Coping under pressure: cognitive strategies for maintaining confidence among soccer referees.
Wolfson, Sandy ; Neave, Nick
Soccer (association football) officials are routinely subjected to
a wide range of potential stressors. Analyses of English (Catterall,
Reilly, Atkinson, & Goldwells, 1993), Danish (Krustrup &
Bangsbo, 2001) and Japanese (Asami, Togari, & Ohashi, 1988) referees
reveal a considerable physical toll, with about 10 km distance covered
in a typical match. Of this distance, it is estimated that 47% is spent
jogging, 23% walking, 12% sprinting, and 18% reverse running, with an
average heart rate of 165 beats per minute recorded.
In addition, referees experience immediate, unrestrained negative
feedback throughout and after a match. As England Premiership referee
David Elleray stated, "Almost every time you blow the whistle, you
upset half the players and at least half the crowd" (Learning
English, 2006).
Soccer referees identify spectators, players, coaches, trainers and
other personnel as sources of aggression (Folkesson, Nyberg, Archer,
& Norlander, 2002). At the extreme, referees and their assistants
have been pushed, punched, kicked, and even shot to death by players,
managers, coaches and fans. In April, 2002, two referees were attacked
by players in separate incidents in Africa when a teammate had been sent
off and when a goal had been disallowed. Referees and their regulating
bodies are well aware of these sources of threat, and some attempts have
been made to provide psychological training to help them cope (Mahoney,
2003).
In addition, match-specific demands are extreme. The referee must
have a thorough knowledge of the current laws of soccer and implement
them while keeping constant vigilance over complex activities and
interactions. Considerable amounts of time must also be spent on
pre-match preparation, travel, and post-match reports.
A few studies have examined referee responses to stress in
basketball (KaissidisRodafinos & Anshel, 2000; Burke, Joyner, Pim,
& Czech, 2000), volleyball (Van Yperen, 1998), baseball and softball (Rainey, 1995), rugby union (Nesti & Sewell, 2003) and soccer
(Taylor & Daniel, 1988), but little was found about the mechanisms
which motivate officials to continue their involvement despite the
variety of potential physical, social and cognitive factors which are
regularly encountered. The present study was designed to allow referees
to convey their perceptions, particularly with regard to their methods
for coping with demands and abuse and their reasons for remaining
referees.
Social psychologists have identified a number of cognitive
strategies that help people explain and deal with negative experiences.
Blaming others for failure while taking personal credit for success,
distorting or ignoring unpleasant information, and interpreting
one's motives as principled and righteous, are among the variety of
available methods of maintaining confidence in difficult circumstances.
Misrepresenting reality with 'positive illusions' (Taylor
& Brown, 1988, 1994) can ultimately be healthy and adaptive. Indeed,
they serve a vital function of removing perceptions of self-blame,
protecting from external censure, and providing palatable explanations
for disturbing events. In the longer term, they may lead to more
positive expectations about the future, greater persistence and
self-efficacy. Taylor and Brown (1988) suggest that people who fail to
use these strategies are more likely to be anxious or depressed.
Sports performers and coaches often make use of self-serving
attributions (Biddle, 1993; McAuley & Duncan, 1989) to explain their
own disappointing results. External factors such as bad luck or other
people's incompetence are used to deal with unpleasant experiences,
defeats, and rejection, while internal factors such as skill and effort
are seen as responsible for victories and other successes. Lau and
Russell's (1980) analysis of newspaper reports showed that players
and coaches were more likely to attribute their successes to internal
factors (such as skill and determination) and their losses to external
factors (such as cheating opponents and poor weather) than sportswriters
describing exactly the same events.
Another potentially useful mechanism is 'illusory
superiority' or self-aggrandizement, where people adopt the view
that they have more positive qualities than others (Alicke, 1985) and
describe their personal strengths as above average compared with those
of other people their age (Kleinke & Miller, 1998). The illusory nature of this is demonstrated by Lewinsohn, Mischel, Chaplin, and
Barton (1980), who found that individuals made more flattering judgments
about themselves than others made of them. Individuals also believe
their personal relationships are of a higher quality than the
relationships of others (Buunk & van der Eijnden, 1977) and remember
experiences as more successful than they really were (Taylor &
Brown, 1988).
