Perceived athletic competence, sociometric status, and loneliness in elementary school children.
Dunn, Janice Causgrove ; Dunn, John G.H. ; Bayduza, Angela 等
Loneliness has been characterized as an individual's
"cognitive awareness of a deficiency in one's social and
personal relationships ... [that is accompanied by] ensuing affective reactions of sadness, emptiness, or longing" (Asher & Paquette,
2003, p.75). Parkhurst and Hopmeyer (1999) have further defined
loneliness as "a sad or aching sense of isolation ... [that
corresponds with] a felt deprivation of, [and] longing for, association,
contact, or closeness [with other people]" (p.58). According to Galanski and Kalantzi-Azizi (1999), when children are asked to define
loneliness they typically refer to their peer group as a reference point
and view loneliness as a state in which one is without friends that can
result in feelings of sadness (e.g., Cassidy & Asher, 1992).
Overall, loneliness for children reflects interpersonal deficits that
are the result of having fewer or less satisfying personal relationships
with other children than desired (Ponzetti, 1990).
Research consistently indicates that there are many psychosocial and emotional problems associated with loneliness in childhood and
adolescence (Kristensen, 1995). Studies with elementary school aged
children reveal that lonely children are less physically active and less
fit (Page, Frey, Talbert, & Falk, 1992) and more likely to
experience tension and anxiety (Page, 1991) than their non-lonely
counterparts. In adolescence, loneliness has been linked to a variety of
potentially health-threatening recreational behaviors including
cigarette smoking and marijuana use (Page, 1990). If loneliness persists
for a prolonged period of time during childhood (i.e., chronic
loneliness), there is an increased risk of school drop out, depression,
and alcoholism in late adolescence and early adulthood (Asher &
Paquette, 2003). Clearly, prolonged loneliness has the potential to
seriously undermine an individual's psychological, emotional, and
physical well-being. As such, understanding the correlates of loneliness
and identifying factors that can potentially decrease the likelihood of
experiencing loneliness for children becomes an important research
endeavor.
Research on children's loneliness has been dominated by a
focus on the influence of sociometric status within the peer system
(Asher & Paquette, 2003). The term "sociometric status" is
often used interchangeably with other terms such as peer acceptance,
social acceptance, or peer status (Weiss & Stuntz, 2004). At its
most basic level, sociometric status refers to the degree to which
children are liked or disliked by other children within the peer group
(Gifford-Smith & Brownell, 2003). Unlike friendship, which
represents a bilateral view of experiences between two specific
individuals, sociometric status represents a general group-oriented
unilateral view that describes how a peer group regards one of its
members (Weiss & Stuntz).
Peer acceptance at school is often assessed by having children
nominate peers they would most like and least like to play with, or by
asking children to indicate on rating scales the extent to which they
like or dislike each of their classroom peers (Cillessen & Bukowski,
2000). Irrespective of the measurement technique employed, sociometric
categories are then created in an attempt to describe the child's
sociometric status within the peer group. By subtracting the number of
negative nominations from the number of positive nominations, a
child's likeability (or level of social acceptance) by the group is
calculated. Children who are rated as popular are well liked by their
peers and seldom disliked (i.e., they receive many positive nominations
and few negative nominations). In contrast, children who receive many
least-like nominations and few most-like nominations are classified as
rejected children.
Research strongly supports a link between sociometric status and
loneliness in children, especially among children who are rejected by
their peers. Rejected children are frequently subjected to physical and
verbal harassment (Boivin & Hymel, 1997) and are often excluded from
social activities by the peer group (Buhs & Ladd, 2001). Rejected
children also have a tendency to disengage from the social environment
as a way of avoiding further abuse (Coie & Kupersmidt, 1983).
Theorists suggest that rejected children may therefore be exposed to a
larger number of social isolation experiences (i.e.,
loneliness-provoking situations) in their daily lives (Hymel, Tarulli,
Thomson, & Terrell-Deutsch, 1999) which in turn increases the degree
of loneliness experienced by the child. Thus, it is not surprising that
empirical research consistently finds that rejected children experience
higher levels of loneliness than their more popular peers (e.g., Asher
& Wheeler, 1985; Crick, Grotpeter, & Rockhill, 1999; Parker
& Asher, 1993; Parkhurst & Asher, 1992).
Although many factors can potentially contribute to loneliness
experiences (e.g., death of a parent or significant other, family
relocation), routine rejection by peers is clearly one of the most
critical factors that contribute to children's feelings of
loneliness (Bullock, 1993; Crick & Grotpeter, 1996). It therefore
becomes important to identify and understand factors that may increase a
child's likelihood of being accepted by the peer group, which in
turn decreases the likelihood of experiencing the destructive
psychosocial and emotional problems that often occur in conjunction with
rejection. Athletic ability or athletic competence may be one such
factor (see Kunesh, Hasbrook, & Lewthwaite, 1992; Page &
Scanlan, 1994; Weiss & Duncan, 1992).
Research in sport and physical activity settings has shown that
children who demonstrate athletic competence and who hold positive
beliefs about their own athletic abilities tend to be more popular with
their peers than less skilled children (Rose, Larkin, & Berger,
1997; Weiss & Duncan, 1992). In contrast, children who are poorly
skilled often become the target of criticism (Portman, 1995), ridicule
and bullying (Fitzpatrick & Watkinson, 2003; Mandich, Polatajko,
& Roger, 2003), and exclusion (Evans & Roberts, 1987; Mandich et
al., 2003). Positions reflecting higher social standing among children
within their social system (i.e., leadership positions) also are
afforded to more capable athletes. For example, Evans and Roberts (1987)
observed team selection among third through sixth grade boys in the
playground. Interviews with the boys about this selection revealed that
those who were viewed as having the highest sport ability were typically
elected as captains, whereas less skilled boys were generally chosen
much later in the selection process, and sometimes even excluded (i.e.,
"locked out") from the game entirely.
