Goal orientation and sensation seeking in relation to optimal mood states among skateboarders.
Boyd, Michael P. ; Kim, Mi-Sook
The popularity of extreme sports in this country has produced a
groundswell of interest over recent years. More than 12 million physical
activity enthusiasts participate annually in the extreme sport of
skateboarding (American Sports Data Inc., 2001). Part of the allure of
skateboarding may be its individualized, freestyle nature, where little
more is demanded than a flat surface, the self-determination to master a
trick, and an inherent desire to engage in sensation seeking behavior.
Sport psychologists have long recognized the importance of psychological
mood states to participation in sport. The present study is designed to
examine whether goal orientations and sensation seeking are related to
psychological mood states among a sample of skateboarders.
Two theoretical models relevant to participation in extreme sport
are achievement goal theory and sensation seeking behavior. Achievement
goal theory emphasizes the relevance of goal orientations, dispositions
to utilize a differentiated or undifferentiated conception of ability,
that are linked to cognitive perceptions, affective responses, and
achievement behavior (Ames, 1992; Dweck, 1986; Dweck & Leggett,
1988; Nicholls, 1984, 1989). Goal orientations are central to the manner
in which individuals define success and judge personal competence, both
critical determinants of motivation (Duda, 2001). Task orientation
represents a self-referenced conception of ability, where perceived
success stems from personal improvement, performing one's best, and
task mastery (Roberts, 2001). Ego orientation reflects an
other-referenced conception of ability and a preoccupation to evaluate
personal ability in relation to the performance of others, as perceived
success is based upon the demonstration of superior ability and
outperforming others (Roberts, 2001). Task and ego orientations are
theoretically orthogonal in nature as an individual may express high or
low levels on either goal orientation (Duda & Whitehead, 1998).
Cognitive-affective and behavioral correlates of goal orientations
in physical activity have been well-documented (Duda & Whitehead,
1998; Roberts, 2001). A task orientation is associated to task mastery,
the beliefs that both effort and hard work eventually lead to success
and that sport should encourage a physically active lifestyle, and to
moderate-to-vigorous physical exercise (Duda, 1989; Kimiecik, Horn,
& Shurin, 1996; Roberts,
Treasure, & Kavassanu, 1996; Spray, Biddle, & Fox, 1999). Ego
orientation, however, is related to the notion that sport is a means to
enhance social status, and to the beliefs that success is the result of
ability, deceptive tactics, and external factors (Duda, 1989; Roberts et
al., 1996; Spray et al., 1999). Dunn and Dunn (1999) demonstrated that
ego-oriented participants endorsed the use of aggressive behavior and
anger in sport including verbal and physical intimidation, and
intentional injurious behavior. Task-oriented individuals, however,
expressed higher levels of sportspersonship such as greater respect for
the rules and social conventions.
Goal orientations are also associated to affective reactions in the
physical domain. Task orientation has been linked to positive affective
reactions including intrinsic motivation, enjoyment,
satisfaction/interest, and perceptions of individual improvement
(Balaguer, Duda, Atienza, & Mayo, 2002; Boyd Weinmann, & Yin,
2002; Roberts, et al., 1996). Ego orientation, on the other hand,
corresponds to negative affection reactions such as cognitive anxiety
leading up to performance, and to both the intensity and direction of
cognitive and somatic anxiety (Hall & Kerr, 1997; Ntoumanis &
Biddle, 1998; White and Zellner, 1996). Conversely, tension/pressure,
cognitive anxiety, and worry prior to competition are negatively related
to task orientation (Boyd, et al., 2002; Hall & Kerr, 1997; White
& Zellner, 1996). Theoretically, such evidence suggests that a task
orientation may be positively associated with the mood state of vigor
and negatively related to the mood states of tension and anger.
Although a task orientation and effort to improve to a higher level
of skill proficiency may be a valuable asset in an extreme sport such as
skateboarding, a measure of sensation seeking may also be adaptive to
participation. Zuckerman (1994) outlines sensation seeking as, "a
trait defined by the need for varied, novel, complex ... sensations and
experiences, and the willingness to take physical, social ... risks for
the sake of such experience" (pg. 27). Roberti (2004) documents
behavioral correlates of sensation seeking such as high-risk sport
participation, biological correlates including lower cortisol responsiveness to acute anxiety, and personality traits related to
sensation seeking such as an openness to experience and high resistance
to both physical and psychological stressors. Sensation seeking is
traditionally measured using the Sensation Seeking Scale (Form V;
Zuckerman, Eysenek, & Eysenck, 1978) which yields four subscales
including experience seeking, disinhibition, boredom susceptibility, and
thrill and adventure seeking (TAS) subscales. The TAS subscale was
designed to measure sensation seeking in sport assessing the desire to
search out risky, adventurous sports, containing speed or danger.
