Examining reactions to the Dale Earnhardt crash: the importance of identification with NASCAR drivers.
Wann, Daniel L. ; Waddill, Paula J.
Social scientists have been interested in spectators'
evaluations of sporting events for many years. Although researchers have
investigated evaluations of and reactions to a number of situations,
such as reading about one's team in the sports section of a
newspaper (Wann & Branscombe, 1992) and the closing of a team's
stadium (Trujillo & Krizek, 1994), most examinations have focused on
responses to a team's performance. That research indicates that
fans tend to be biased in their analyses. For instance, researchers have
found that fans' attributions tend to be self-serving as they
internalize team successes (e.g., "we won because of our
talent") and externalize team failures (e.g., "we lost because
of poor officiating," see Wann & Dolan, 1994a; Wann &
Schrader, 2000; Warm & Wilson, 2001). In addition, fans are often
positively biased in their evaluations of the team's past and
future performances, such as recalling more victories from past seasons
than actually occurred (Wann, 1994; Wann & Dolan, 1994b).
Researchers have found that spectators with a high degree of
identification with the team (i.e., fans who feel a strong psychological
connection to the team, see Warm & Branscombe, 1993) are
particularly likely to report biased evaluations.
However, a limitation of past research on the evaluations and
analyses of sport fans concerns the fact that, to date, investigators
have focused on reactions to team sport events (e.g., wins and losses).
Researchers have yet to sufficiently examine the reactions of fans
following individual player sports (e.g., golf, gymnastics, tennis,
etc.). Such was the focus of the current investigation. Rather than
focusing on the reactions of fans after their favorite player had
succeeded or failed, however, we were interested in reactions to a
potentially more meaningful event: the crash and death of professional
racecar driver Dale Earnhardt, Sr.. Earnhardt died on February 18,2001
after his car was hit by another and slid into the wall on the last turn
of the last lap of the Daytona 500.
Because previous research had yet to examine spectator reactions to
such events, and because a theory of such reactions was not available,
we used the disposition theory of sport spectatorship to guide our
predictions. Developed by Zillmann, Bryant, and Sapolsky (1989),
disposition theory argues that fans gain enjoyment from witnessing two
events: watching their team perform well and watching a rival team
perform poorly. Thus, the greatest amount of enjoyment should occur when
a favored team defeats a despised rival. Disposition theory argues
further that a fan's disposition toward a favorite team and that
team's rivals will impact the intensity of affect felt subsequent
to a competitive event. Specifically, enjoyment from watching a favorite
team perform well should increase with positive sentiments toward the
team while enjoyment from watching a rival team lose is expected to
increase as sentiments toward that team decrease. Researchers have found
strong empirical support for disposition theory (Madrigal, 1995;
Sapolsky, 1980; Zillmann et al., 1989; Zillmann & Paulas, 1993). For
instance, Wann, Dolan, McGeorge, and Allison (1994) asked college
students to report their level of identification (i.e., disposition, see
Bryant & Raney, 2000) with their university's men's team
prior to witnessing the team win or lose a regular season contest.
Subsequent to the games, the participants were asked to complete a
questionnaire assessing their affective state. Consistent with
disposition theory, highly identified fans (i.e., those with positive
sentiments toward the team) reported strong negative affect after
watching their team lose and a rival succeed and positive affect after
watching their team win and a rival lose.
With respect to evaluations of and reactions to the Earnhardt crash
and death, the following hypotheses were generated using disposition
theory. First, it was predicted that fans of Earnhardt would be highly
sympathetic in their reactions to the crash and that their responses
would indicate that the event was quite painful to them. Conversely, we
expected racing fans whose favorite driver was someone other than
Earnhardt to be rather unsympathetic in their evaluations of the event.
Certainly, we did not expect fans of other drivers to feel happy in
response to Earnhardt's death. Rather, we were interested in
testing the possibility that these persons would trivialize the crash.
For instance, we expected fans of other drivers to be more likely to
blame Earnhardt for the crash or to indicate that auto racing is simply
a dangerous sport and these things happen from time to time. With
respect to Earnhardt fans, we expected to find more reactions that
indicated emotions like shock and sadness, and more perceptions that the
event was a tragedy.
Recall from our earlier discussion that disposition theory predicts
that the intensity of a fan's reaction to a sporting event will be
influenced by his or her identification with the teams involved.