Athletic competence also has been identified as a highly valued
attribute among school children. For example, Chase and Dummer (1992)
asked fourth to sixth grade children to rank the importance of being
good at sports, making good grades, being physically attractive, and
having money in relation to their popularity with classmates. Boys
ranked sports ability as most important followed by physical appearance,
whereas girls ranked physical appearance as most important, followed by
sport ability (also see Eccles, Wigfield, Harold, & Blumenfeld,
1993). Buchanan, Blankenbaker, and Cotten (1976) also reported that boys
in elementary school viewed athletic competence as being the most
important social acceptance criteria at school. Given these findings,
Parkhurst and Hopmeyer (1999) speculated that deficits in athletic
competence may have implications for the development of loneliness in
children "if these skill deficits ... interfere with the
establishment and maintenance of peer relationships" (p. 72).
Findings from the extant literature clearly suggest that
children's athletic ability (whether actual or perceived) is linked
to (or may influence) sociometric status (see Weiss & Stuntz, 2004,
for a review). For example, Lubbers, van der Werf, Kuyper, and Offringa
(2006) recently reported that higher self-perceptions of athletic
competence predicted higher levels of peer acceptance among large
samples of first-year junior-high boys (n = 2983) and girls (n = 3864)
from the Netherlands (M age = 13 years). Boivin and Begin (1989) found
that popular third- and fourth-grade children reported higher levels of
self-perceived athletic competence, and received higher athletic
competence ratings from teachers than rejected children. Participation
in school athletic/sport teams has also been linked with students'
popularity in a sample of sixth to eighth grade students (see Eder &
Kinney, 1995). However, despite these findings, we could find no studies
that specifically examined the relationship between perceived athletic
competence and loneliness in children. Page et al. (1992) did report
that elementary school children (in Grades 1 to 6) with higher levels of
perceived loneliness tended to be less physically fit and less
physically active than less lonely children, however, the researchers
did not assess perceptions of athletic ability. Thus, the first purpose
of the present study was to examine the relationship between loneliness
and perceptions of athletic competence (as rated by self and peers) in
elementary school children. Given the valued role that athletic
competence plays for many elementary school children (Chase &
Dummer, 1992; Eccles et al., 1993), and given the potential
socialization opportunities that athletic competence may create (via
involvement in athletic/sport teams), we hypothesized that children with
higher levels of perceived athletic competence (as rated by the self and
by others) would experience less loneliness than children who had lower
levels of perceived athletic competence.
The second purpose of this study was to further investigate the
relationship between sociometric status and perceived athletic ability
in elementary school children. Although Weiss and Duncan (1992) found a
strong positive correlation between peer acceptance and perceived
athletic competence (as rated by the self and by teachers) among a
sample of elementary-aged children, it is possible that their findings
were influenced by the social context of the study. Specifically, Weiss
and Duncan examined the relationship between peer acceptance and
athletic competence in a sample of 8-13 year-old summer sport-camp
participants. Presumably, athletic ability is a highly desirable
attribute in this physical activity setting because it impacts success
in almost every situation in the camp. In other words, high athletic
competence likely provides multiple opportunities to gain friendships
and peer-status by consistently helping one's team to be successful
or competitive. In the school setting, however, athletic ability may
provide these opportunities on a less frequent basis. Thus, the degree
to which Weiss and Duncan's findings generalize to the regular
school setting is unknown.
Weiss and Duncan (1992) also interpreted their findings along a
continuum of peer acceptance, referring primarily to higher and lower
levels of peer acceptance. Although children who experience higher
levels of peer acceptance are typically well liked and have many friends
(i.e., popular children), children who have lower levels of peer
acceptance can be differentiated into those who are
"neglected" and those who are "rejected." Given the
importance of differentiating between neglected and rejected children
(see Asher & Wheeler, 1985), the present study sought to extend
Weiss and Duncan's findings by examining differences in perceived
athletic competence among children who were classified as
"popular" and those who were classified as
"rejected" (on the basis of peer nominations).
Although Boivin and Begin (1989) did examine differences in self-
and teacher-rated athletic competence levels between popular and
rejected elementary school children, Boivin and Begin did not ask the
children in their study to rate each others' athletic competence
levels. Therefore, building upon the previous work of Weiss and Duncan
(1992) and Boivin and Begin, we hypothesized that popular children would
(a) give themselves higher self-rated athletic ability ratings, and (b)
receive higher athletic ability ratings from peers than rejected
children. In accordance with theory, we also hypothesized that popular
children would report less loneliness than rejected children (Parker
& Asher, 1993). Overall, this study was designed to improve our
understanding of the potential role that athletic competence plays in
the experience of childhood loneliness and sociometric status. We
suggest that this is an important research endeavor because the results
have the potential to inform teachers and/or parents about positive or
healthy psycho-social experiences associated with the development of
athletic competence at a young age.
Method
Participants
Children were selected from 29 classes in seven elementary schools
from a western Canadian city. Information letters were distributed to
578 children in Grades 4 through 6. On average, 36% of children from
each class participated in the study, with a total of 99 boys (M age =
10.08 years, SD = 1.13) and 109 girls (M age = 10.05, SD = 1.03)
agreeing to participate.
Measures
Children completed self-report instruments to measure loneliness
levels in school, as well as perceived athletic ability. In addition,
participants rated the athletic ability of their classmates, and
identified classmates who they most liked and who they least liked in
order to assess the sociometric status of the in-class peer group.
Loneliness. Asher and Wheeler's (1985) modified
school-specific version of Asher, Hymel, and Renshaw's (1984)
Illinois Loneliness and Social Dissatisfaction Scale (ILSDS) was used to
assess children's levels of loneliness in school. The scale was
originally designed for use with 3rd through 6th grade children and
contains 24 items (eight of which are filler items that are not scored).
