The nature of athlete leadership.
Loughead, Todd M. ; Hardy, James ; Eys, Mark A. 等
Leadership has been assigned great importance in sport by athletes,
coaches, and spectators (Chelladurai & Riemer, 1998). Despite the
value placed on leadership, Riemer and Chelladurai (1995) noted that
leadership research in sport has been sparse and sporadic. In fact, the
majority of leadership research in sport has focused on the coach (see
Chelladurai, 1994; Chelladurai & Riemer, 1998 for reviews). This is
not surprising given that the coach is typically the one responsible for
making final decisions in regard to several team matters (e.g.,
strategy, tactics, team personnel). Nonetheless, there is another
important source of leadership within teams--the athletes. Gould, Hodge,
Peterson, and Peflichkoff (1987) noted coaches believe athlete
leadership to be an important component for effective team performance.
Further to this, Glenn and Horn (1993) suggested that coaches require
one or two athletes within the team who can provide motivation and
direction to their teammates. Therefore, it is not surprising that
coaches conventionally either have team elections or appoint an athlete
to serve as captain (or assistant captain) in order to provide
leadership to the team.
The designation of an athlete as captain may be viewed from a role
perspective as fulfilling a formal function (Loughead & Hardy,
2005). A formal leader can be viewed as an individual who has been
prescribed that position by the organization (e.g., coach) or group
(e.g., team elections). However, the election or appointment of a formal
leader does not ensure that the leadership provided will be effective or
fulfill team leadership needs (Glenn & Horn, 1993). Mabry and Barnes
(1980) identified another type of role that an individual can occupy
within a group--informal role. An informal role (in this case an
informal leader) emerges as a result of the interactions that occur
among group members. Taken together, athletes who occupy either a formal
or informal leadership role within their team represent a source of
athlete leadership.
To date, research examining athlete leadership has mostly been
descriptive in nature. Yukelson, Weinberg, Richardson, and Jackson (1983) examined the characteristics of collegiate athletes who were
rated high as leaders compared to those rated lower in leadership
status. The results showed that athletes scoring high in leadership
status, as rated by their peers, tended to be better performers, had
more seniority on their team, and had a greater internal locus of
control than teammates who were rated low in leadership status. Another
line of study in athlete leadership has been to examine the playing
position of team captains (i.e., formal leaders). Lee, Coburn, and
Partridge (1983) hypothesized that soccer players occupying a central
playing position (i.e., center fullback, midfield) were more likely to
be designated team captains since they occupied positions requiring high
interactions with others. The results showed that for professional
soccer teams, captains were more likely to occupy the center fullback
position, whereas in high school teams, captains were more likely to be
center fullbacks or midfielders. Taken together, the results indicated
that captains were more likely to occupy central playing positions on
soccer teams regardless of the level of competition. Finally, Tropp and
Landers (1979) examined both formal and informal leadership in
collegiate female field hockey players. Contrary to Lee et al., the
results showed that captains were not as likely to be playing a
high-interacting position (e.g., center halfback, center fullback).
Although previous research does provide some insights into athlete
leadership, this body of literature does have its shortcomings. A first
shortcoming has been the lack of a clear and consistent definition of
athlete leadership. This lack of definitional clarity has led to
inconsistent measurement of the construct, which in turn has led to
research findings that cannot be compared to one another. In an attempt
to remedy this situation, it may be beneficial to look at broader
definitions of leadership. In fact, over the last five decades there
have been approximately 65 different taxonomies used to define
leadership (Fleishman, Mumford, Zaccaro, Levin, Korotkin, & Hein,
1991). Despite the various ways leadership has been conceptualized,
Northouse (2001) identified several components that are central to
leadership: (a) leadership is a process, (b) leadership involves
influence, (c) leadership occurs within a group context, and (d)
leadership involves goal attainment. Using these components, Northouse
defined leadership as "a process whereby an individual influences a
group of individuals to achieve a common goal" (p. 3).
Consequently, when leadership is defined in this manner, it becomes
available to everyone, meaning that it is not restricted to only the
formal designated leader(s) of a group (Northouse, 2001). That is,
leadership not only involves formal leaders (e.g., coach, team captains)
but also informal leaders--those individuals who are not designated as
leaders by the organization and their role is not related to a formal
position within the organization hierarchy (Neubert, 1999). Therefore,
athlete leadership maybe viewed as an athlete occupying a formal or
informal role within a team who influences a group of team members
(i.e., a minimum of two team members) to achieve a common goal.