Sensation seeking in sport significantly predicts degree of
involvement in extreme sports such as skateboarding, surfing, in-line
skating, snow skiing, and mountain biking (Schrader & Wann, 1999).
Individuals participating in the high-risk sports of hang-gliding, rock
climbing, skiing, scuba diving, and white water canoeing express higher
sensation seeking (TAS) scores than those who refrain from participation
or those who choose low-risk sports such as golf and tennis (Brevnik,
1996; Malkin & Rabinowitz, 1998; Wagner & Houlihan, 1994). Ball,
Famill, and Wangeman (1984) indicate that TAS subscale scores peak for
males 17 to 29 years of age then markedly drop off.
Sensation seeking in sport is also manifested in the reduction of
negative affective reactions such as emotional arousal and anxiety.
Smith, Ptacek, and Smoll (1992) find that sensation seeking leads to
qualitatively better management of anxiety, more freedom from worry, and
successful sport performance, including an ability to peak under
pressure. High sensation seekers evidently perceive physically risky
situations as less threatening, demonstrating blunted sympathetic
nervous system reactivity, subsequently leading to less fear, stress,
and anxiety (Franken, Gibson, & Rowland, 1992). High sensation
seekers also express lower cortisol release in response to stressors,
higher tolerance for aversive stimulation, and both lower emotional
arousal and irritability, than low sensation seekers (Netter, Henning,
& Roed, 1996). Evidence suggests that sensation seeking in sport may
be positively linked to the mood state of vigor, in the form of openness
to experience and participation in extreme sports, and inversely
associated to the mood states of tension and anger.
Mood states have been examined extensively in the sport psychology
literature (LeUnes & Berger, 2000). Successful, in relation to less
successful sport performance, is characterized by mood profiles
reflecting above average levels of vigor, and below average levels of
tension, anger, depression, fatigue, and confusion (Morgan 1985; Morgan
& Costill, 1996). This pattern of mood responses has come to be
known as the iceberg profile, documented recently among a sample of two
thousand athletes plotted against college norms (Terry & Lane,
2000).
The utility of mood states in predicting successful performance
outcome, however, has not been without controversy. In a meta-analysis,
Rowley, Landers, Kyllo, and Etnier (1995) found that mood states had
little value in predicting sport performance. However, Beedie, Terry,
& Lane (2000) contend, that use of a total mood profile for analysis
in that study, could have masked the direction of the six independent
mood measures. Furthermore, failure to tease out level of achievement,
from level of performance comparisons, may have also contributed to
insignificant results in that study. In response, Beedie, et al.,
(2000), also using meta-analysis, examined the magnitude of direction of
the six independent mood state measures and demonstrated specific
conditions under which mood states were indeed predictive of successful
sport performance. They found that although small mood effects existed
between levels of achievement (ie. experts vs. novice), small to
moderate pre-performance mood effects were demonstrated between
successful and less successful performance, from individuals of similar
achievement levels, all in the direction of Morgan's (1985) iceberg
profile.
Mood profiles also discriminate between successful and less
successful performance when self-referenced performance criteria are
used, such as percentage of personal best, which may be more sensitive
to mood fluctuations than objective performance criteria. For instance,
compared to a group of under-performers, bobsledders and rowers who
perceived that they had performed up to personal expectations, exhibited
pre-performance mood profiles consistent with the iceberg profile (Hall
& Terry, 1995; Terry, 1994). These effects were shown to be larger
for open than closed skills, and shorter than longer duration activities
(Terry, 1995). Skateboarding is an open sport of short duration where
participants potentially generate personal best goals.
Skateboarding also shares many characteristics of other sensation
seeking sports such as surfing or snowboarding that contain elements of
speed and danger. Theorists have not examined mood states linked to
successful performance among less traditional, yet contemporarily
popular thrill seeking sports, which are easily accessible to a large
segment of the population. Moreover, although sensation seeking has been
examined in relation to participation in high-risk sport, the utility of
the construct has not been studied in combination with other relevant
variables such as goal orientations. A task orientation, in combination
with sensation seeking, may be linked to mood states reflective of the
iceberg profile, known to be associated with successful performance. The
purpose of this study, therefore, is to examine the relationship between
goal orientations, sensation seeking, and psychological mood states
among skateboarders. It is predicted that a task orientation and
sensation seeking in sport will be positively related to the mood state
of vigor and negatively associated with the mood states of tension and
anger.