Applying this logic to reactions to the Earnhardt crash, we therefore
expected that high levels of identification with Earnhardt or another
driver would magnify the effects noted above. Specifically, it was
hypothesized that, compared to Earnhardt fans with a low level of
identification, those with a high level identification with this driver
would be particularly likely to report painful evaluations of the crash
and less likely to report unsympathetic responses (i.e., to trivialize
the event). An opposite pattern of effects was expected for persons
whose favorite driver was someone other than Earnhardt. Among those
fans, we predicted that unsympathetic reactions would be especially
prominent among highly identified fans while those same fans were
expected to report fewer painful reactions.
Method
Participants
The sample contained 234 (84 male; 150 female) college students
earning extra course credit in exchange for their participation (most
students were enrolled in introductory classes). While this may seem to
be a large percentage of female participants (64%), this mirrors the
percentages of male and female students at the university in which
testing took place. Further, research indicates that approximately 40%
of NASCAR television viewers are female and that this number is rising
(McGinnis, Chun, & McQuillan, 2003; Wiessman, 1999). The
participants had a mean age of 20.75 years (SD = 3.74, range = 18 to
48).
Materials and Procedure
Upon providing their consent, participants (tested in groups)
completed a questionnaire packet containing five sections. Section one
asked respondents to complete demographic items assessing age and
gender. Section two contained the Sport Fandom Questionnaire (SFQ), a
reliable and valid measure assessing one's level of interest in
sport as a fan (Wann, in press). The SFQ contains five Likert-scale
items with anchors ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 8 (strongly
agree). Thus, higher numbers indicate greater levels of fandom. Although
the SFQ was developed to assess general sport fandom, the items were
altered slightly for use in the current study to allow for an assessment
of auto racing fandom. A sample item from this adapted scale read,
"Being an auto racing fan is very important to me." Section
three asked participants to provide their open-ended reactions to the
fatal crash involving Dale Earnhardt, which occurred February 18,2001
during the Daytona 500 (testing occurred between six and twelve months
subsequent to the crash). Specifically, subjects were asked, "What
are your thoughts and emotions about the death of racecar driver Date
Earnhardt? That is, how do you feel about and how would you analyze his
death?" Respondents were also told that there were no right or
wrong answers and that they were to be complete in their responses.
The fourth section contained two forced-choice items assessing
responses to the crash. First, subjects were asked, "In your view,
how tragic was the death of racecar driver Dale Earnhardt?"
Responses to this item ranged from 1 (not at all tragic) to 9 (extremely
tragic). For the second item, participants read, "In your view,
which of the following statements best summarizes your reaction and
analysis of Dale Earnhardt's crash and death?" The five
choices to this item were:
a) Any way you look at it, Earnhardt's death was tragic. His
death is a huge loss for the sport of auto racing and sport in general.
b) Although Earnhardt's death was certainly tragic, as a
professional racecar driver, he certainly knows the risks involved.
Although unfortunate, accidents and injury are simply a part of this
sport.
c) Although Earnhardt's death was tragic, the fact is the
crash was his fault. Thus, he is generally responsible for his own
death.
d) Individuals who choose to become professional racecar drivers
must be "nuts." Why else would someone voluntarily do
something so dangerous?
e) None of the above--I have no reaction and/or analysis of the
Earnhardt crash. Respondents were to circle which of the aforementioned responses best matched theft feelings.
The fifth and final section of the questionnaire packet contained
two versions of the Sport Spectator Identification Scale (SSIS; Wann
& Branscombe, 1993). The SSIS contains 7 Likert-scale items with
response options ranging from 1 (low) to 8 (high). Thus, higher numbers
represented greater levels of identification. The SSIS has been used in
a number of studies involving sport fans and has strong reliability and
validity (Wann & Branscombe, 1993; Wann, Melnick, Russell, &
Pease, 2001). Subjects were to complete only one version of the SSIS.
The first version specifically targeted Dale Earnhardt (e.g., "How
important was being a fan of Dale Earnhardt to you?"). Subjects
read that if Dale Earnhardt was theft favorite professional racecar
driver, or if he had been theft favorite driver prior to his death, they
were to complete this version of the SSIS. Those participants whose
favorite driver was someone other than Earnhardt were to complete the
second version of the SSIS. Here, subjects were asked to listed theft
favorite driver (open-ended) and to target this person when completing
the scale [e.g., "How important is being a fan of your favorite
professional racecar driver (the person listed above) to you?"].