Illusory superiority may be accompanied by unrealistic optimism
(Hoorens & Buunk, 1992). Many studies show that people expect more
positive and fewer negative outcomes for themselves than others.
Weinstein (1980) found that students believed they were more likely than
other students to live past the age of 80 and have a mentally gifted
child, and less likely to lose their job, be sterile, or have a heart
attack. This study also revealed that the greater the perceived
controllability of the event, the greater the optimism. This suggests
that people have greater confidence in their likelihood of taking
appropriate steps to ensure a promising outcome. In support of this,
Hoorens and Harris (1998) demonstrated that people report higher
frequencies of healthy behaviors for themselves than for others.
The illusory nature of this self-aggrandizement was further
demonstrated by Schmidt, Berg and Deelman (1999) who reported that
although older adults were willing to admit to a decline in their memory
when their reference point was that of their own memory at age 25, they
expressed superiority when they compared themselves with their own peers
and even 25-year old adults.
Biases such as illusory superiority and self-serving attributions
may thus encourage people to maintain the view that criticism from
others is undeserved and based on faulty reasoning. The present study
was designed to examine the extent to which potential stressors are seen
as problematic by referees and identify the psychological mechanisms
used to maintain confidence. It was predicted that referees would make
use of a number of cognitive strategies to help them cope with the
demands of their role. These are likely to include externalizing reasons
for abuse to absolve themselves of blame, construing errors as a means
for improvement, assuming superiority over others, and conceptualizing
their refereeing as a positive contribution to soccer.
Method
Participants
A senior English soccer league endorsed a project on referees'
perceptions and provided a list of 63 names and addresses of qualified
county officials who had refereed for at least three years. All were
sent a letter giving procedural details of the project and asking if
they would volunteer to complete a battery of questionnaires. Forty-two
of the referees (67%) agreed to take part.
The average age of the volunteers was 40.04 (SD=3.83), ranging from
33-46. Of these, 95% were in full-time employment in a variety of
professions, including five police officers, five teachers, three in
management, and two in the fire service. Of the sample 94% were educated
to GCSE level or higher. Their average years as a referee was 12.33 (SD
= 5.50), ranging from 4-25. All were male.
Instrument
A 4-page questionnaire was constructed to gather personal details
of background and perceptions. Information regarding years of
experience, level of refereeing and time spent on duties, support and
training were obtained. Referees also rated the extent to which various
factors motivated them in their role as an official, with blank spaces
left so that the participants could indicate additional factors.
Following this, participants were asked to indicate their perceptions of
themselves and responses to various stimuli such as hostile crowds,
verbal and physical abuse, lack of recognition, and failure to be
selected). The items were identified through a focus group of different
referees prior to the study, where a brainstorming session was used to
identify the most salient influences. Some of the items related to
stress were adapted from the Soccer Officials' Stress Survey
(Taylor & Daniel, 1988). Finally, the referees were asked to rate
their qualities as an official, compared to other officials operating at
their level, on 15 characteristics such as knowledge of the rules,
honesty, fitness, and decisiveness. Aside from the personal details
section and open-ended questions, all items used 1-4 Likertformat
scales, with the exception of the 5-point illusory superiority scales,
where a midpoint was required for referees to indicate if they felt
neither superior nor inferior to other referees. The items were analyzed
at the individual level rather than pooled into factors.
Procedure
Permission was received by the official referees' association
to approach potential volunteers by mail. All participants completed an
informed consent form indicating that their individual responses would
be confidential and that generalized feedback of results from the study
would be provided after completion of the project. The questionnaires
were accompanied by self-addressed postage-paid envelopes. The protocol
described above was approved by Northumbria University School of
Psychology & Sport Sciences Ethics Committee.
Results
Potential Stressors
All respondents stated that they officiated 'most weeks'
during the soccer season. An average time of 16.10 (SD=7.24) hours,
ranging from 5-36 hours per week, was spent on travel, preparation,
officiating, and match reports, and 71% of the sample stated that they
felt physically drained after matches. Some referees noted that these
demands sometimes led to conflicts with occupation (50%) and family
(38%) commitments. However, 100% asserted that the time and energy they
put into refereeing was worthwhile.