Using a 5-point scale (1 = Always true about me; 5 = Not at all true
about me), children rate the extent to which each of the items relates
to their own personal experiences in school. Ten of the 16 items are
reverse scored (e.g., "I feel lonely at school", "I feel
left out of things at school") and then all 16 items are summed to
provide a single composite score. Higher composite scale scores reflect
higher levels of school-based loneliness (Asher & Gazelle, 1999).
The ILSDS has been the most commonly used measure of loneliness in
children's peer-relation studies (Asher & Gazelle, 1999). Asher
and Wheeler (1985) conducted an exploratory factor analysis upon ILSDS
data provided by 200 children from Grades 3 through 6 and obtained a
single-factor solution. Asher and Wheeler also reported a very high
level of internal consistency (Cronbach's [alpha] = .90) for the
scale. Cassidy and Asher (1992) obtained a single-factor solution for
ILSDS data provided by 440 kindergarten and Grade 1 children, and again
reported adequate internal consistency ([alpha] = .79). A recent study
by Chen et al. (2004) with Brazilian (n = 807), Canadian (n = 274),
Chinese (n = 819) and Italian (n = 363) children in Grades 3 to 6 also
found high levels of internal consistency for the 16-item scale:[alpha]
= .84, .90, .95, and .84 respectively. In accordance with theory,
construct validity evidence for the ILSDS has been established in
studies where children who were classified as being "rejected"
by the peer group had significantly higher loneliness scores than
children who were classified as being "popular" by the peer
group (e.g., Asher & Wheeler; Cassidy & Asher; Parkhurst &
Asher, 1992). Overall, the ILSDS appears to possess adequate reliability
and validity characteristics.
It should be noted that in addition to asking direct questions
about loneliness (e.g., "I'm lonely at school"), as the
instrument's name suggests, the ILSDS also contains items that
focus on social dissatisfaction (e.g., "I have lots of friends in
my class," "I can find a friend in my class when I need
one"). However, for the sake of brevity, and because the ILSDS is
treated as a unidimensional instrument from which inferences about
loneliness are made (Asher & Wheeler, 1985), ILSDS results in this
study will refer only to loneliness rather than to loneliness and social
dissatisfaction (for a related discussion see Asher & Gazelle,
1999).
Athletic ability. Children were presented with a complete list of
all students in their classroom and asked to rate each classmate's
athletic ability. Specifically, participants responded to the
instruction, "Please tell us how good your classmates are at
playing Sports and doing physical skills during gym class" using a
5-point scale (1 = not very good, 3 =fairly good; 5 = very good).
Children were verbally instructed to provide a self-rating of their own
atkletic ability when they reached their own name on the class list.
Although we recognized the psychometric and measurement limitations of
using single item indicators to rate athletic ability, similar
single-item procedures have been used successfully in previous studies
that have measured perceived athletic competence in older populations
(e.g., Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1996; Lubbers et al., 2006; Senko
& Harackiewicz, 2002; Vallerand & Reid, 1984). Moreover, we were
concerned about the potential time (and fatigue) implications associated
with asking children to rate their own ability and the ability of their
classmates using multiple indicators.
Given that each class had different numbers of students and
different participation rates, mean scores for peer-ratings of athletic
ability were computed for each child to ensure that data could be
meaningfully compared across children. Thus, the peer rating of athletic
ability assigned to a child was computed by taking the composite
athletic ability score for the child (based upon the summation of scores
provided by the children in the class who participated in the study)
then dividing this composite score by the number of children who
provided a rating. For example, if 11 children from a class rated Jill,
and her composite score was 41, the peer-rated level of athletic ability
assigned to Jill was 3.73 (i.e., 41/11).
Sociometric status. Children were presented with a complete list of
the names of all students in their respective classes and given the
following set of instructions:
Imagine going on a school trip with your class. For the entire
trip, your teacher has told you to stay in a group with three other
people. Which three children from your class would you most like to
have in your group? Circle the names of the three people you would
most like to have in your group.
On a separate page, children were presented with another complete
list of children's names from their class and asked to identify
three children who they would least like to have in their group during
the trip. This type of positive and negative peer nomination procedure
has been widely used with elementary school children to obtain peer
acceptance and peer rejection information (e.g., Asher & Wheeler,
1985; Cassidy & Asher, 1992; Coie, Dodge, & Coppotelli, 1982).
The names within each class list were presented to children in random
orders for each respondent to minimize potential presentation order
effects.
The frequency of positive and negative peer nominations received by
each participant was adjusted by taking into consideration the number of
students in the class who provided peer nominations. For example, if
Jill received five positive peer nominations from the 11 other children
in her class who participated in the study, the "most-like"
score assigned to Jill would have been .45 (i.e., 5/11). Similarly, if
Jill received two least-like nominations, her least-like rating would
have been. 18 (i.e., 2/11). An overall sociometric status score was then
computed (see Cillesen & Bukowski, 2000) by subtracting the
least-like rating from the most-like rating. (In Jill's case, her
sociometric status score would have been .27 [i.e., .45 - .18]). Thus,
the final sociometric score assigned to a child could range from -1.0
(i.e., all participants in the class gave the child a least-like
nomination) to 1.0 (i.e., all participants in the class gave the child a
most-like nomination). Children with negative scores receive more
least-like nominations than most-like nominations, and children with
positive scores receive more most-like nominations than least-like
nominations. Larger negative scores are indicative of greater peer
rejection, and larger positive scores are indicative of greater peer
acceptance (Asher & Gazelle, 1999).