Subjects who did not have a favorite racecar driver were instructed to
leave both versions blank. After completing the questionnaire packet
(20-30 minutes), the participants were debriefed and excused from the
testing session.
Results
Preliminary analyses
The seven items comprising the SSIS targeting Earnhardt and the
seven item comprising the SSIS targeting a different favorite driver
were combined to form two separate indices of driver identification
(Cronbach's alpha = .95 and .94, respectively). The five items
comprising the SFQ were combined to form a single index of racing fandom
(alpha = .93). Based on theft responses to the two versions of the SSIS,
participants were classified into three groups: nonfans (i.e., those who
left both forms of the SSIS blank, thus indicating that they did not
have a favorite driver, n = 155; 41 male, 114 female), (1) Earnhardt
fans (i.e., those who completed the SSIS targeting Earnhardt, thus
indicating that he was theft favorite racecar driver, n = 36; 23 male,
13 female), and other fans (i.e., those who completed the SSIS targeting
a driver other than Earnhardt, thus indicating that they had a different
favorite driver, n = 43; 20 male, 23 female).
A comparison of SSIS scores for those whose favorite driver was
Earnhardt (M = 30.58, SD = 12.90) and those whose favorite driver was
someone else (M = 25.74, SD = 13.93) revealed that the two groups did
not differ significantly in their level of identification with the
target drivers, F(1,77) = 2.53, p >. 10. Scores on the SFQ were also
examined by group. A one-way ANOVA revealed a significant difference in
auto racing fandom, F(1,231) = 38.68, p <.001. Post hoc analyses
(Newman-Keuls) indicated that persons in the Earnhardt fan (M = 15.33,
SD = 9.00) and other driver fan (M = 14.58, SD = 9.13) groups did not
differ in the level of racing fandom. However, each of these groups
scored significantly higher on the SFQ than persons in the nonfan group
(M = 7.21, SD = 4.41).
A final series of preliminary analyses involved using scores on the
two versions of the SSIS to construct groups of fans high or low in
identification with either Earnhardt or a different driver. Frequencies
of SSIS scores for each version were calculated and a median split was
then use to develop the groups. With respect to Earnhardt fans, 18
persons were placed into the low identification group (MSSIS score =
19.50, SD = 6.10, range = 7 to 29) and 18 were placed into the high
identification group (MSSIS score = 41.67, SD = 6.73, range = 30 to 53).
An ANOVA confirmed that the high identification group was indeed higher
in driver identification than the low identification group, F(1, 34) =
107.21, p < .001. In addition, a separate ANOVA indicated that the
high identification group reported higher levels of general racecar
fandom (M = 19.28, SD = 10.45) than those in the low identification
group (M = 11.39, SD = 4.95), F(1,34) = 8.38, p < .01. As for
participants whose favorite driver was someone other than Earnhardt, 20
subjects were placed into the low identification group (MSSIS score =
13.35, SD = 5.38, range = 7 to 23) and 23 were placed into the high
identification group (MSSIS score = 36.48, SD = 8.66, range = 24 to 49).
An ANOVA confirmed that the high identification group was indeed higher
in driver identification than the low identification group, F(1,41) =
106.63, p <.001. In addition, a separate ANOVA indicated that the
high identification group reported higher levels of general racecar
fandom (M = 19.70, SD = 9.03) than those in the low identification group
(M = 8.70, SD = 4.68), F(1,41) = 24.01, p < .001.
Analysis of open-ended item
The first set of analyses designed to test the predictions
generated by disposition theory involved coding the participants'
responses to the open-ended item requesting their thoughts and emotions
about the death of racecar driver Dale Earnhardt (coders were blind to
the participants' driver group). The number of independent coded
responses listed by the respondents ranged from a low of one to a high
of four. A total of 296 reactions were listed by 155 subjects in the
nonfan group (M = 1.91 reactions per participant). The 36 participants
in the Earnhardt fan group listed a total of 67 reactions (M = 1.86)
while those in the other fan group listed a total of 74 reactions (M =
1.72).