Method
Participants
In order to circumvent the inherent problem of obtaining parental
consent from minors, which was not circumstantially feasible due to lack
of parental accessibility, only skateboarders over 18 years of age were
solicited to participate in the study. Male skateboarders (N=68), age
18-28 (M=20.84, SD = 2.8), voluntarily participated anonymously in the
study. Ethnicity of the sample was Caucasian (69.1%), Asian (11.8%),
Hispanic (10.3%), African American (4.4%), and Other (4.4%). Although
qualitative assessment of skill level was not methodologically feasible,
average skateboarding experience of the sample was 7.66 years (SD =
4.0).
Materials
Goal orientation. Goal orientation was quantified using the Task
and Ego Orientation in Sport Questionnaire (TEOSQ; Duda, 1989), a
13-item instrument composed of two subscales designed to measure task
and ego orientation for sport and physical activity. The TEOSQ has been
used extensively among team sports and physical activities as diverse as
snow-skiing, aerobic exercise, track and field, recreational sport, and
exercise behavior, among others (Duda & Whitehead, 1998). Each item
is preceded by the phrase, "I feel most successful in sport when
...", answered on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly
disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The composite score for each subscale
was used as the unit of measurement. The TEOSQ has consistently
demonstrated acceptable psychometric properties including confirmatory
factorial validity, internal reliability (means of .79 and .81 for task
and ego orientation subscales, respectively), and concurrent and
predictive validity (Duda & Whitehead, 1998).
Sensation seeking. The Thrill and Adventure Seeking (TAS) subscale
of the Sensation Seeking Scale (Form V; Zuckerman et al., 1978) was
designed to assess sensation seeking in sport and physical activity. The
TAS contains 10 items that reflect participation in high-risk sports
such as surfing, snow skiing, water skiing, mountain climbing, scuba
diving, and parachuting, among others. Skateboarding shares
characteristics, such as balance and speed, with similar sensation
seeking sports (eg. surfing, water skiing) that have been assessed with
the TAS subscale.
Respondents answered items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), in order to minimize response
format variation. The average score of all items for each individual was
used as the sensation seeking variable. Psychometric properties,
including superior factor loadings (.44-.76, males) and subscale
reliabilities (.72, .87, males) have been demonstrated for the TAS
(Ridgeway & Russell, 1980; Zuckerman et al., 1978).
Mood State. A shortened version of the Profile of Mood States (POMS; McNair, Lorr, & Droppleman, 1992) was used to quantify
enduring mood states. The instrument is a psychometrically valid,
abbreviated 30-item version of the original 65-item measure. LeUnes and
Burger (2000) contend there is no need to use the 65-item measure when a
shorter form with half as many items is completed in markedly less time,
especially when used in conjunction with other response measures. In
order to maintain consistency with the other measures, enduring mood
state was assessed in the context of the past week, as pre-performance
mood was not the variable of interest (Terry, 1995).
Participants were requested to, "describe how you have been
feeling during the past week including today", using a 5-point
Likert scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). The
instrument contains six subscales assessing tension-anxiety,
anger-hostility, fatigue-inertia, vigor-activity, depression-dejection
and confusion-bewilderment. Scores of each subscale were summed and used
as the unit of measurement. Acceptable psychometric properties of the
shortened form, including internal reliability of the various subscales
(.79-.91), have been reported among males (McNair et al., 1992).
Procedures
All skateboarders were approached for participation at one of two
locations in a major West Coast urban area where skateboarders
congregated daily for recreational activity and informal competition.
Those who agreed to participate were seated, given a clipboard and
pencil, and provided informed consent in an isolated area removed from
other skateboarders. Participants were then administered response
measures and completed the instruments anonymously, either individually
or in small groups. Instructions for each measure, as well as the first
four items of each subscale, were read aloud by the researcher adhering
to a reliable protocol. Skateboarders were requested to stop after
completing each measure and wait for further instruction. Participants
were also encouraged to answer all items in an honest manner and ask
questions for clarification whenever necessary. Upon completion,
skateboarders were offered two dollars for their participation.