A second shortcoming of previous athlete leadership research has
been to focus primarily on one function of the leadership role--task
related functions. However, many researchers studying groups readily
agree that leaders serve two important functions internal to the team:
(a) task functions (e.g., helping the group accomplish its task
objectives), and (b) social functions (e.g., satisfying member needs)
(Carton, Hausenblas, & Eys, 2005; Kegler Hill, 2001; Rees, 1983).
The notion that leaders can occupy specific and varying roles within a
team, known as role differentiation, was examined by Rees and Segal
(1984). They examined the two internal functions (i.e., task and social
functions) of athlete leaders on two collegiate football teams. Overall,
they found that all of the task leaders were starters, while the social
leaders were equally divided between starters and non-starters. In
addition, they found that task leaders were spread out amongst seniors,
juniors, and sophomores, 33%, 56%, and 11% respectively and that social
leaders were mostly seniors (90%).
Related to the above point, a third shortcoming is that research
involving athlete leadership has focused almost exclusively on the
internal leadership functions. Leaders serve two critical functions
internal to the team--a task function and a social function. However,
the examination of only internal functions is somewhat limiting since
Kogler Hill (2001) has suggested that when the research focus moves from
the laboratory to the more naturalistic field setting, a third critical
leadership function related to the external environment is present.
Thus, in addition, to having to deal with the internal needs of the
group (task and social issues); a leader may also have to assist the
group to adapt and cope with the external environment. In fact, Mosher (1979) noted that one of the roles of team captains on sport teams was
to represent the team at external functions such as receptions,
meetings, and press conferences.
A fourth shortcoming is that the majority of studies examining
athlete leadership have utilized a cross-sectional design (e.g., Glenn
& Horn, 1993; Lee et al., 1983; Rees & Segal, 1984; Yukelson et
al., 1983), meaning that they have examined this construct at only one
moment in time--either prior to the start of the season or during the
season. Consequently, this begs the question as to whether there are
changes in athlete leadership status over the course of the season. This
is based on the premise that leadership is an ongoing process whereby
all members have the opportunity to engage in this process and assist
the team to adapt to changing situations (Kogler Hill, 2001).
Furthermore, a group is dynamic in nature meaning that it changes over
time as members join or leave the group, as norms and roles develop, and
as the group's foci changes from one task to another (Forsyth,
1998). For example, a sport team may shift focus over the course of the
season from learning strategic plays to implementing and executing the
plays in a real-life competitive context.
A final shortcoming is the absence of research examining the
relative influence of athlete leadership within a team. While every team
member can theoretically become a leader (Northouse, 2001); some athlete
leaders' influence will be more dominant than others. Thus, their
ability to influence a larger number of teammates is greater. Therefore,
these individuals offer a more pronounced leadership role within a team,
which we have labeled team leader since a majority of team members are
in agreement as to the leadership provided by these individuals. On the
other hand, there are individuals who offer leadership to a fewer number
of teammates. While their influence may not be as widespread as team
leaders, this type of leadership still can exert influence on its group
members; which we have labeled peer leadership. Although no research has
attempted to explicitly distinguish between team and peer leaders, it
appears that Rees and Segal (1984) may have inadvertently made a
distinction between team and peer leaders. The authors asked collegiate
football players from two teams to list five team members who exhibited
the highest amounts of task and social leadership. The results showed
that a total of 40 players out of 101 surveyed were nominated at least
once for either type of leadership. However, Rees and Segal noted
"it is still unreasonable to count as leaders all 40 players
nominated" (p. 115). As a result, they examined only those athletes
they called "top leaders"--the ten athletes scoring the
highest in social leadership and the ten athletes scoring the highest in
task leadership. It could be argued that those 40 athletes that were
initially nominated were indeed peer leaders while the "top
leaders", given that they were nominated the most, could in fact be
considered team leaders. However, the question then arises: Why were the
top 10 chosen as team leaders? Why not the top 5? By arbitrarily
choosing the number of athletes as team leaders, the likelihood that
some athletes may be misclassified is increased--those athletes who
should be considered team leaders are not and those athletes who are may
not have the consensus of their teammates to truly be considered a team
leader. The issue then becomes how much agreement or consensus is
required for an athlete to be considered a team leader? One of the most
common methods for measuring consensus is calculating the percentage of
individuals within a population (e.g., team) who endorse a particular
belief (e.g., leadership) (Conway & Schaller, 1998). In fact, Shaw
(1981) suggested 50% agreement should be the minimum standard for a
belief to be considered as consensus. Consequently, a minimum of 50%
agreement was required for an athlete to be considered a team leader. On
the other hand, a peer leader can be viewed as an athlete who influenced
at least two teammates.