The two independent raters judged and classified the responses,
agreeing on approximately 98 percent of the cases. All disagreements
were settled through discussion. Nine different categories of responses
were established. Four of these categories reflected negative (i.e.,
painful and sympathetic) reactions to the crash. They were: tragedy
(i.e., "it was a tragedy," "it was horrible"),
sadness (i.e., "it was a sad thing that happened," "I
felt sad"), shock (i.e., "I was in shock," "I
refused to believe it"), and loss (i.e., "it was a great loss
to racing," "a great athlete died"). Four other
categories reflected unsympathetic (i.e., trivializing) reactions to the
crash. These reactions were: blame (i.e., "it was his fault,"
"he should have driven better"), thought of someone else
(i.e., "I thought about his family," "I thought about my
friends who are fans"), indifference (i.e., "I have no
reaction," "none"), and dangerous sport (i.e., "it
is a dangerous sport," "he knew the risks"). The final
category was an other category containing idiosyncratic and unusual
responses that did not fit into the other eight categories.
The percentage of fans in each group listing each type of response
was then calculated. These percentages appear in Table 1. For instance,
with respect to the tragedy category, 56 of the total (i.e., 296)
responses from those in the nonfan group were classified into this
category. Thus, 18.9 percent of the responses listed by these persons
reflected the belief that the event was tragic. Similarly, 14 of the
total responses listed by the Earnhardt fan group were placed into the
tragedy category, resulting in a percentage of 20.9. Finally, 8 of the
74 responses listed by fans in the other fan condition were categorized as reflecting the tragedy group. Consequently, this group received a
score of 10.8 percent for that category.
Overall, 46.6 percent of the responses offered by subjects in the
nonfan group reflected a painful/sympathetic response to the crash (see
Table 1). Similarly, 50.1 percent of the responses by persons with a
different favorite driver reflected pain. However, for participants in
the Earnhardt fan group, 70.2 percent of the responses were in one of
the four pain categories. With respect to unsympathetic, trivializing
reactions, 45.0 of the responses of nonfans and 43.4 percent of the
responses by persons in the other fan group were classified into this
category. Only 24.0 percent of the responses by Earnhardt fans reflected
a lack of concern for Earnhardt and the crash. As an initial test of the
disposition theory-generated hypothesis that fans of other racecar
drivers would be relatively unsympathetic about the Earnhardt crash,
percentages of unsympathetic responses for the three groups were
compared using tests of proportions. The first series of analyses
revealed that, as expected, responses of fans of other drivers were
indeed less sympathetic than were the responses of Earnhardt fans, z =
2.43, p < .05. Similarly, responses of nonfans were more often
categorized as unsympathetic than were the reactions of Earnhardt fans,
z = 3.18, p < .01. The lack of sympathy expressed by fans of other
drivers and nonfans did not differ significantly, z = 0.25, p > .50.
A second series of analyses examined frequencies of painful responses.
(2) This analysis indicated that, as expected Earnhardt fans were more
sympathetic toward the crash that fans of other drivers, z = 2.42, p
< .05. Responses of nonfans were also less likely to be sympathetic
than reactions of Earnhardt fans, z = 3.47, p < .001. Similar to the
analysis on unsympathetic responses, nonfans and fans of other drivers
did not differ in the frequency of reporting painful reactions, z =
0.54, p > .50.
Although the above analyses provided initial support for
disposition theory, a more complete test needed to consider the level of
identification the Earnhardt and other driver fans felt for their
favorite competitors. That is, not only were fans of other drivers
expected to be less sympathetic about the crash, we also predicted that
those fans with a strong disposition (i.e., degree of identification)
toward other drivers would be particularly unsympathetic. Similarly, not
only were Earnhardt fans expected to report a particularly high level of
painful responses, we predicted that highly identified Earnhardt fans
would report the greatest proportion of these reactions. To test these
predictions, comparisons of open-ended responses were made between fans
with high and low levels of identification with their favorite driver.
(3) With respect to persons in the Earnhardt fan group, as expected,
persons with a high level of identification with Earnhardt were
especially likely to report painful reactions and unlikely to report
unsympathetic responses. An examination of Table 2 reveals that the
proportions were consistent with expectations. However, tests of
proportions also revealed that, although the difference in unsympathetic
responses between those high (.121) and low (.354) in identification was
significant, z = 2.24, p < .05, the difference in painful responses
between highly identified persons (.788) and fans with a low level of
identification (.618) was only marginally different, z = 1.55, p = .12.