Many referees expected to be the object of abuse or dissent from
players (41%), coaches or managers (44%), and spectators (63%). However,
of the 22 items which referred to factors contributing to stress, few
were endorsed as negatively affecting the officials. On only four items
was the mean rating over 2.5, the theoretically neutral point on the
4-point scale. These results are indicated in Table 1.
Other experiences such as physical assaults, verbal abuse from
players, awarding a penalty, sending off a player, disagreeing with
co-officials, failure to be selected for important games, maintaining
concentration, bad weather conditions, and media reactions were not
rated as at all stress-provoking.
Explanations for dissent and abuse
Table 2 indicates that the officials tended to externalize the
reasons for dissent and abuse by focusing on other people's bias
and lack of knowledge. Actual referee error was deemed least responsible
for criticism received. The most popular explanations were interpreted
as observer bias, misunderstandings of the laws of soccer, and emotional
responses. Crowd influences, sincere beliefs, and attempts to persuade
the referee were moderately endorsed.
Responses to errors
All officials admitted to having made errors and having experienced
a 'bad game'. Responses to these negative experiences are
shown in Table 3. The most highly endorsed items refer to the positive
opportunity to analyze and learn from mistakes, while those least
endorsed involved worrying and dwelling over errors.
Illusory superiority
The referees rated themselves as superior to fellow referees on all
of the 15 characteristics listed (see Table 4) by indicating 4 (I am
slightly better) or 5 (I am much better) on the 5-point scale. One
sample t-tests revealed that the mean responses on every item, as well
as the total means, were significantly higher (all p<.001) than the
'average' point 3.
Rarely did the referees rate themselves as worse than others, the
highest level being 10% reached for 'fitness'. However, when
the referees were asked how their officiating would change if they were
fitter, 33% said only a little better, 29% somewhat better, and only 17%
much better. The remaining 21% believed their officiating would be no
better.
Involvement in refereeing
The most highly endorsed reasons for refereeing were those relating
to the intrinsic devotion to soccer, while the most rejected were
concerned with rewards such as power, income, prestige, recognition and
respect (see Table 5).
The referees generally felt that valuable support systems were in
place and took advantage of these, with 93% having attended local and
national meetings and 86% having sought advice about officiating issues.
In addition, 43% trained with other officials and 79% felt able to talk
about negative experiences with close friends and family members. There
were significant correlations between the use of the various support
systems (meeting attendance and seeking advice (r =. 62, df = 40,
p<.001); meeting attendance and training with others (r = .40, df =
40, p<.01); and seeking advice and training with others (r = .51, df
= 40, p<.001).
Individual differences
Analyses of relationships between individual differences
(education, age, years as referee) and other questionnaire items yielded
no significant differences between subgroups.
Discussion
The results portray soccer officials as confident and resilient
individuals who are highly motivated by their devotion to soccer, the
opportunity to contribute to the sport, and pride in their
accomplishments. Indeed, the love of soccer was the only item on the
questionnaire where every single respondent gave the highest possible
answer. This result is consistent with that found by Burke, Joyner, Pim,
and Czech (2000), where 'love of the game' was the most
frequently cited reason for choosing to become a basketball official.
Similarly, Purdey and Snyder (1985) found that enthusiasm for their
sport was the main motivator for high school basketball officials.
However, these basketball officials also endorsed extra money and
feelings of power, factors which were rejected by the soccer referees in
the current study.
The present sample admit to experiencing occasional 'bad
games' and identify these as moderately stressful but do not feel
unduly disheartened by such experiences. In addition, they acknowledge
the fact that they are targets of criticism but do not express concern
about disparagement; indeed, one respondent described verbal abuse as
"like water off a duck's back". This result is consistent
with the findings of Rainey and Winterich (1995) with basketball
referees and Rainey and Hardy (1997, 1999) with rugby referees, where
only low to moderate levels of stress were indicated. The latter gave
questionnaires to over 600 rugby union referees from Wales, Scotland,
and England, and found that fear of physical harm was unrelated to
stress levels, while time pressure and interpersonal conflict were only
mildly related. Similar findings were reported by Goldsmith and Williams
(1992), who found that physical harm, verbal abuse, time pressures and
fear of failure did not contribute to stress levels in volleyball and
soccer referees.