Given the aforementioned shortcomings, the purpose of the present
study was threefold. The first purpose was to examine some of the
characteristics of identified team and peer leaders serving task,
social, and external leadership functions. More specifically, the
formalization of the athletes' leadership roles (i.e., formal vs.
informal), starting status, and tenure on team were the characteristics
that were assessed. Consequently, a number of hypotheses were put forth.
Using previous research by Loughead and Hardy (2005) as a basis, it was
hypothesized that athlete leaders would occupy both informal and formal
roles on their respective teams. Also, based on research previously
outlined by Rees and Segal (1984) and Yukelson et al. (1983), it was
hypothesized that individuals with longer tenure on their teams would
more likely be identified as leaders. Furthermore, based on the work of
Rees and Segal, it was hypothesized that starters (as compared to
non-starters) would be more likely to occupy leadership roles. However,
no specific a priori hypotheses were advanced for each type of
leadership function (task, social, and external).
A second purpose was to determine the amount of team and peer
leadership present on interactive sport teams. Given that the present
study examined team and peer leadership within a variety of interactive
team sports (e.g., hockey, basketball) with varying roster sizes, it is
important to have a statistic that is usable with a wide range of group
sizes when interested in quantifying the number of team and peer
leaders. One approach used in the organizational literature has been to
divide the number of peer leaders by the number of group members. This
method has been termed leader dispersion (Neubert, 1999), and may be
viewed as the concentration of leaders within the group. Accordingly,
team and peer leader dispersion values were calculated for the three
functions of leadership. Due to a lack of prior research in this area,
no a priori hypotheses were advanced.
A third purpose was to determine the stability of team and peer
leadership over time. That is, whether athletes identified as leaders at
the start of the season were also recognized as providing leadership at
the end of the season. In order to provide more detailed information,
the characteristics and dispersion values of team and peer leaders
across the three leadership functions (task, social, and external) were
also examined at these two different time points. As noted earlier,
previous athlete leadership studies in sport has been cross-sectional in
nature. Thus, given the exploratory nature of the present study using a
prospective design to examine leadership stability, no a priori
hypotheses were proposed.
Method
Participants
The participants were 258 varsity student athletes (140 males and
118 females) from two Canadian universities representing a total of
thirteen teams; seven teams consisted of female athletes and six of male
athletes. The mean age of the participants was 20.6 years (SD = 2.10,
range = 17-31 yrs). To enhance the generalizability of the results, the
athletes represented a variety of interactive team sports including
rugby (n = 69), soccer (n = 57), field lacrosse (n = 37), volleyball (n
= 40), ice hockey (n = 21), field hockey (n = 19), and basketball (n =
15). On average, the athletes had participated in their respective sport
for 9.20 years (SD = 4.47). As for tenure on current team, there were 94
first-year players, 76 second-year players, 58 third-year, 26
fourth-year players, and four fifth-year players. The study utilized a
prospective design to allow for the examination of leadership at two
times in the season; a total of 258 participants completed the measures
at Time 1 and 171 at Time 2 from the thirteen teams sampled.
In order to determine if there were systematic explanations for the
attrition rate (34%) in participants a review of demographic
characteristics was conducted across time periods. The results
demonstrated that those athletes who missed the second data collection
period were similar to those who were present. More specifically,
demographic information for those not completing Time 2 information was
proportionally equivalent to the original Time 1 sample with regard to
gender, age, sport type, and tenure on their respective teams.