Similar comparisons were computed for persons with a different
favorite driver. An examination of Table 2 reveals that not only was the
hypothesis not supported, the pattern of proportions was directly
opposite of predictions. That is, rather than reporting a particularly
high level of unsympathetic reactions, fans with a high level of
identification with a different driver (.304) were actually less likely
to report such reactions than persons with low levels of identification
(.536), z = 2.00, p < .05. Also contrary to expectations, the highly
identified fans of other drivers were more likely to list painful
responses (.607) than low identified persons in this group (.414),
although this difference was only approached statistical significance, z
= 1.65, p =.10.
Analysis of forced-choice items
Tragedy of event. The next series of analyses involved examinations
of the two forced-choice items. First, responses to the item assessing
perceptions of tragedy were examined using a one-way analysis of
variance (ANOVA). This ANOVA revealed a main effect for group, F(2, 231)
= 7.37, p < .001. Post hoc tests (Newman-Keuls) revealed that, as
expected, persons in the Earnhardt fan group (M = 7.89, SD = 1.04)
viewed the event as more tragic than persons in the other driver group
(M = 6.58, SD = 2.18). Earnhardt fans also viewed the crash as more
tragic than nonfans (M = 6.34, SD = 2.36). The nonfan and other fan
groups did not differ significantly in their perceptions of the event as
a tragedy.
To test the hypothesis that the perceptions of the crash would be
related to one's identification with the drivers, responses to the
tragedy item were analyzed by comparing fans in the high and low
identification groups. For fans in the Earnhardt group, such a
comparison revealed that although highly identified fans (M = 8.06, SD =
0.94) tended to view the event as more tragic than low identified fans
(M = 7.72, SD = 1.13), this difference was not statistically
significant, F(1, 34) = 0.93, p > .30. For fans whose favorite driver
was someone other than Earnhardt, the results again contradicted
expectations. Highly identified persons in this group (M = 7.30, SD =
1.69) viewed the crash as significantly more tragic than persons with
low levels of identification (M = 5.75, SD = 2.43), F(1,41) = 6.07, p
< .05.
Summary of event. The next series of analyses examined
participants' responses to the item requesting their summary of the
crash. Although participants could choose from one of five reactions
(see above), very few subjects (< 8 percent) chose the items that
placed the blame on Earnhardt (option c), that said Earnhardt was
"nuts" (d), or that reported no reaction (e). Because the
number of these "other" reactions was so small (18 out of
234), participants choosing one of these three categories were not
included in the analyses. Frequencies of the remaining responses to the
summary item were examined using a 2 x 3 chi-square test of
independence. This analysis indicated significant frequency differences
in the matrix, [chi square](2, N = 216) = 18.10, p < .001. As shown
in Table 3 and consistent with expectations, participants in the other
driver group were less likely to view Earnhardt's death as
"tragic" and a "loss for the sport of auto racing and
sport in general" than were those in the Earnhardt fan group.
Rather, those in the other fan group tended to believe that Earnhardt
"knew the risks" and that "accidents and injury are
simply a part of this sport." Responses of those in the nonfan
condition were similar to those in the other driver condition.
Finally, responses to the summary item were examined by
identification groups using separate 2 x 2 chi-square tests (see Table
4). The analysis on persons in the Earnhardt fan group was marginally
significant, [chi square](1, N = 34) = 2.98, p = .08 (with Yates'
correction, [chi square] = 1.91, p = .17). An examination of the
responses indicates that they were in the predicted direction: highly
identified Earnhardt fans were, in comparison to those low in
identification, more likely to view Earnhardt's crash and death as
"tragic" and a "loss" but less likely to conclude
that "he knew risks" and that it was a "part of the
sport". As for persons in the other driver group, the chi-square
analysis revealed a statistically significant finding that was opposite
of expectations, [chi square](1, N = 41) = 4.04, p < .05 (with
Yates' correction, [chi square] = 2.74, p <.10). That is,
although we had expected highly identified fans of another driver to be
unsympathetic in their summary of the crash and death (i.e., to indicate
that Earnhardt knew what he was doing and that it is a part of the
sport), in fact these persons were more sympathetic than persons with
low levels of identification with another driver.