Procedures
Ethical clearance to conduct the study was obtained from the
University's human ethics research board and from the school
district prior to contacting individual schools. Information letters and
parental consent forms were sent home with all children from every
class. Only children who returned signed parental consent forms were
allowed to participate in the study. Given that the research team only
had access to information about the age and gender of participants, it
was not possible to determine if any systematic differences existed
between participants and non-participants in this study. In other words,
no deviant case analysis was possible. Each child who participated in
the study was removed from the classroom and individually tested in a
quiet location under the supervision of a research assistant. Completion
time for the questionnaire package was approximately 20 minutes. A total
of 55 children (26 boys, 29 girls: Mage = 10.13 years, SD = 1.02) were
re-tested to permit the assessment of test-retest reliability for the
loneliness responses (i.e., ILSDS scores) and athletic ability ratings.
Average time between the two test administrations was 3.91 days (SD =
1.76). The test administrator provided oral directions for the
participants when requested by children to do so.
Results
Preliminary Data Analyses
Loneliness. To ensure that the ILSDS (Asher & Wheeler, 1985)
was functioning in accordance with theoretical expectations, a Principal
Components analysis was conducted upon the correlation matrix of the
ILSDS data (N= 208). Five eigenvalues > 1.0 were obtained
([[lambda].sub.1] = 4.81, [[lambda].sub.2] = 1.33, [[lambda].sub.3] =
1.16, [[lambda].sub.4] = 1.09, [[lambda].sub.2] = 1.01), however,
examination of the eigenvalue scree plot (using Cattell's [1978]
scree criteria for interpretation purposes) strongly suggested the
retention of a single factor. Velicer, Eaton, and Fava (2000)
recommended that one of the best ways to determine the number of factors
is to use parallel analysis in conjunction with Cattell's scree
test. Results of a parallel analysis using procedures described by
Lautenschlager (1989) indicated that a single factor be retained (i.e.,
only the first eigenvalue exceeded the criterion eigenvalue in the
parallel analysis). Given that previous research has also suggested a
single-factor solution (i.e., Asher & Wheeler, 1985), one factor was
chosen to represent the latent dimensionality of the 16 items. Factor
loadings ranged in magnitude from .33 to .67, indicating that all items
had meaningful loadings on the factor (see Gorsuch, 1983). Internal
consistency for the scale was acceptable ([alpha] = .83), and
test-retest reliability (as measured by an intraclass correlation coefficient [ICC]) for the composite scale score was also acceptable
([M.sub.Time 1] = 28.43, SD = 8.41 ; [M.sub.Time 2] = 27.20, SD = 9.61 ;
ICC = .77, p < .001). Overall, these results suggest that the ILSDS
had adequate internal validity and reliability characteristics in this
study.
Athletic ability ratings. Given that single-item indicators were
employed to measure students' ratings of their own athletic ability
and the athletic ability of their classroom peers, we examined the
test-retest reliability of these ratings using intraclass correlation
coefficients [ICC]. The ICC for self-ratings of athletic ability
([M.sub.Time 1] = 4.24, SD = .77; [M.sub.Time 2] = 4.33, SD = .75; n =
54) was .86 (p < .001), suggesting that the single-item self-ratings
of athletic ability were highly reliable across time.
To compute the ICC for athletic ability ratings of peers, a
classmate was selected at random from each participant's class
list. This selected child's athletic ability ratings at time 1 and
time 2 were then used for analytic purposes. Thus, for each of the
retested children (n = 55), we used the rating for the randomly selected
peer at time 1, and the rating for the same peer at time 2. The ICC for
ratings of peer athletic ability ([M.sub.Time 1] = 2.47, [SD.sub.Time 1]
= 1.12; [M.sub.Time 2] = 2.53, [SD.sub.Time 2] = 1.30) was .66 (p <
.001). Although the ICC was statistically significant, Vincent (1995)
suggests that ICCs > .70 in the behavioral sciences are desirable, so
the test-retest reliability of the peer-ratings of athletic ability must
be considered marginal, and subsequent results pertaining to this
variable should be interpreted with some degree of caution.
Gender differences. Prior to examining the relationships among
loneliness, athletic ability, and sociometric status, we screened the
data for potential gender differences using a one-way MANOVA. Gender was
entered as the independent variable. The four dependent variables were
loneliness (i.e., composite ILSDS score), self-rating of athletic
ability, peer rating of athletic ability, and sociometric status. Table
I contains the means and standard deviations for the four dependent
variables according to gender.
A significant multivariate test statistic was obtained: Wilks'
[LAMBDA] = .699, F (4,203) = 22.06,p < .001, partial [[eta].sup.2] =
.30. Follow up univariate F-tests revealed statistically significant
gender differences on three of the four dependent variables. On average,
boys gave themselves higher self-ratings of athletic ability (M = 4.51)
than girls (M = 3.96), boys received higher ratings of athletic ability
from their classmates (M = 4.11) than girls (M = 3.52), and boys
received lower sociometric status ratings from classmates (M = -.02)
than girls (M = .05). Given these differences, all subsequent analyses
were conducted separately upon data provided by boys and girls.
Inter-Variable Relationships
Bivariate correlations (r) were computed among the four variables
and are shown in Table 2. Fairly consistent patterns of correlations
were observed across gender. Notably, for both boys and girls,
significant negative correlations were observed between (a) loneliness
and the average athletic ability rating that children received from
their peers, and (b) loneliness and sociometric status. In other words,
boys and girls who reported lower levels of loneliness tended to receive
higher athletic ability ratings and higher sociometric ratings from
their peers. In addition, girls (but not boys) also indicated that
higher levels of self-rated athletic competence were associated with
lower levels of loneliness (r = -.28). Collectively, these results
suggest that loneliness experienced at school is related to perceptions
of athletic competence (both self and peer rated) and sociometric
status.
Also consistent across both sexes were the correlations between
athletic ability ratings provided by peers and sociometric status.
Specifically, higher levels of sociometric status were associated with
higher athletic ability ratings from peers. The positive correlation
between sociometric status and athletic ability (as judged by peers) was
particularly strong for girls (r = .49). These results suggest that
sociometric status may be linked to individuals' levels of publicly
displayed athletic ability.