Measures
Athlete leadership was measured by having the participants list all
of the athlete leaders on their respective teams. In order to measure
the three functions of leaders (task, social, and external leadership),
three separate questions were asked. Specifically, participants were
asked to "List the names of team members (including yourself if
applicable) you feel most strongly contribute to your team's
task/social/external factors. That is, please list team members who do
or have done at least one, some, or all of the following actions."
Following these instructions a list of behavioral characteristics of
task, social, and external leadership were provided to give the
participants a frame of reference (see Table 1). These behavioral
characteristics were adapted from Kogler Hill's (2001) team
leadership model and a previous version of the survey piloted with a
university aged, mixed gender soccer team resulted in minor changes
being suggested. Participants were given ample space to list the names
of individuals who performed these tasks. As well, basic descriptive
data were also collected on each participant that included age, gender,
sport, competitive level, starting status, formal leadership status
(e.g., captain, assistant captain), and tenure on the team.
Participants' responses were first scored via raw frequencies.
In other words, team leaders were classified as such if at least half of
their team members who responded to the questionnaire endorsed them as a
task, social, or external leader. Conversely, task, social, or external
peer leaders were classified as such if at least two team members
endorsed them as leaders. In order to create dispersion values, the
number of identified peer and team task, social, and external leaders
were then divided by respective team sizes. Specifically, team size was
operationalized as team roster size (Carron, Hausenblas, & Eys,
2005).
Procedures
Coaches were contacted to outline the study and ask permission to
administer the survey to the athletes on their teams. After securing
approval from the coaches, athletes were approached by the researchers
and a complete description of the project was provided. Informed consent
was implied through completion of the questionnaire. Specifically, the
participants were informed that they would need to complete the
questionnaire once at the beginning of the season--at least three weeks
into the season (Time 1) and the same questionnaire would again be
administered within three weeks of the end of the regular season (Time
2). The timing of the questionnaire at Time 1 allowed for athletes to
develop perceptions of who were the team and peer leaders. The timing of
the questionnaire at Time 2 provided an equal basis for comparison since
not all of the teams would have qualified for a spot in post-season play
and could potentially confound the findings. Participants were assured
anonymity and confidentiality of their responses. The two
administrations of the questionnaires occurred at the convenience of the
team, usually following a practice session and lasted approximately 20
minutes.
Results
Description of Task Athlete Leadership
Team task leaders. Overall, the results at Time 1 showed a team
task leadership dispersion ratio of. 15--indicating that 15% of athletes
were viewed as a task leader at the team level (see Table 2 for a
summary of dispersion values). In terms of the characteristics of these
task team leaders, approximately two thirds (65%) served a formal
leadership role (i.e., captain or assistant captain) within the team. In
addition, nearly all of these task team leaders were starters and three
quarters of them were either in their third (49%) or fourth (26%) season
on the team. A summary of the personal characteristics of athlete
leaders is provided in Table 3. As can be seen from Tables 2 and 3, a
similar picture for team leader dispersion and leadership
characteristics emerged at Time 2.
Peer task leaders. A peer leader dispersion value of .35 was found
at Time 1 indicating that 35% of athletes were viewed as peer leaders on
their respective teams. Two-thirds (66%) of these peer task leaders were
informal leaders. That is, they did not hold any formal leadership
designation within the team such as captain or assistant captain. A
large majority of these players were starters (81%) and their tenure
varied with 40% in their third season, a quarter (26%) in their fourth
season, and another quarter (24%) in their second season with their
team. Inspection of Table 2 and 3 revealed a similar pattern for peer
task leaders at Time 2.
Stability of team and peer task leadership. The results showed
there were a total of 40 team task leaders. In particular, the results
indicated that 73% of the team task leaders were nominated at both Time
1 and Time 2, while 23% were only nominated at time 1, and 4% were only
nominated at Time 2. As for peer task leadership, a total of 88 peer
task leaders were identified. Specifically, the results indicated a
total of 70% of peer task leaders were nominated at both Time 1 and Time
2, 13% were nominated only at Time 1 and 17% only at Time 2. Taken
together the results suggest that the majority of athletes identified as
team or peer task leaders at the beginning of the season were likely to
be identified again as task leaders near the end of the season.