Discussion
The current study examined sport fans' evaluations of the
crash and death of professional racecar driver Dale Earnhardt. Using the
disposition theory of sport spectatorship (Zillmann et al., 1989) as a
framework for the development of specific hypotheses, it was predicted
that fans of Earnhardt would be more likely to express painful
evaluations (e.g., shock) and less likely to report unsympathetic
reactions (e.g., blame). Fans of drivers other than Earnhardt were
expected to report the opposite pattern. Further, these findings were
hypothesized to be most prominent among persons with high levels of
identification with their favorite driver. The results described above
presented a fairly clear pattern of effects, some of which were
consistent with predictions and some of which were quite unexpected. It
warrants mention that the unexpected finding involving identification
with rival drivers and evaluations of the crash should not be considered
as contrary to disposition theory as outlined by Zillmann and his
associates (1989). Disposition theory was not developed to explain
non-emotional reactions such as evaluations of non-outcome events.
Rather, we simply used disposition theory as a background from which to
generate testable hypotheses. Because this was merely a first step in
understanding such evaluations, future research is still needed on other
events (athlete injury, suspension, retirement, etc.), as is theory
development specifically related to events such as those examined here.
One finding that was consistent with predictions involved the
tendency for Earnhardt fans to report a great deal of sympathy and pain
in their evaluations of the event while fans of other drivers were more
likely to trivialize the event. This finding was noted on each of the
dependent measures (e.g., persons in the Earnhardt fan group were more
likely to list sympathetic evaluations, report that the event was
tragic, and summarize the event as a loss) and was found despite the
fact that Earnhardt fans and rival driver fans reported similar levels
of interest in auto racing and identification with their favorite
driver. Responses of persons in the other driver group were quite
similar to those in the nonfan group. It is important to note that
although these group differences did exist, there was still a
substantial amount of sympathy reported by fans of other drivers
(particularly among those with high levels of identification, see
below). For instance, consider the item assessing perceptions of the
degree to which the event was tragic. Although responses to this item
did indicate significant group differences in the predicted direction,
the mean score for fans of other drivers was well above the midpoint on
the scale. This illustrates an important point about the evaluations of
those persons analyzing the crash and death of a rival. As noted
earlier, we did not expect fans of different drivers to report joy in
their evaluations of the crash. Experiencing and reporting a positive
reaction to an event such as this would be clearly inappropriate and
violate many norms operating within sport and our culture at large.
Rather, we expected rival fans to downplay the event (e.g., concluding
that auto racing is a dangerous sport and Earnhardt knew the risks
involved). An example of this downplaying can be seen in the
participants' responses to the tragedy item. Fans of other drivers
were much more comfortable concluding that the crash was somewhat tragic
than highly tragic. Similarly, on the open-ended item requesting a
general analysis, no fans of another driver expressed happiness or
delight at the death of Earnhardt, nor were they expected to do so.
However, they were comfortable in reporting less sympathetic reactions
such as blaming Earnhardt or feeling general indifference.
The hypothesis that Earnhardt fans with a high level of
identification would be more likely than low identified fans to report
painful evaluations of the crash and less likely to report unsympathetic
responses garnered only modest support. Although the patterns of means
and proportions were unanimously in the expected direction, more often
than not statistical analyses revealed that the difference were only
marginally significant, at best. Thus, we are left to conclude that if
highly identified Earnhardt fans are more sympathetic in their
evaluations, the differences are small. Perhaps an explanation for this
finding can be found in previous research on the tendency for fans with
varying degrees of identification to bask in the glow of their
team's victories (Cialdini et al., 1976). Wann and Branscombe
(1990) have found that, among fans of sport teams, even fans with low to
moderate levels of identification will jump on the bandwagon and
increase their association with the team when they are performing well.
Even if the trait of team follower is only peripheral to their overall
social-identity, fans with low levels of identification will use the
team's successes as an opportunity to boost their self-image.
Perhaps somewhat of an opposite effect has been shown here. That is, if
only a modest link between the self and a sport entity is needed to
experience joy when a team (or player) performs well, it stands to
reason that a similarly modest connection is all that is required to
evaluate an event such as the Earnhardt crash as tragic and painful.