Results
Internal Reliability of the Scales
The subscales contained within the TEOSQ, TAS, and the POMS
generally demonstrated acceptable internal reliability. The ego
orientation subscale contained in the TEOSQ, however, initially
exhibited borderline reliability ([alpha] = .67). One item ("I am
the only one who can do the play or skill") demonstrated an
item-total correlation of .22 suggesting this item did not assess ego
orientation within this sample. It makes theoretical sense that the item
was poorly responded to by participants, as the term "play" is
not part of the lexicon specific to skateboarding. With the item
deleted, reliability of the ego orientation subscale increased to a
desirable level of .70 (Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). Item-total
correlation of the remaining ego orientation items ranged from .35-.59.
Cronbach (1951) alpha of the task orientation subscale was .81.
Internal reliability of the thrill and adventure seeking subscale
(TAS) was .76. Reliability coefficients of the mood state subscales
contained in the POMS were .77 (tension), .81 (anger), .82 (fatigue),
.79 (vigor), .75 (depression), and .50 (confusion). The confusion
subscale demonstrated tentative reliability and was subsequently deleted
from further analysis.
All measured mood states demonstrated means ranging in difference
from .07-. 15 of a standard deviation from male college age norms
established for the shortened version of the POMS (McNair, et al.,
1992), and standard deviations ranging in difference from .70-10.0
percent of norms. Table 1 displays means, standard deviations, and
internal reliabilities for all measured variables.
Intercorrelations among Subscales
The interfactor correlation between task and ego orientation was
shown to be negligible (r = .10) confirming the orthogonal nature of the
two measures within a skateboarding population. Sensation seeking was
unrelated to both task and ego orientation. A cluster of significant
correlations emerged among the negative emotions of tension, anger,
fatigue, and depression ranging in magnitude from .47 to .69.
Correlation coefficients among all measures are displayed in Table 2.
Multivariate Relationships Among Variables
In order to examine the multivariate relationship between the
linear combination of variables consisting of goal orientations and
sensation seeking, and a second set of variables consisting of mood
states, a canonical correlation analysis was performed. The first set of
variables
entered were task orientation, ego orientation, and sensation
seeking. Mood state variables included tension, anger, fatigue, vigor,
and depression. Canonical loadings greater than .30 were retained for
meaningful interpretation (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001).
One significant canonical function emerged (Wilks' lambda =.
.643, F (15, 166)= 1.91, p < .025). The canonical correlation was .49
(R-square =.24). Table 3 contains canonical loadings, within set
variance accounted for by the canonical variates, and the canonical
correlation. Variables in the first set that loaded on the canonical
variate, in order of magnitude, were task orientation (.856) and
sensation seeking (.599). Ego orientation (.082) failed to reach the
cutoff criterion of .30 for meaningful contribution to the multivariate
relationship (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). The relevant mood state
variables retained for meaningful interpretation were vigor (.871),
tension (-.469), and anger (-.374). Fatigue and depression failed to
meaningfully contribute to the canonical correlation. Thus, canonical
variates revealed that task orientation and sensation seeking were
positively associated with the mood state of vigor and negatively
related to the mood states of tension and anger.
Discussion
The present study examined the relationship between goal
orientations/sensation seeking and psychological mood states among
moderately skilled skateboarders. Canonical correlation demonstrated
that a high positive loading on task orientation and a moderately
positive loading on sensation seeking corresponded positively to the
mood state of vigor and negatively to the mood states of tension and
anger among participants engaged in a thrill seeking sport. Ego
orientation, however, failed to contribute meaningfully to the
multivariate relationship and was found to be unrelated to any of the
mood state response measures contained in the POMS.
Skateboarders who expressed both a high task orientation and a
propensity for sensation seeking reported higher levels of the mood
state of vigor, and lower levels of the mood states of tension and anger
than their low sensation seeking cohorts. This pattern of mood states,
known as the iceberg profile, has been recognized to distinguish between
successful and less successful sport performances (Beedie et al., 2000),
especially among open sports of short duration where participants
potentially generate self-referenced performance criteria (Hall &
Terry, 1995; Terry, 1995), such as skateboarding.