Description of Social Athlete Leadership
Team social leaders. The results from Time 1 showed a team social
leadership dispersion ratio of .11. Thus, 11% of athletes were viewed as
team social leaders. With regard to the characteristics of these
leaders, just over half (57%) served a formal leadership role (i.e.,
captain or assistant captain) within the team, nearly all were starters
(93%), and over two-thirds (70%) were in their third or fourth season
with their respective teams. The data collected at Time 2 displayed a
compatible set of results to those just described for Time 1.
Peer social leaders. The results obtained for Time 1 revealed a
peer social leadership dispersion ratio of .47--that is, nearly half of
the athletes were viewed as a peer social leader. Nearly three-quarters
of these peer social leaders (74%) were informal leaders, two-thirds
(66%) were starters, and just over half (55%) were in their third or
fourth season with their team. Again, results associated with Time 2
illustrated a similar description of peer social leaders to those
previously depicted for Time 1.
Stability of team and peer social leadership. The results revealed
that 32 athletes were nominated as team social leaders. More
specifically, 53% of these team social leaders were nominated at both
Time 1 and Time 2, 22% were nominated only at Time I, and 25% at Time 2
only. For peer social leadership, there were a total of 117 peer social
leaders. In particular, 62% of peer social leaders were nominated at
both Time 1 and Time 2, 21% were nominated only at Time 1 and 17% for
Time 2 only. The overall results suggest that the majority of athletes
identified as team or peer social leaders at the beginning of the season
are likely to be identified again as (team or peer) leaders near the end
of the season. However, it would appear that team, and to a lesser
extent peer social leadership, was not as stable across the season as
task leadership.
Description of External Athlete Leadership
Team external leaders. For Time 1, a team external leadership
dispersion ratio of .08 was found, indicating that 8% of athletes were
viewed as team external leaders. A large majority of these team external
leaders (79%) were formal leaders, almost all were starters, and most
were either in their third or fourth season with their respective teams
(88%). As with the task and social leadership functions, Time 2 data for
team leaders for the external function was congruent with Time 1.
Peer external leaders. The Time I results revealed a peer external
leadership dispersion ratio of .31--thus, 31% of athletes were viewed as
peer external leaders. Results indicated that nearly two-thirds (63%) of
these peer leaders had an informal leadership role within their team and
three-quarters of these athletes (75%) were starters. With regard to
team tenure, a little less than two-thirds (62%) of these identified
leaders were either third or fourth year players. Similar set of results
were exhibited for peer external leaders identified at Time 2.
Stability of team and peer external leadership. A total of 24
athletes were identified as team external leaders. More specifically, it
was found that 71% of team external leaders were nominated at both Time
1 and Time 2, 13% were nominated only at time 1, and 16% only at Time 2.
For peer external leadership, there were a total of 81 peer external
leaders; 58% were nominated at both Time 1 and Time 2, 26% were
nominated only at Time 1, and 16% only at Time 2. Similar to task and
social leadership, the results for external leadership suggested that
the majority of athletes identified as team or peer leaders at the
beginning of the season were also likely to be identified as team or
peer external leaders, respectively, near the end of the season.
Discussion
The purpose of the present study was three-fold. The first purpose
was to examine the personal characteristics of both team and peer
leaders. The second purpose was to determine the amount of team and peer
leadership on interactive sport teams. Finally, the third purpose was to
assess the stability of team and peer leadership over the course of a
season. In general, the presence of both team and peer leaders in each
of the three leadership functions (i.e., task, social, external) was
found. In this regard, the results of the present study are similar to
those of Rees and Segal (1984) who examined both task and social athlete
leaders. The present study, however, extended the current literature as
this was the first in-depth investigation to (a) draw a distinction
between and assess team and peer athlete leaders, and (b) determine the
presence of external leaders in sport.
Insofar as the first purpose is concerned, the present study
examined several personal characteristics of athlete leaders both at the
team and peer levels. It was hypothesized that formal and informal
leaders would occupy leadership roles with sport teams. Overall, the
results replicated Loughead and Hardy's (2005) finding that both
team captains and other teammates were sources of leadership. However,
the present study extended the current knowledge base by finding that
regardless of leadership function (task, social, external), formal
leaders (e.g., captains) were more likely to be identified as team
leaders. As Mosher (1979) suggested, formal leaders (i.e., captains) are
members of both the team and the coaching staff. Given that the formal
leader may be viewed as a member of both parties, he/she serves as a
liaison between the coaching staff and players. Therefore, from an
athlete's perspective it appears that it is important to have
athlete leaders who have the consensus of their teammates to represent
them. Thus, it could be hypothesized that it is important for players to
elect their captains as opposed to having the coaching staff
unilaterally appoint the captain. This could have important
ramifications in fostering effective communication between the athletes
and the coaching staff.