Predicting Loneliness
Simultaneous multiple regression analyses were conducted separately
on boys' and girls' data sets to further examine the
relationships between loneliness and athletic ability and sociometric
status. Loneliness was regressed on self-rated athletic ability,
athletic ability as rated by peers, and sociometric status. Results of
these regression analyses are contained in Table 3. For boys, only
sociometric status was a significant predictor of loneliness ([beta] =
-.215), although the level of statistical significance for athletic
ability as rated by peers was marginal (p = .06). For girls, both
sociometric status ([beta] = -.243) and self-rated athletic ability
([beta] = -.224) were significant predictors of loneliness (ps <
.05). These results suggest that an understanding of children's
loneliness levels in school can be furthered by considering
children's sociometric status and their athletic competence
(whether perceived by the self in the case of girls, or judged by peers
in the case of boys).
Although the bivariate correlations (Table 2) between athletic
ability (as judged by peers) and sociometric status for boys and girls
were quite strong (r = .33 and .49 respectively), the athletic ability
rating that children received from peers was not a strong predictor of
loneliness in the regression analyses. We speculated that sociometric
status and athletic competence (as judged by peers) explained similar
parts of the variance in loneliness scores. Consequently, we removed the
sociometric status variable from the regression analyses to see if
athletic ability (as rated by peers) would then predict children's
loneliness scores. In these subsequent analyses, peer ratings of
athletic ability became significant predictors of loneliness for both
boys ([beta] = -.266, t = -2.688, p < .01) and girls ([beta] = -.263,
t = -2.788, p < .01).
Differences Between Popular and Rejected Children
By definition, popular children receive a large number of
most-liked peer nominations and few least-liked peer nominations from
their classmates. In contrast, rejected children receive many
least-liked peer nominations and few most-liked peer nominations. To
examine differences between popular children and rejected children on
the variables of interest in this study, an extreme-groups paradigm was
adopted whereby a select number of participants were assigned to a
"popular" group or a "rejected" group on the basis
of their sociometric standings.
Children who had a sociometric status score > 1 SD above the
mean (within their corresponding gender) were assigned to their
respective gender's popular group. In contrast, children who had a
sociometric status score > 1 SD below the mean (within their
corresponding gender) were assigned to their respective gender's
rejected group. A total of 15 boys (M = .35, SD = .11) and 11 girls (M =
.34, SD = .07) met the criterion for assignment to the popular groups,
and 15 boys (M = -.42, SD = .13) and 13 girls (M = -.37, SD = .12) met
the criterion for assignment to the rejected groups.
Separate MANOVAs for boys and girls were conducted to examine
between-group differences (i.e., popular vs rejected) on loneliness,
self-ratings of athletic ability, and peer-ratings of athletic ability.
Descriptive statistics for each group on the three dependent variables
are contained in Table 4. Significant multivariate test statistics were
obtained for both boys (Wilks' [LAMBDA] = .671, F [3, 26] = 4.25, p
< .05, partial [[eta].sup.2] = .33) and girls (Wilks' [LAMBDA]=
.491, F [3, 20] = 6.91 ,p < .005, partial [[eta].sup.2] = .51).
Follow up univariate F-tests revealed statistically significant
between-group differences on loneliness and athletic ability (as rated
by peers) for both boys and girls (see Table 4). These results indicate
that popular children, on average, reported less loneliness and received
higher athletic ability ratings from classroom peers than rejected
children.
Discussion
The first purpose of this study was to examine the relationship
between loneliness and perceived athletic ability (as rated by self and
peers). Results generally supported the hypothesis that children with
higher levels of perceived athletic competence (as rated by the self and
by peers) would experience less loneliness than children who had lower
levels of perceived athletic competence. As shown in Table 2, athletic
ability as rated by peers was negatively correlated with loneliness for
both boys and girls. Self-rated athletic ability also was negatively
correlated with loneliness for boys and girls, although the former
correlation was not statistically significant. While the correlational
design of this study precludes causal inferences, results of previous
research conducted with children who have movement difficulties (i.e.,
who demonstrate low athletic ability) may shed light on these
relationships.
The literature suggests that poorly skilled children are often
ridiculed in physical activity situations (Mandich et al., 2003) and as
a result may withdraw from participation in these settings to avoid
further embarrassment and humiliation (Fitzpatrick & Watkinson,
2003). In addition, low skilled children are sometimes excluded from
participation or relegated to peripheral roles in an activity (Evans
& Roberts, 1987). Evans and Roberts suggested that this voluntary
withdrawal or involuntary exclusion increases "the social distance
between [these children] ... and their peers" (p. 27), thereby
reducing opportunities to interact with peers and develop friendships.
This cycle of events can lead to increased feelings of sadness and
isolation (Fitzpatrick & Watkinson, 2003) that are commonly
associated with loneliness (see Cassidy & Asher, 1992).
In contrast to the previous explanation, we cannot rule out the
possibility that increased feelings of loneliness may actually influence
an individual's self-perceptions of athletic ability. Ernst and
Cacioppo (1999) suggest that loneliness provides a negative lens through
which people interpret the world. In other words, lonely individuals
have a tendency to engage in negative cognitive appraisals or
evaluations of their circumstances (Ernst & Cacioppo). To this end,
children who are higher in loneliness may be more inclined to evaluate
themselves in a negative way (including evaluations of their athletic
ability) in comparison to children who are less lonely. Further research
is clearly required to determine the causal pathways and processes that
underlie the relationships between loneliness and athletic competence
observed in this study.