A task orientation may be an especially viable approach in a
sensation seeking sport like skateboarding where one must persist
through endless hours of practice and exert relentless effort in order
to master a particularly challenging trick. The high levels of vigor
observed among task-oriented skateboarders coincide well with research
linking task orientation with effort, hard work, and intrinsic
motivation (Boyd et al., 2002; Spray et al., 1999). Such vigor may be a
requisite demanded of the skateboard warrior who must navigate
innumerable spills and mishaps in order to improve to the next
subjective level of boarding proficiency. Moreover, the inverse
multivariate relationship which emerged between task orientation and
both tension and anger is consistent with literature indicating a task
orientation to be negatively related to cognitive anxiety and positively
linked to sportspersonship (Dunn & Dunn, 1999; Hall & Kerr,
1997). A task orientation may serve to insulate the skateboarder from
the debilitating effects of performance anxiety.
Sensation seeking, in conjunction with a task orientation, was also
aligned with desirable mood states and may therefore also be an integral
factor for the skateboarder. The negative relationship between sensation
seeking and both tension and anger, parallels studies linking sensation
seeking to anxiety management, ability to peak under pressure, blunted
nervous reactivity to physical risk, and both lower irritability and
emotional arousal (Franken et al., 1992; Netter et al., 1996; Smith et
al., 1992). The mood state of vigor was also related to sensation
seeking, as sensation seekers possess an inherent openness to new
experience and an affinity to search out high-risk activity (Roberti,
2004; Shrader & Wann, 1999) such as extreme sport.
Skateboarding shares many characteristics, including speed and
danger, with similar sports considered high-risk activity such as
surfing, snow boarding, and roller blading, where a willingness to
"push the envelope" may result in an obligatory mishap and
potential injury. For the skateboarder, aspiring to a higher level of
skill proficiency, there must be a willingness to attempt a threatening
jump or "hug a rail" just a little longer. Sensation seeking
may serve to provide the skateboarder the motivation to attempt the next
level of measured risk, still within one's limitations of
capability. Of course, engaging in reckless risk, or venturing beyond
one's perceived capability, is not well advised and may potentially
cause bodily injury.
Results also indicated that ego orientation failed to contribute
meaningfully to the multivariate relationship linking goal orientations
and sensation seeking to optimal mood states. Ego orientation, and
concomitant attention to the performance of others, apparently does not
correspond well with mood states known to facilitate performance. A
negligible interfactor correlation confirmed the orthogonal nature of
task and ego orientation within this sample of skateboarders. Means and
standard deviations of the two subscales were shown to be well within
ranges reported for these dispositional constructs in sport, physical
activity, and exercise (Duda & Whitehead, 1998).
The depression and fatigue mood state measures failed to
meaningfully load on the mood state canonical variate. These mood states
evidently have little relevance to task-oriented skateboarders who are
also sensation seekers. Perhaps a task orientation and perceptions of
effort, in conjunction with a desire to seek out thrilling physical
activity, is not a mindset conducive to entertaining feelings of
depression. Furthermore, the opportunity for periodic rest is readily at
the immediate voluntary discretion of the skateboarding enthusiast,
perhaps rendering the mood state of fatigue inconsequential to the
skateboarder.
The present results indicated that a task orientation and sensation
seeking were significantly related to the mood states of vigor, tension,
and anger, in a manner consistent with the iceberg profile, known to be
aligned with successful sport performance. Future research should
examine other correlates of goal orientation and sensation seeking. In
order to enhance generalization, the two constructs should also be
examined among other currently popular, individualized sensation seeking
sports such as surfing or snow boarding.
Some controversy exists concerning whether skateboarding is truly a
sport. In response, Howe (2004), a long-term skateboarder, and writer,
quotes a professional skateboarder who was asked this question, and
responded, "Yes. Wait, no. I don't know" (pg. 353). A
study designed to ascertain whether task-oriented skateboarders express
significantly different affective reactions than ego-oriented
skateboarders, in a competitive skateboarding event, would shed some
light on this controversy. Given the current popularity of extreme
sports such as skateboarding in this country, ongoing formal inquiry is
certainly warranted.
References
American Sports Data Inc. (2001). Hartsdale, New York.
Ames, C. (1992). Achievement goals, motivational climate, and
motivational processes. In G.C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in sport and
exercise (pp. 161-176). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Balaguer, I., Duda, J.L., Atienza, F.L., & Mayo, C. (2002).
Situational and dispositional goals as predictors of perceptions of
individual and team improvement, satisfaction, and coach ratings among
elite female handball teams. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 3,
293-308.