The present questionnaire responses provide evidence for the use of
a number of strategies which might reduce or eliminate the troublesome
nature of such factors. Support systems allow officials to talk and
train with each other, facilitating the exchange of experiences and
constructive advice. Such support is likely to engender feelings of
group identity (see Hogg & Abrams, 1988), and it is possible that
referees are bolstered by the fact that they share many views and
experiences. Considering oneself an 'ingroup' member results
in taking pride in and defending the collective (Schmader & Major,
1999). Referees are further supported by friends and family, with whom
many referees feel able to discuss negative or controversial incidents.
Although all officials noted that they had at times made errors,
they contended that such mistakes are ultimately beneficial to their
professional development by providing opportunities for improvement. The
most common responses relate to the desire to learn from errors by
mentally replaying and analyzing them and imagining how a different
behavior might have represented a more appropriate decision on their
part. Such counterfactual thinking, where people generate mental
representations of alternatives to the existing reality, can function to
improve preparation for similar future events (Roese, 1994).
The evidence also suggests that officials engage in a variety of
confidence-enhancing cognitions which can serve to console them. They
are unlikely to ignore their errors, feel embarrassed by them, or worry
about their recurrence and implications. Instead, they try to analyze
their mistakes and remind themselves that they tried their best, that
their errors are rare, and that getting every decision right would be
impossible. This allows them to remain self-assured and expectant about
future encounters. Perceiving errors as temporary and unusual might
allow referees to sustain the belief that such situations are less
likely to recur and are in fact likely to improve their competence.
These processes may help officials explain the mismatch between their
perceived competence and the criticism received from others. Such
discrepancies are known to produce extreme emotions such as confusion
and unhappiness (Higgins, 1987) which people are motivated to resolve.
The use of external attributions also helps officials to come to
terms with negative assessments of their performance. Rather than
assuming that criticism is mainly a result of their own errors, the
referees focus on misunderstandings and partiality on the part of
players, fans, coaches, and managers. The most highly endorsed reasons
for abuse centered on other people's lack of knowledge of the laws
of soccer, interpersonal bias toward one's team, and heated
emotions. Discounting responsibility and re-attributing blame in this
way has been shown to protect emotional well-being (Major, Kaiser, &
McCoy, 2003).
Self-aggrandizement is another coping strategy which was highly
evident among the referees. Rarely did anyone admit to being lower than
average on a characteristic deemed important for refereeing; most
respondents asserted that they were better than fellow referees
operating at the same level. Since the referees were comparing
themselves with each other, the illusory nature of these ratings is
striking: not everyone in the group could be above average. This result
is consistent with the findings of Kleinke and Miller (1998), whose
participants viewed themselves as being above average on a number of
personal qualities. The researchers also found a linear relationship
between the level of superiority expressed and measures of psychological
well-being.
The characteristic which attracted the greatest level of confession
of inferiority was fitness, and even here only 10% admitted to being
'worse than others'. Interestingly, however, many of the
referees contended that their officiating would not improve if they were
fitter, suggesting the possibility of further self-serving distortion or
denial.
Following Taylor and Brown's (1988) original paper on the
benefits of positive illusions, deceptive self-enhancing mechanisms have
continued to be associated with psychological well-being by several
recent theorists (Taylor, Lerner, Sherman, Save, & McDowell 2003;
Gramzow, Elliot, Asher, & McGregor 2003). Taylor and Armor (1996)
even suggest that higher levels of such positive illusions can help
people to deal with stressful events of an extreme nature such as
cancer, heart disease, and HIV infection. However, Colvin and Block
(1994) have criticized the basis of this view and questioned the extent
to which the processes Taylor and Brown (1988) identified in their paper
actually represent distortions of reality. Also, studies by Robins and
Beer (2001) suggest that while self-enhancing beliefs have positive
short term effects, their deceptive nature can lead to arrogance or
complacency and thus render them maladaptive over time.
Withstanding abuse and dissent while concurrently carrying out a
complex task requires assuredness and conviction. The origins of such
characteristics in officials are unclear and need further exploration.
It is possible that refereeing attracts people who have a high level of
self esteem and already use coping mechanisms successfully, while people
with lower levels of self worth may find that they are unable to cope
with the demands of refereeing and drop out. Additionally, holding a
position of power and being supported by a strong infrastructure could
serve to increase confidence. Such qualities, however, may not
inevitably lead to good performance, as some people with high levels of
self-esteem react aggressively and defensively when their self-image is
threatened (Baumeister, Smart & Boden, 1996), particularly if their
self-esteem is unstable and only high in certain situations (Kernis
& Waschull, 1995).