As noted previously, the negative correlation between girls'
self-ratings of athletic ability and loneliness was statistically
significant (r = -.28), but this correlation was not significant for
boys (r = -.18). The means and standard deviations in Table 1 reveal a
potential ceiling effect in the boys' self-ratings of athletic
ability. Specifically, the mean rating for the boys was 4.51 (on a
5-point scale), with a standard deviation of .79. These descriptive
statistics suggest that there is a restriction in range in the
boys' self-ratings of athletic ability which in turn may have
attenuated the magnitude of the correlation with loneliness. This
ceiling effect may be influenced by the tendency for boys to
overestimate their athletic competence. As discussed by Cole et al.
(2001), boys tend to overestimate their own athletic competence because
they are more self-congratulatory than girls. The significance tests in
Table 1 appear to corroborate the potential validity of this position
given that boys had significantly higher self-ratings of athletic
ability than girls (p < .001, effect size = .68).
The second purpose of this study was to examine levels of perceived
athletic ability and loneliness as a function of peer status. The
hypothesis that popular children would receive higher athletic ability
ratings from peers in comparison to rejected peers was supported. This
is consistent with the findings of previous research showing that higher
levels of peer acceptance or sociometric status correspond to higher
levels of other-rated athletic competence (e.g., Weiss & Duncan,
1992). As discussed previously, athletic competence has been identified
as a highly valued attribute (Chase & Dummer, 1992; Eccles et al.,
1993) and an important criterion for social acceptance among school
children (Boivin & Begin, 1989; Buchanan et al., 1976). Although the
criteria for sociometric nominations were not assessed in this study,
the current results demonstrate support for Weiss and Stuntz's
(2004) conclusion that "athletic ability (actual and perceived) and
popularity ... are strongly linked for children" (p. 175).
Contrary to our hypothesis (and previous research findings: Boivin
& Begin, 1989; Lubbers et al., 2006; Weiss & Duncan, 1992),
self-rated perceptions of athletic ability did not differ as a function
of sociometric status (see Table 4). Although Boivin and Begin observed
a cluster of popular children in their sample who had significantly
higher self-perceptions of athletic competence than a cluster of
rejected children, they also reported a cluster of rejected children who
had high self-perceptions of athletic competence. The researchers
suggested that this latter cluster's overly favorable self-evaluations of competence served as a self-protective mechanism to
preserve and enhance self-esteem. It is possible that a similar
phenomenon was operating here.
Finally, as expected, popular children reported less loneliness
than their rejected peers (see Table 4). This result was similar for
both boys and girls, and is consistent with previous research in the
area of children's loneliness and sociometric status (e.g., Asher
& Wheeler, 1985; Crick & Ladd, 1993).
The results of this study were generally consistent with
expectations based on theory and previous work. Nevertheless, the study
is not without limitations. First, the average classroom participation
rate was quite small (i.e., 36%), introducing the possibility that
certain groups of children may have been systematically under or over
represented in our sample. Second, we used single item indicators to
assess participants' athletic ability ratings of themselves and of
their classmates. Concerns about single item indicators generally center
on their lack of reliability and validity evidence. Although multiple
item indicators of athletic competence would overcome this problem, the
single item indicators used in this study did demonstrate adequate
levels of test-retest reliability, thereby increasing our confidence in
the findings. Third, we acknowledge that relying solely upon self-report
data of young children may be problematic because younger children
(compared to older children) are sometimes less reliable in their use of
self-report protocols (see Mellor, 2004). Consequently, future research
may be strengthened if sociometric ratings and athletic competence
ratings were also provided by teachers who observe the children on a
daily basis.
Given the negative impact of loneliness on psychological and
emotional well being, identifying the relative contributions of factors
that influence children's sociometric status (and loneliness) is an
important research endeavor. Factors such as physical appearance,
athletic competence, and social skills are believed to affect
children's peer acceptance (Page & Scanlan, 1994). Although
some researchers have even suggested that "athletic skills may be
more central to popularity than social competence" (Boivin &
Begin, 1989, p. 594), the mechanisms and processes by which these
factors influence peer acceptance require more research.
Author's Note
This research was supported in part by funding from the Sport,
Recreation, and Lotteries Branch of Saskatchewan Municipal Government
and by a standard research grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities
Research Council of Canada (SSHRC).
References
Asher, S. R., & Gazelle, H. (1999). Loneliness, peer relations,
and language disorder in childhood. Topics in Language Disorders, 19
(2), 16-33.
Asher, S. R., Hymel, S., & Renshaw, P. D. (1984). Loneliness in
children. Child Development, 55, 1457-1464.
Asher, S. R., & Paquette, J. A. (2003). Loneliness and peer
relations in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
12(3), 75-78.
Asher, S. R., & Wheeler, V. A. (1985). Children's
loneliness: A comparison of rejected and neglected peers. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 500-505.
Boivin, M., & Begin, G. (1989). Peer status and self-perception
among early elementary school children: The case of the rejected
children. Child Development, 60, 591-596.
Boivin, M., & Hymel, S. (1997). Peer experiences and social
self-perceptions: A two-stage mediational model. Developmental
Psychology, 33, 135-145.
Buchanan, H. T., Blankenbaker, J., & Cotten, D. (1976).
Academic and athletic ability as popularity factors in elementary school
children. Research Quarterly, 47, 320-325.
Buhs, E. S., & Ladd, G. W. (2001). Peer rejection as an
antecedent of young children's school adjustment: An examination of
mediating processes. Developmental Psychology, 3 7, 550-560.
Bullock, J. R. (1993). Lonely children. Young Children, 48, 53-57.
Cassidy, J., & Asher, S. R. (1992). Loneliness and peer
relations in young children. Child Development, 63,350-365.
Cattell, R. B. (1978). The scientific use of factor analysis in
behavioral and life sciences. New York: Plenum Press.
Chase, M. A., & Dummer, G. M. (1992). The role of sports as a
social status determinant for children. Research Quarterly for Exercise
and Sport, 63, 418-424.