Ball, I. L., Farnhill, D., & Wangeman, J. F. (1984). Sex and
age differences in sensation seeking. British Journal of Psychology, 75,
257-265.
Beedie, C.J., Terry, P.C., & Lane, A.M. (2000). The profile of
mood states and athletic performance: Two meta-analyses. Journal of
Applied Sport Psychology, 12, 49-68.
Boyd, M.P., Weinmann, C.A., & Yin, Z. (2002). The relationship
of physical self-perceptions and goal orientation to intrinsic
motivation for exercise. Journal of Sport Behavior, 25, 1-18.
Brevnik, G. (1996). Personality, sensation seeking and risk taking
among Everest climbers. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 27,
308-320.
Cronbach, L.J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure
of tests. Psychometrica, 16, 427.
Duda, J.L. (1989). Relationship between task and ego orientation
and the perceived purpose of sport among high school athletes. Journal
of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11, 318-335.
Duda, J.L. (2001). Goal perspective research in sport: Pushing the
boundaries and clarifying some misunderstandings. In G.C. Roberts (Ed.),
Advances in motivation in sport and exercise (pp. 129-182). Champaign,
IL: Human Kinetics.
Duda, J.L., & Whitehead, J. (1998). Measurement of goal
perspectives in the physical domain. In J.L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in
sport and exercise psychology measurement (pp.21-48). Morgantown, WV:
Fitness Information Technology.
Dunn, J.G.H., & Dunn, J.C. (1999). Goal orientations,
perceptions of aggression, and sportspersonship in elite male ice hockey players. The Sport Psychologist, 13, 183-200.
Dweck, C.S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning.
American Psychologist, 41, 1040-1048.
Dweck, C.S., & Leggett, E. (1988). A social--cognitive approach
to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256-273.
Franken, R.E., Gibson, K.J., & Rowland, G.L. (1992). Sensation
seeking and the tendency to view the world as threatening. Personality
and Individual Differences, 13, 31-38.
Hall, H.K., & Kerr, A.W. (1997). Motivational antecedents of
pre-competitive anxiety in youth sport. The Sport Psychologist, 11,
24-42.
Hall, A., & Terry, P. (1995). Mood profile trends during the
preparation and racing phases of the 1993 World Rowing Championships,
Roundnice, the Czech Republic. Journal of Sports Sciences, 13, 56-57.
Howe, J. (2004). A report from the extreme world. In R.E. Rinehart
& S. Sydnor (Eds.), To the extreme: Alternative sports, inside and
out (pp. 353-369). State University of New York Press.
Kimiecik, J.C., Horn, T.S., & Shurin, C.S. (1996). Relationship
among children's beliefs, perceptions of their parents'
beliefs, and their moderate-to-vigorous physical activity. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 67, 324-336.
LeUnes, A., & Burger, J. (2000). Profile of mood states
research in sport and exercise psychology: Past, present, and future.
Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 12, 5-15.
Malkin, M.J., & Rabinowitz, E. (1998). Sensation seeking and
high-risk recreation. Parks and Recreation, 33, 34-40.
McNair, D.M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L.F. (1992). Revised
Manual for the Profile of Mood States. San Diego, CA: Educational and
Industrial Testing Services.
Morgan, W.P. (1985). Selected psychological factors limiting
performance: A mental health model. In D.H. Clarke, & H.M. Eckert
(Eds.), Limits of Human Performance (pp. 70-80). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Morgan, W.P., & Costill, D.L. (1996). Selected psychological
characteristics and health behaviors of aging marathon runners: A
longitudinal study. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 17,
305-312.
Netter, P., Hennig, J., & Roed, I.S. (1996). Serotonin and
dopamine as mediators of sensation seeking behavior. Neuropsychobiology,
34, 155-165.
Nichols, J.G. (1984). Conceptions of ability and achievement
motivation. In R. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research and motivation in
education: Student motivation. (Vol. 1, pp. 39-73). New York: Academic
Press.
Nicholls, J.G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic
education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Ntoumanis, N., & Biddle, S. (1998). Affect and achievement
goals in physical activity: A meta-analysis. Scandinavian Journal of
Medicine and Science in Sports, 9, 315-332.
Nunnally, J.C., & Bernstein, R.H. (1994). Psychometric theory.
McGraw-Hill, Inc.
Ridgeway, D., & Russell, J.A. (1980). Reliability and validity
of the sensation-seeking scale: Psychometric problems in Form V. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 662-664.