Although Folkesson et al. (2002) found that younger referees were
most susceptible to threat and aggression, no such difference was found
in the present sample. The responses to abuse and the coping process
appear to be similar for groups regardless of their age, experience, and
education. It should be noted, though, that males have been found to use
more positive illusions than females (Boyd-Wilson, Walkey, McClure,
& Green, 2000). Given that only one female referee was represented
in the present sample, it is possible that different coping mechanisms
might be found in a less male-dominated group.
In summary, the results suggest that soccer referees are able to
remain confident in the face of considerable abuse and criticism. Two
pertinent questions remain unanswered, however. First, whether the
hardiness that characterizes the participants in the present study is
the result of a stable underlying disposition or acquired with training
and experience is unclear at this point. This could have important
implications for the recruitment and training of referees. Second,
processes such as illusory superiority and self-serving bias are
apparent among people of all walks of life, and it would be useful to
know whether referees have a particular need or tendency to rely on
these. These issues are currently being addressed. In any case, it does
appear to be clear that referees make use of a range of coping
mechanisms which might well underlie the robustness which characterises
their approach to officiating and their ability to withstand abuse. As
Premiership referee David Elleray said, "... you get more criticism
than praise, and you have to accept that as part of the job."
(Learning English, 2006).
Author Note
We thank the referees who took part in this study and two anonymous
referees who provided comments on the initial draft.
References
Alicke, M. D. (1985). Global self-evaluation as determined by the
desirability and controllability of trait adjectives. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1621-1630.
Asami, T., Togari, H., & Ohashi, J. (1988). Analysis of
movement patterns of referees during soccer matches. In T. Reilly, A.
Lees, K. Davids, & W. J. Murphy (Eds.). Science and Football (pp.
341-345). London: E & FN Spun.
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation
of threatened egotism to violence and aggression: the dark side of high
self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 5-33.
Biddle, S. (1993). Attribution research and sport psychology. In R.
N. Singer, M. Murphey, & L. K. Tennant (Eds.). Handbook on research
on sport psychology (pp. 437-464). New York: Macmillan.
Boyd-Wilson, B. M., Walkey, F. H., McClure, J., & Green, D. E.
(2002). Do we need positive illusions to carry out plans? Illusion and
instrumental coping. Personality and Individual Differences, 29,
1141-1152.
Buunk, B. P., & van der Eijnden, R. J. J. M. (1997). Perceived
prevalence, perceived superiority, and relationship satisfaction: most
relationships are good, but ours is best. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 23, 219-228.
Burke, K. L., Joyner, A. B., Pim, A., & Czech, D. R. (2000). An
exploratory investigation of the perceptions of anxiety among basketball
officials before, during, and after the contest. Journal of Sport
Behavior, 23, 11-19.
Catterall, C., Reilly, T., Atkinson, G., & Goldwells, A.
(1993). Analysis of the work rates and heart rates of association
football referees. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 27, 193-196.
Colvin, C. R., & Block, J. (1994). Do positive illusions foster
mental health--an examination of the Taylor and Brown formulation.
Psychological Bulletin, 116, 3-20.
Folkesson, P., Nyberg, C., Archer, T., & Norlander, T. (2002).
Soccer referees' experience of threat and aggression: effects of
age, experience and life orientation on outcome of coping strategy.
Journal of Aggressive Behavior, 28, 317-327.
Goldsmith, P. A., & Williams, J.M. (1992). Perceived stressors
for football and volleyball officials from three rating levels. Journal
of Sport Behavior, 15, 106-118.
Gramzow, R. H., Elliot, A. J., Asher, E., & McGregor, H. A.
(2003). Self-evaluation bias and academic performance: some ways and
some reasons why. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 41-61.
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and
affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319-340.
Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. (1988). Social identifications: a
social psychology of intergroup relations and group processes. London:
Routledge.
Hoorens, V., & Buunk, B. P. (1992). Self-serving biases in
social comparison--illusory superiority and unrealistic optimism.
Psychologica Belgica, 32, 169-194.
Hoorens, V., & Harris, P. (1998). Distortions in reports of
health behaviors: The time span effect and illusory superiority.