Chen, X., He, Y., De Olivera, A. M., Coco, A. L., Zappulla, C.,
Kaspar, V., et al. (2004). Loneliness and social adaptation in
Brazilian, Canadian, Chinese, and Italian children: a multi-national
comparative study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45,
1373-1384.
Cillessen, A. H. N., & Bukowski, W. M. (2000). Recent advances
in the measurement of acceptance and rejection in the peer system. San
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral
sciences (revised ed.). New York: Academic Press.
Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). Dimensions
and types of social status: A cross-age perspective. Developmental
Psychology, 18, 557-570.
Coie, J. D., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1983). Abehavioral analysis
of emerging social status in boys' groups. Child Development, 54,
1400-1416.
Cole, D. A., Cho, S., Martin, J. M., Seroczynski, A. D., Tram, J.,
& Hoffman, K. (2001). Effects of validity and bias on gender
differences in the appraisal of children's competence: Results of
MTMM analyses in a longitudinal investigation. Structural Equation
Modeling, 8, 84-107.
Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1996). Children's
treatment by peers: Victims of relational and overt aggression.
Development and Psychopathology, 8, 367-380.
Crick, N. R., Grotpeter, J. K., & Rockhill, C. M. (1999). A
social-informantion-processing approach to children's loneliness.
In K. J. Rotenberg & S. Hymel (Eds.), Loneliness in childhood and
adolescence (pp. 153-175). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Crick, N. R., & Ladd, G. W. (1993). Children's perceptions
of their peer experiences: Attributions, loneliness, social anxiety, and
social avoidance. Developmental Psychology, 29, 1-11.
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Harold, R. D., & Blumenfeld, P.
(1993). Age and gender differences in children's self- and task
perceptions during elementary school. Child Development, 64, 830-847.
Eder, D., & Kinney, D. A. (1995). The effect of middle school
extracurricular activities on adolescents' popularity and peer
status. Youth & Society, 26, 298-324.
Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and
avoidance achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: A mediation analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 461-475.
Ernst, J. M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1999). Lonely hearts:
Psychological perspectives on loneliness. Applied & Preventive
Psychology, 8, 1-22.
Evans, J., & Roberts, G. C. (1987). Physical competence and the
development of children's peer relations. Quest, 39, 23-35.
Fizpatrick, D. A., & Watkinson, E. J. (2003). The lived
experience of physical awkwardness: Adults' retrospective views.
Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 20, 279-297.
Galanski, E. P., & Kalantzi-Azizi, A. (1999). Loneliness and
social dissatisfaction: Its relation with children's self-efficacy
for peer interaction. Child Study Journal, 29(1), 1-22.
Gifford-Smith, M. E., & Brownell, C. A. (2003). Childhood peer
relationships: Social acceptance, friendships, and peer networks.
Journal of School Psychology, 41, 235-284.
Gorsuch, R. L. (1983). Factor analysis (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, N J:
Erlbaum.
Hymel, S., Tarulli, D., Thomson, L. H., & Terrell-Deutsch, B.
(1999). Loneliness through the eyes of children. In K. J. Rotenberg
& S. Hymel (Eds.), Loneliness in childhood and adolescence (pp.
80-106). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Kristensen, K. (1995). The lived experience of childhood
loneliness: A phenomenological study. Issues in Comprehensive Nursing,
18, 125-137.
Kunesh, M., Hasbrook, C. A., & Lewthwaite, R. (1992). Physical
activity socialization: Peer interactions and affective responses among
a sample of sixth grade girls. Sociology of Sport Journal, 9, 385-396.
Lautenschlager, G. J. (1989). A comparison of alternatives to
conducting Monte Carlo analyses for determining parallel analysis
criteria. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 24, 365-395.
Lubbers, M. J., van der Werf, M. P. C., Kuyper, H., & Offringa,
G. J. (2006). Predicting peer acceptance in Dutch youth: A multilevel analysis. Journal of Early Adolescence, 26, 4-35.
Mandich, A.D., Polatajko, H.J., & Rodger, S. (2003). Rites of
passage: Understanding participation of children with developmental
coordination disorder. Human Movement Science, 22,583-595.
Mellor, D. (2004). Furthering the use of the Strengths and
Difficulties Questionnaire: Reliability with younger child respondents.
Psychological Assessment, 16, 396-401.
Page, R. M. (1990). Loneliness and adolescent health behavior.
Health Education, 21 (5), 14-17.
Page, R. M. (1991). Loneliness as a risk factor in adolescent
hopelessness. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 189-195.
Page, R. M., Frey, J., Talbert, R., & Falk, C. (1992).
Children's feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction:
Relationship measures of physical fitness and activity. Journal of
Teaching in Physical Education, 11, 211-219.
Page, R. M., & Scanlan, A. (1994). Childhood loneliness and
isolation: Implications and strategies for childhood educators. Child
Study Journal, 24, 107-118.
Parker, J. Ct, & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship and friendship
quality in middle childhood: Links with peer group acceptance, and
feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. Developmental
Psychology, 29, 611-621.
Parkhurst, J. T., & Asher, S. R. (1992). Peer rejection in
middle school: Subgroup differences in behavior, loneliness, and
interpersonal concerns. Developmental Psychology, 28, 231-241.
Parkhurst, J. T., & Hopmeyer, A. (1999). Developmental change
in the sources of loneliness in childhood: Constructing a theoretical
model. In K. J. Rotenberg & S. Hymel (Eds.), Loneliness in childhood
and adolescence (pp. 56-79). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ponzetti, J. J. (1990). Loneliness among college students. Family
Relations, July, 336-340.
Portman, P. A. (1995). Who is having fun in physical education
classes? Experiences of sixth-grade students in elementary and middle
schools. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 14, 445-453.
Rose, B., Larkin, D., & Berger, B. G. (1997). Coordination and
gender influences on the perceived competence of children. Adapted
Physical Activity Quarterly, 14, 210-221.
