-time exercise, and mood. Psychology of Sports and
Exercise, 6, 67-81.
Godin G,, & Shephard, R. (1985). A simple method to assess
exercise behaviour in the community. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport
Science, 10, 141-146.
Hektner, J. M., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). The experience
sampling method: Measuring the context and content of lives. In R. B.
Bechtel & A. Churchman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology
(pp. 233-243). New York: Wiley.
Kennedy, M. M., & Newton, M. (1997). Effect of exercise
intensity on mood in step aerobics. Journal of Sports Medicine and
Physical Fitness, 37, 200-204.
Landers, D. M., & Petruzzello, S. J. (1994). Physical activity,
fitness, and anxiety. In C. Bouchard, R. J. Shephard, & T. Stephens
(Eds.), Physical activity, fitness and health: International proceedings
and consensus statement (pp. 868-882). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Lutz, R. S., Lochbaum, M., & Turnbow, K. (2003). The role of
autonomy in postexercise affect responding. Journal of Sport Behavior,
26, 137-154.
Morgan, W. P. (1997) Methodological considerations. In W. P. Morgan
(Ed.), Physical activity and mental health. Washington, DC: Taylor &
Francis.
Noble, B. J., & Robertson, R. J. (1996). Perceived exertion.
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
O'Connor P. J., Petruzzello, S. J., Kubitz, K. A., &
Robinson, T. L. (1995). Anxiety responses to maximal exercise testing.
British Journal of Sports Medicine, 29, 97-102.
Penner, L. A., Shiffman, S., Paty, J. A., & Fritzsche, B. A.
(1994). Individual differences in intraperson variability in mood.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 712-721.
Petruzzello, S. J. (1995). Anxiety reduction following exercise:
Methodological artifact or "real" phenomenon? Journal of Sport
and Exercise Psychology, 17, 105-111.
Petruzzello, S. J., Jones, A. C., & Tate, A. K. (1997).
Affective responses to acute exercise: A test of opponent process theory. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 37, 205-212.
Pollock, M. L., & Wilmore, J. H. (1990) Exercise in health and
disease: Evaluation and prescription for prevention and rehabilitation (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders Company.
Purcell, A. T. (1982). The structure of activation and emotion.
Multivariate Behavioural Research, 17, 221-251.
Raglin, J. S. (1997). Anxiolytic effects of physical activity. In
W. P. Morgan (Ed.), Physical activity and mental health. Washington, DC:
Taylor & Francis.
Raglin J. S., & Wilson, M. (1996). State anxiety following 20
minutes of bicycle ergometer exercise at selected intensities.
International Journal of Sports Medicine, 17, 467-471.
Rejeski, W. J., Gauvin, L., Hobson, M. L., &Norris, J. L.
(1995). Effects of baseline responses, in-task feelings and duration of
physical activity on exercise-induced feelings states. Health
Psychology, 14, 350-359.
Saklofske, D. H., Blomme, G., & Kelly, J. W. (1992). The
effects of exercise and relaxation on energetic and tense arousal.
Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 623-625.
Scollon, C. N., Kim-Prieto, C., & Diener, E. (2003). Experience
sampling : Promises and pitfalls, strengths and weaknesses. Journal of
Happiness Studies, 4, 5-34.
Scollon, C. N., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Diener, R.
(2005). An experience sampling and cross-cultural investigation of the
relation between pleasant and unpleasant affect. Cognition and Emotion,
19, 27-52.
Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety
Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Swendsen, J. D. (1998). The Helplessness-Hopelessness Theory and
daily mood experience: An idiographic and cross-situational perspective.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1398-1408.
Stone, A. A., & Shiffman, S. (2002). Capturing momentary,
self-report data: A proposal for reporting guidelines. Annals of
Behavioral Medicine, 24, 236-43.
Thayer, R. E. (1978). Toward a psychological theory of
multidimensional activation (arousal). Motivation and Emotion, 2, 1-34.
Thayer, R. E. (1986). Activation-Deactivation Adjective Check List
: Current overview and structural analysis. Psychological Reports, 58,
607-614.
Thayer, R. E. (1987a). Energy, tiredness and tension effects of a
sugar snack vs. moderate exercise. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 52, 119-125.
Thayer, R. E. (1987b). Problem perception, optimism, and related
states as a function of time of day (diurnal rhythm), and moderate
exercise: Two arousal states in interaction. Motivation and Emotion, 11,
19-36.
Thayer, R. E. (1989). The biopsychology of mood and arousal. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Thayer, R. E., Peters, D. P., Takahashi, P. J., &
Birkhead-Flight, A. M. (1993). Mood and behavior (smoking and sugar
snacking) following moderate exercise: A partial test of self-regulation
theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 97-104.
Thayer, R. E. Takahashi, P. J., & Pauli, J.A. (1988).
Multidimensional arousal states, diurnal rhythms, cognitive and social
processes, and extraversion. Personality and Individual Differences, 9,
15-24.
Updegraff, J.A., Gable, S.L, & Taylor, S.E. (2004). What makes
experiences satisfying? The interaction of dispositional motivations and
emotion in cognitive well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 86, 496-504.
Van Eck, M., Nicolson, N. A., & Berkhof, J. (1998). Effects of
stressful daily events on mood states: Relationship to global perceived
stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1572-1585.
Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219-235.
Yip, T. (2005). Sources of situational variation in ethnic identity
and psychological well-being: A Palm Pilot study of Chinese American students. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1603-1616.
(1) In Van Eck el al. (1988), participants were required to achieve
a 40% response rate to be included in the analysis. Five (of 95)
participants did not meet this criterion and are not reflected in the
mean response rate reported here.
(2) Yip (2005) used a 40% response rate cut-off criterion, Five (of
67) participants did not meet this standard and are not reflected in
this mean value
(3) It is also acknowledged that other cut-off values could be
used, and it should be noted that exploratory MANOVA and follow-up tests
to compare the mean of daily affect and exercise-related affect were
also performed using all participants (n = 36) and participants who
exhibited greater than 33% compliance (> 11 experience sampling
moments; n = 20) and results were comparable.
Rafer Lutz
Baylor University
Marc It. Lochbaum
Tyler Carson
Staci Jackson
Texas Tech University
Mike Greenwood
Baylor University
Allyn Byars
Angelo State University
Address Correspondence To: Rafer Lutz, Ph.D., Baylor University,
One Bear Place, #97313, Dept. of Health, Human Performance, and
Recreation, Waco, TX 76798-7313, Ph:(254) 7104024, E-mail:
Rafer_Lutz@baylor.edu
Table 1. Ad ACL Subscales Scores from Experience Sampling and
Exercise in a Class Setting.
Affective Dimension
Daily Affect Preexercise
M SD M SD ES
Energy 10.60 (a) (2.46) 10.00 (a) (3.93) -0.18
Tiredness 13.54 (a) (2.31) 9.91 (b) (4.24) -1.06
Tension 7.08 (a) (2.06) 5.87 (b) (1.87) -0.62
Calmness 11.10 (a) (1.74) 11.30 (a) (3.23) 0.08
Exercise Postexercise
M SD M SD
Energy 15.43 (b) (2.97) 14.91 (b) (2.98)
Tiredness 5.78 (c) (2.88) 6.09 (c) (2.94)
Tension 7.30 (a,b) (2.08) 6.91 (a,b) (1.93)
Calmness 7.70 (b) (2.72) 9.87 (a,c) (3.06)
Note. ESM = Experience Sampling Moments; ES = (M Preexercise - M
ESM)/pooled SD. All means within each affective dimension not
sharing the same superscript letter differ significantly (p <.05).