Psychology and Health, 13, 451-466.
Kaissidis-Rodafinos, A., & Anshel, M. H. (2000). Psychological
predictors of coping responses among Greek basketball referees. Journal
of Social Psychology, 140, 329-344.
Kernis, M. H., & Waschull, S. B. (1995). The interactive roles
of stability and level of self-esteem: research and theory. In M. P.
Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 93-41). San
Diego: Academic Press.
Kleinke, C. L., & Miller, W. F. (1998). How comparing oneself
favorably with others relates to well-being. Journal of Social and
Clinical Psychology, 17, 107-123.
Krustrup, P., & Bangsbo, J. (2001). Physiological demands of
top-class soccer refereeingin relation to physical capacity: effect of
intense intermittent exercise training. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19,
991-891.
Learning English: David Elleray, Referee. Retrieved March 5, 2006
from (http://www.bbc.co.uk/worldservice/learningenglish/sport/
football/elleray_interview.shtml).
Lau, R. R., & Russell, D. (1980). Attributions in the sports
pages: a field test of some current hypotheses about attribution
research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 29-38.
Lewinsohn, P. M., Mischel, W., Chaplin, W., & Barton, R.
(1980). Social competence and depession: the role of illusory
self-perceptions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 203-212.
Mahoney, C. (2003). Psychology and Premier League refereeing:
professionalism and accountability. Paper presented at the Annual
British Psychological Society Conference, Bournemouth.
Major, B., Kaiser, C. R., & McCoy, S. K. (2003). It's not
my fault: when and why attributions to prejudice protect self-esteem.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 772-781.
McAuley, E., & Duncan, T. E. (1989). Causal attributions and
affective reactions to disconfirming outcomes in motor performance.
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11, 187-200.
Nesti, M., & Sewell, D. (2003). Referees are human too!: an
idiographic analysis of psychological states in Super League rugby
league referees. Paper presented at the Annual British Psychological
Society Conference, Bouruemouth.
Purdy, D..A., & Snyder, E. E. (1985). A social profile of high
school basketball officials. Journal of Sport Behavior, 8, 54-65.
Rainey, D. W. (1995). Stress, burnout, and intention to terminate
among umpires. Journal of Sport Behavior, 18, 312-323.
Rainey, D. W., & Hardy, L. (1999) Sources of stress, burnout
and intention to terminate among rugby union referees. Journal of Sports
Sciences, 17, 797-806.
Rainey, D. W., & Hardy, L. (1997) Ratings of stress by rugby
referees. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 728-730.
Rainey, D. W., & Winterich, D, (1995). Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 81, 1241-1242.
Robins, R. W., & Beer, J. S. (2001). Positive illusions about
the self: short-term benefits and long-term costs. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 340-352.
Roese, N. J. (1994). The functional basis of counterfactual
thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 805-818.
Schmader, T., & Major, B. (1999). The impact of ingroup vs
outgroup performance on personal values. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 35, 47-67.
Schmidt, I. W., Berg, I. J., & Deelman, B. G (1999). Illusory
superiority in self-reported memory of older adults. Aging
Neuropsychology and Cognition, 6, 288-301.
Taylor, A. H., & Daniel, J. V. (1988). In T. Reilly, A. Lees,
K. Davids, & W. J. Murphy (Eds.) Science and Football (pp. 538-544).
E&FN Spon: London.
Taylor, S. E., & Armor, D. A. (1996). Positive illusions and
coping with adversity. Journal of Personality, 64, 873-898.
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being:
a social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological
Bulletin, 103, 193-210.
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1994). Positive illusions and
well-being revisited: separating fact from fiction. Psychological
Bulletin, 116, 21-27.
Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sherman, D. K., Save, R. M., &
McDowell, N. K. (2003). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
84, 165-176.
Van Peryn, N. W. (1998). Predicting stay/leave behavior among
volleyball referees. Sport Psychologist, 12, 427-439.
Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life
events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 806-820.
Sandy Wolfson and Nick Neave
Northumbria University
Address Correspondence To: Dr. Sandy Wolfson Fax: 44-191-227-3190,
Telephone: 44 (191)2273427, E-mail: s.wolfson@unn.ac.uk., Division of
Psychology, School of Psychology & Sport Sciences, Northumberland
Building, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon Tyne, N-E1 8ST, U.K.