Senko, C., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2002). Performance goals: The
moderating roles of context and achievement orientation. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 603-610.
Weiss, M. R., & Duncan, S. C. (1992). The relation between
physical competence and peer acceptance in the context of
children's sport participation. Journal of Sport & Exercise
Psychology, 14, 177-191.
Weiss, M. R., & Stuntz, C. P. (2004). A little friendly
competition: Peer relationships and psychosocial development in youth
sport and physical activity contexts. In M. R. Weiss (Ed.),
Developmental sport and exercise psychology: A lifespan perspective (pp.
165-196). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology.
Vallerand, R. J., & Reid, G. (1984). On the causal effects of
perceived competence on intrinsic motivation: A test of cognitive
evaluation theory. Journal of Sport Psychology, 6, 94-102.
Velicer, W. F., Eaton, C.A., & Fava, J. L. (2000). Construct
explication through factor or component analysis: A review and
evaluation of alternative procedures for determining the number of
factors or components. In R.D. Goffin & E. Helmes (Eds.), Problems
and solutions in human assessment: Honoring Douglas N. Jackson at
seventy (pp. 41-71). Boston, MA: Kluwer.
Vincent, W. J. (1995). Statistics in kinesiology. Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics.
Janice Causgrove Dunn, John G. H. Dunn and Angela Bayduza
University of Alberta
Address Correspondence To: Dr. Janice Causgrove Dunn, Faculty of
Physical Education & Recreation, E-488 Van Vliet Centre, University
of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada T6G 2H9. Phone: (780)-492-0580.
FAX: (780)-492-2364. e-mail: janice.causgrovedunn@ualberta.ca
Table 1. Means, Standard Deviations, and Univariate Test Statistics
for Between-Gender Comparisons
Gender
Boys Girls
Variables M (SD) M (SD)
Loneliness 27.64 (8.02) 27.49 (8.19)
Self-rating of
athletic ability 4.51 (0.79) 3.96 (0.83)
Athletic ability as
rated by peers 4.11 (0.59) 3.52 (0.66)
Sociometric status -0.02 (0.24) 0.05 (0.20)
Univariate test statistics
Variables F (dj) p ES (a)
Loneliness .018 (1,206) ns .02
Self-rating of
athletic ability 24.078 (1,206) <.001 .68
Athletic ability as
rated by peers 46.578 (1,206) <.001 .94
Sociometric status 5.160 (1,206) <.05 -.32
(a) Effect size (ES) was computed using Cohen's (1977) effect
size index (d) for independent samples.
Table 2. Correlations Between Loneliness, Athletic Ability
Ratings, and Sociometric Status for Boys and Girls
Loneliness Self-rating Athletic Socio-
of athletic ability rated metric
ability by peers status
Loneliness -- -.18 -.29 *** -.29 ***
Self-rating of
athletic ability -.28 *** -- .21 * .07
Athletic ability
as rated by peers -.32 *** .30 *** -- .33 ***
Sociometric
status -.32 *** .06 .49 **** -
Note. Correlations for boys (n = 99) are contained in the upper
triangular matrix, and correlations for girls (n = 109) are contained
in the lower triangular matrix.
* p < .05. ** p < .0l. *** p < .005. **** p < .001.
Table 3. Simultaneous Multiple Regression Analyses of Athletic Ability
Variables and Sociometric Status on Loneliness for Boys and Girls
Gender Variables [beta] t
Boys F(3,95)=5.185, p<.005, [R.sup.2]=.14
Self-rating of athletic ability -.122 -1.253
Athletic ability as rated by peers -.195 -1.897
Sociometric status -.215 -2.132
Girls F(3,105)=7.956, p<.001, [R.sup.2]=.19
Self-rating of athletic ability -.224 -2.416
Athletic ability as rated by peers -.136 -1.275
Sociometric status -.243 -2.391
Semipartial
Gender Variables p Correlation
Boys F(3,95)=5.185, p<.005, [R.sup.2]=.14
Self-rating of athletic ability ns -.119
Athletic ability as rated by peers .061 -.180
Sociometric status <.05 -.203
Girls F(3,105)=7.956, p<.001, [R.sup.2]=.19
Self-rating of athletic ability <.05 -.213
Athletic ability as rated by peers ns -.112
Sociometric status <.05 -.211
Table 4. Means, Standard Deviations, and Univariate Test Statistics
for Between-Group Comparisons of Popular and Rejected Children
According to Gender
Sociometric Status
Popular Rejected
Gender Variables M (SD) M (SD)
Boys
Loneliness 25.40 (5.07) 33.20 (12.04)
Self-Rating of 4.40 (1.12) 4.44 (0.67)
Athletic Ability
Athletic Ability as 4.48 (0.50) 3.84 (0.60)
Rated by Peers
Girls
Loneliness 23.64 (5.51) 32.59 (8.92)
Self-Rating of 4.18 (0.75) 3.85 (0.67)
Athletic Ability
Athletic Ability as 3.87 (0.43) 2.85 (0.78)
Rated by Peers
Univariate test statistics
Gender Variables F (df) p ES (a)
Boys
Loneliness 5.346 (1, 28) <.05 0.84
Self-Rating of 0.012 (1, 28) ns 0.04
Athletic Ability
Athletic Ability as 10.282 (1, 28) <.005 1.09
Rated by Peers
Girls
Loneliness 8.348 (1, 22) <.01 1.18
Self-Rating of 1.304 (1, 22) ns 0.46
Athletic Ability
Athletic Ability as 14.773 (1, 22) <.001 1.58
Rated by Peers
Note. Group sizes: popular boys (n = 15), rejected boys (n = 15),
popular girls (n = 11), rejected girls (n = 13).
(a) Effect size (ES) was computed using Cohen's (1977) effect size
index (d) for independent samples.