Roberti, J.W. (2004). A review of behavioral and biological
correlates of sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences,
38, 256-279.
Roberts, G.C. (2001). Understanding the dynamics of motivation in
physical activity: The influence of achievement goals on motivational
processes. In G.C. Roberts (Ed.), Advances in motivation in sport and
exercise (pp. 1-50). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Roberts, G.C., Treasure, D.C., & Kavussanu, M. (1996).
Orthogonality of achievement goals and its relationship to beliefs about
success and satisfaction in sport. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 398-408.
Rowley, A.J., Landers, D.M., Kyllo, L.B., & Etnier, J.L.
(1995). Does the iceberg profile discriminate between successful and
less successful athletes? Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 17,
183-199.
Schrader, M.P., & Warm, D.L. (1999). High-risk recreation: The
relationship between participant characteristics and degree of
involvement. Journal of Sport Behavior, 22, 426-441.
Smith, R.E., Ptacek, J. T., & Smoll, F.L. (1992). Sensation
seeking, stress, and adolescent injuries: A test of stress-buffering,
risk-taking, and coping skills hypotheses. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 62, 1016-1024.
Spray, C.M., Biddle, S.J.H., & Fox, J.R. (1999). Achievement
goals, beliefs about the causes of success and reported emotion in
post-16 physical education. Journal of Sports Sciences, 17, 213-219.
Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (2001). Using multivariate
statistics (4th ed.) New York: Harper & Rowe.
Terry, P. (1994). Mood state profiles as indicators of performance
among Olympic and World Championship athletes. Journal of Sports
Sciences, 12, 214-215.
Terry, P. (1995). The efficacy of mood state profiling with elite
performers: A review and synthesis. The Sport Psychologist, 9, 309-324.
Terry, P.C., & Lane, A.M. (2000). Normative values for the
profile of mood states for use with athletic samples. Journal of Applied
Sport Psychology, 12, 93-109.
Wagner, A.M., & Houlihan, D.D. (1994). Sensation seeking and
trait anxiety in hang-gliders and golfers. Personality and Individual
Differences, 16, 975-977.
White, S.A., & Zellner, S.R. (1996). The relationship between
goal orientation, beliefs about the causes of sport success, and trait
anxiety among high school, intercollegiate, and recreational sport
participants. The Sport Psychologist, 16, 416-430.
Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of
sensation seeking. NewYork: Cambridge Press.
Zuckerman, M., Eysenck, S.B.G., Eysenck, H.J. (1978). Sensation
seeking in England and America; Cross-cultural, age, and sex
comparisons. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 139-149.
Michael P. Boyd
California State University, Fullerton
and
Mi-Sook Kim
San Francisco State University
Address Correspondence To: Michael Boyd, Ph.D., Department of
Kinesiology, California State University, Fullerton, P.O. Box 6870,
Fullerton, CA 92834.
Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, and Internal Reliability of Measures
Variables M SD [alpha]
Predictor Variables
Task Orientation 4.41 .57 .81
Ego Orientation 2.18 .73 .70
Sensation seeking 3.23 .68 .76
POMS
Tension 6.97 4.07 .77
Anger 4.76 4.14 .81
Fatigue 7.18 4.29 .82
Vigor 11.49 3.76 .79
Depression 4.44 4.07 .75
Table 2.
Correlation Coefficients among All Measured Variables
2 3 4 5
1. Task orientation .10 .10 -.28 * -.24 *
2. Ego orientation .12 .14 .21
3. Sensation seeking .02 .05
4. Tension .69 ***
5. Anger
6. Fatigue
7. Vigor
8. Depression
6 7 8
1. Task orientation -.09 .31 ** -.03
2. Ego orientation .22 .05 .15
3. Sensation seeking .09 .33 ** -.02
4. Tension .47 *** -.11 .51 ***
5. Anger .49 *** -.20 .60 ***
6. Fatigue -.16 .50 ***
7. Vigor -.12
8. Depression
Note: * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001
Table 3
Canonical Loadings, Percents of Variance, and Canonical Correlation
between Goal Orientation/Sensation seeking and Mood States
Canonical loadings
Predictor Variables
Task orientation .856
Ego orientation .082
Sensation-seeking .599
Percent of variance .37
Criterion Variables
Tension -.469
Anger -.374
Fatigue -.077
Vigor .871
Depression -.094
Percent of variance .23
Canonical Correlation .49