Table 1. Potential stressors receiving highest ratings.
Potential stressor MEAN SD % endorsing
Having a 'bad' game 2.93 .84 62
People who protest decisions
when they don't understand 2.74 1.13 60
the laws of the game
Possible demotion to a lower
level of officiating 2.71 2.35 33
Verbal abuse by managers,
coaches or spectators 2.60 .91 50
Table 2. Perceived reasons for dissent and abuse.
Reason Mean SO % endorsing
People not understanding the 3.52 .64 93
laws of football.
People's bias toward their 3.52 .70 89
team.
People reacting to the heat 3.41 .64 93
of the moment.
People being influenced by 2.89 .71 77
other dissenters.
People expecting me to ignore 2.56 .86 37
or reinterpret the laws of
football.
People's sincere beliefs that 2.56 .80 44
I am wrong.
People's conscious attempt to 2.52 .80 41
persuade me to be more lenient
toward their team.
My actual errors. 2.07 .66 11
Table 3. Responses to errors.
%
RESPONSE Mean SD endorsing
Try to learn something from it. 3.81 .40 100
Analyse what happened so I can
understand it better. 3.78 .51 96
'Replay' the situation in my mind and
imagine what I should have done. 3.63 .69 96
Remind myself that I tried my best. 3.52 .80 89
Talk to fellow officials about what
happened. 3.41 .64 93
Remind myself that my errors are rare. 3.19 .62 90
Enjoy the comfort and company of my
family and/or friends. 3.15 .91 74
Remind myself of all the good games I've had 2.96 1.04 69
Remind myself of the difficulties of getting
every decision right. 2.96 .94 70
Try to think of other things rather than
dwell on it. 2.74 1.10 63
Feel a temporary loss of pride. 2.67 .83 67
Feel embarrassed. 2.16 .89 32
Nothing--just ignore it. 1.82 .96 29
Worry that it might happen again. 1.74 .90 22
Toss and turn over it. 1.65 .69 12
Have a stiff drink. 1.63 1.01 22
Worry that my 'superiors' will hold it
against me. 1.63 .84 15
Worry that I might get a bad reputation. 1.44 .64 7
Table 4. Perceptions ofsuperiority.
% better % worse
than than
others others
Visual perception 55 2
Commitment 69 0
Honesty 67 0
Willingness to
accept a mistake 67 0
Accurate judgments
during a game 50 2
Fitness 50 10
Decisiveness 64 0
Dealing with
heated situations 59 2
Confidence 76 2
Knowing the
intentions of the players 64 5
Reading the game 67 2
Interacting with
fellow officials 57 5
Spotting a player who
'cheats,' e.g., by diving 41 0
Making offside decisions 64 0
Knowledge of the rules 62 0
TOTAL MEAN 60.80 2.00
Mean SD t *
Visual perception 3.71 1.04 4.44
Commitment 4.17 1.10 6.86
Honesty 4.07 1.09 6.37
Willingness to 4.10 1.10 6.45
accept a mistake
Accurate judgments 3.71 1.09 4.25
during a game
Fitness 3.74 1.23 3.89
Decisiveness 3.91 1.03 5.68
Dealing with 3.88 1.11 5.15
heated situations
Confidence 4.22 1.11 7.05
Knowing the 3.91 1.12 5.23
intentions of the players
Reading the game 3.98 1.09 5.79
Interacting with 3.86 1.16 4.79
fellow officials
Spotting a player who 3.71 1.12 4.03
'cheats,' e.g., by diving
Making offside decisions 4.00 1.08 5.99
Knowledge of the rules 3.98 1.09 5.79
TOTAL MEAN 3.92 0.94 6.24
* p < .001 for all
Table 5. Reasons for refereeing (Not at all [1]--Very much [4]):
Means (and standard deviations).
Most endorsed Mean (SD)
The love of football 4.00 (0)
The opportunity to 3.71 (.46)
contribute to football
Pride in accomplishments 3.69 (.56)
The challenge and 3.62 (.62)
excitement
Least endorsed Mean (SD)
The feeling of power 1.64 (.76)
and control
The extra income 1.81 (.80)
The prestige and 2.55 (.99)
recognition
The respect received 2.62 (.85)