Perhaps the most intriguing finding, and one that was contrary to
expectations, concerns the tendency for highly identified fans of other
drivers to be more likely than less identified fans to offer sympathetic
evaluations and less likely to express trivializing summaries. In fact,
a close inspection of the data reveals a striking similarity in the
responses of those high in identification with a different driver and
persons with low levels of identification with Earnhardt. For instance,
as shown in Table 2, roughly 61.8 percent of the open-ended responses of
fans with a high degree of identification with other drivers were
classified as sympathetic, a total that is quite similar to the 60.7
percent of such responses reported by low identified Earnhardt fans.
Aplausible explanation for this finding is that the highly identified
other driver fans were able to empathize with the pain and loss felt by
the Earnhardt fans. Because they, too, had a favorite driver, they may
have understood how they would have felt had the crash happened to their
favorite. Consequently, they were more sympathetic in their evaluations
of the crash. Support for these assertions can be found in social
psychological research indicating that feelings of empathy toward
another often moderate reactions involving pain and suffering (see
Batson, 1991, 1995; Batson, Duncan, Ackerman, Buckley, & Birch,
1981). Because this is a post hoc explanation, future work is needed to
test its validity. Such research could be completed subsequent to a
similar event by assessing level of identification with the athletes in
question (i.e., the person involved and that athlete's rivals) as
well as levels of empathy for the victim.
References
Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruistic question." Toward a
social-psychological answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Batson, C. D. (1995). Prosocial motivation: Why do we help others?
In A. Tesser (Ed.), Advanced social psychology (pp. 333-382). New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Batson, C. D., Duncan, B., Ackerman, P., Buckley, T., & Birch,
K. (1981). Is empathetic emotion a source of altruistic motivation?
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 290-302.
Bryant, J., & Raney, A. A. (2000). Sport on the screen. In D.
Zillmann & P. Vorderer (Eds.), Media entertainment: The psychology
of its appeal (pp. 153-174). New York: Erlbaum.
Cialdini, R. B., Borden, R. J., Thome, A., Walker, M. R., Freeman,
S., & Sloan, L. R. (1976). Basking in reflected glory: Three
(football) field studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
34, 366-375.
Madrigal, R. (1995). Cognitive and affective determinants of fan
satisfaction with sporting event attendance. Journal of Leisure
Research, 27, 205-227.
MvGinnis, L., Chun, S., & McQuillan, J. (2003). A review of
gendered consumption in sport and leisure. Academy of Marketing Science
Review, 03 (5). Retrieved January 5, 2006, from
http://www.amsreview.org/articles/mcginnis05-2003.pdf
Sapolsky, B. S. (1980). The effect of spectator disposition and
suspense on the enjoyment of sport contests. International Journal of
Sport Psychology, 11, 1-10.
Trujillo, N., & Krizek, B. (1994). Emotionality in the stands
and on the field: Expressing self through baseball. Journal of Sport
& Social Issues, 18, 303-325.
Wann, D. L. (in press). Preliminary validation of a measure for
assessing identification as a sport fan: The Sport Fandom Questionnaire.
International Journal of Sport Management.
Warm, D. L. (1994). Biased evaluations of highly identified sport
spectators: A response to Hirt and Ryalls. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
79, 105-106.
Wann, D. L., & Branscombe, N. R. (1990). Die-hard and
fair-weather fans: Effects of identification on BIRGing and CORFing
tendencies. Journal of Sport and Social Issues, 14, 103-117.
Wann, D. L., & Branscombe, N. R. (1992). Emotional responses to
the sports page. Journal of Sport & Social Issues, 16, 49-64.
Wann, D. L., & Branscombe, N. R. (1993). Sports fans: Measuring
degree of identification with the team. International Journal of Sport
Psychology, 24, 1-17.
Wann, D. L., & Dolan, T. J. (1994a). Attributions of highly
identified sports spectators. Journal of Social Psychology, 134,
783-792.
Wann, D. L., & Dolan, T. J. (1994b). The influence of spectator
identification on the evaluation of the past, present, and future
performance of a sports team. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78, 547-552.
Wann, D. L., Dolan, T. J., McGeorge, K. K., & Allison, J. A.
(1994). Relationships between spectator identification and
spectators' perceptions of influence, spectators' emotions,
and competition outcome. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 16,
347-364.
Wann, D. L., Melnick, M. J., Russell, G. W., & Pease, D. G.
(2001). Sport fans: The psychology and social impact of spectators. New
York: Routledge Press.
Wann, D. L., & Schrader, M. P. (2000). Controllability and
stability in the self-serving attributions of sport spectators. Journal
of Social Psychology, 140, 160-168.
Wann, D. L., & Wilson, A. M. (2001). The relationship between
the sport team identification of basketball spectators and the number of
attributions generated to explain a team's performance.
International Sports Journal, 5, 43-50.
Weissman, R. X. (1999). The green flag is up: High-tech companies
are burning rubber to reach NASCAR fans. American Demographics, 21,
33-36.
Zillmann, D., Bryant, J., & Sapolsky, B. S. (1989). Enjoyment
from sports spectatorship. In J. H. Goldstein (Ed.), Sports, games, and
play: Social and psychological viewpoints (2nd ed., pp. 241-278).
Hillsdale, N J: Erlbaum.
Zillmann, D., & Paulas, P. B. (1993). Spectators: Reactions to
sports events and effects on athletic performance. In R. N. Singer, M.
Murphey, & L. K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport
psychology (pp. 600-619). New York: Macmillan.
Daniel L. Wann and Paula J. Waddill
Murray State University
Address Correspondence To: Daniel L. Wann, Department of
Psychology, Murray State University, Murray, KY 42071, email:
dan.wann@murraystate.edu.
(1) Although reactions of persons in the nonfan group were not
involved in the hypotheses, they are included in the basic analyses for
comparison purposes.
(2) Although the analyses of unsympathetic and painful responses
may seem redundant, the inclusion of an "other" category in
the classification scheme rendered separate analyses of proportions
necessary (i.e., the sum of the sympathetic and painful responses did
equal 100 percent due to the "other" responses).
(3) High and low identified fans in both groups reported a similar
number of total reactions. Specifically, among persons in the Earnhardt
group, highly identified fans listed a total of 33 responses while low
identified fans listed 34. As for persons in the other driver group,
highly identified fans listed 33 responses while low identified fans
listed 41.
Table 1
Percent of Responses Listed for Each Category by Group.
Earnhardt Other
Category Nonfans Fans Driver Fans
Painful reactions
Tragedy 18.9 20.9 10.8
Sadness 21.3 22.4 28.4
Shock 2.0 16.4 4.1
Loss 4.4 10.5 6.8
Total painful 46.6 70.2 50.1
Unsympathetic reactions
Blame 5.1 6.0 6.8
Thoughts 13.9 9.0 9.5
Indifference 11.8 1.5 9.5
Dangerous sport 14.2 7.5 17.6
Total unsympathetic 45.0 24.0 43.4
Other 8.5 6.0 6.8
Note: Percentages may not sum to 100 due to rounding.
Table 2
Percent of Responses Listed for Each Category by Persons with High
and Low Levels of Identification with Their Favorite Driver.
Earnhardt Fans Other Driver Fans
Category High ID Low ID High ID Low ID
Painful reactions
Tragedy 21.2 20.6 6.1 14.6
Sadness 24.2 20.6 36.4 22.0
Shock 18.2 14.7 6.1 2.4
Loss 15.2 5.9 12.1 2.4
Total painful 78.8 61.8 60.7 41.4
Unsympathetic reactions
Blame 6.1 5.9 6.1 7.3
Thoughts 3.0 14.7 3.0 14.6
Indifference 0.0 3.0 6.1 12.2
Dangerous sport 3.0 11.8 15.2 19.5
Total unsympathetic 12.1 35.4 30.4 53.6
Other 9.1 2.9 9.1 4.9
Note: Percentages may not sum to 100 due to rounding.
Table 3
Frequencies (and Percentages) of Responses to Summary Item by Group
Group
Earnhardt Other
Reaction Nonfans Fans Driver Fans
Death was "tragic" 28 19 11
and "loss" (19.9) (55.9) (26.8)
"He knew risks" and 113 15 30
"part of sport" (80.1) (44.1) (73.2)
Note: Percentages (appearing in parentheses)
may not sum to 100 due to rounding.
Table 4
Frequencies (and Percentages) of Responses to Summary Item by Group
Earnhardt Fans Other Driver Fans
Reaction High ID Low ID High ID Low ID
Death was "tragic" 12 7 9 2
and "loss" (70.5) (41.2) (39.1) (11.1)
"He knew risks" and 5 10 14 16
"part of sport" (29.4) (58.8) (60.9) (88.9)
Note: Percentages (appearing in parentheses)
may not sum to 100 due to rounding.