On the other hand, informal leaders or those without a formal
designation were more likely to be viewed as peer leaders by their
teammates. Research in business and industry has shown that informal
leaders play an important role in groups containing formal leaders
(Wheelan & Johnston, 1996). That is, informal leaders significantly
influenced the group's activities, created aspects of the
group's culture, and influenced the group's processes and
structure. As Pescosolido (2002) noted, informal leaders assist group
members in making sense of ambiguous scenarios. For instance, an athlete
may receive instructions from the coach and may not fully understand
them, thus he/she will be inclined to ask one of their peer leaders for
clarification. Taken together, it seems reasonable to assume that both
types of leadership--formal and informal--play an important role in
effective group functioning.
Insofar as tenure is concerned, it was hypothesized the longer
athletes were members of a team, the more likely they would be
identified as a leader. The results offered partial support for this
hypothesis. It was consistently found that the majority of team and peer
leaders identified were third-year players, followed almost equally by
either second or fourth year players regardless of leadership function.
The dynamic nature of leadership may help explain the finding that
athletes in a leadership role were seldom in their first-year with the
team. For example, first-year athletes are new to their teams, need to
learn the teams' dynamics (e.g., norms, goals), as well as earn or
prove their leadership capabilities to the rest of the group. By the
time an athlete has reached second-year, they begin to emerge as an
athlete leader. Then, by the third year, team and peer leaders have
solidified their status within the team; hence the propensity for
athlete leaders to be in their third season with the team. Finally, if
an athlete remains with the team into their fourth and fifth seasons,
they appear to maintain a leadership role (despite their reduced numbers
on team rosters). It should be noted that this is a somewhat speculative
explanation as the present study was not a multi-year longitudinal design. The adoption of such a design is a possible avenue for future
research to consider. It should also be noted that the number of fifth
year players in the present study was low. It is possible that the
majority of players at this level, while allowed to compete for five
years, take the opportunity to play for four years.
Support was found for the hypothesis that starters would be more
likely to fulfill leadership roles within their respective teams.
Furthermore, being a starter was more pronounced for those nominated as
a team leader. Specifically, being a starter and a team leader was most
prominent for task leaders, followed by external leaders, and social
leaders. Given that one of the objectives of varsity sports is
successful performance--a task related outcome--it is not surprising
that the two task-related functions (task and external) of leadership
had the highest proportion of starters (who are usually the individuals
most likely to perform successfully). Based on these current and past
results (e.g., Rees & Segal, 1984), it appears that athletic ability
(e.g., being a starter) is important given that these task leaders must
lead their team onto the playing surface and have the respect of fellow
team members. This would be particularly difficult if the task leader
had reduced abilities. Although the majority of social leaders were also
starters, it was less definitive clear cut than task and external
leadership. This result is somewhat similar to Rees and Segal (1984) who
found half of the social leaders nominated were starters. The small
reduction in being a starter and a social leader may be related to the
nature of goal-directed groups (e.g., sport teams). While it appears
that it is essential to have task leaders that are starters, having
starters as social leaders may not be as crucial since team member
relational activities can occur outside of the playing surface.
The study's second purpose was related to the quantity of team
and peer leaders. Due to the more stringent agreement levels required
(i.e., at least 50% of teammates) as well as the predominantly formalized leader composition of team leaders, the fewest number of
athlete leaders and lowest dispersion values occurred at this level.
With regard to the three leadership functions, it is interesting to note
that there were fewer athletes who occupied the external leadership role
compared to task and social leaders. This could be related to the fact
that the task and social leaders are internal to the team, whereas the
external leaders' functions are for the most part related to
external functions such as promoting the team within the community.
Therefore, less athlete leaders are required to fulfill this external
function. Nonetheless, the finding of the presence of external leaders
empirically confirms the notion that athletes perform functions outside
of the team similar to what Mosher (1979) proposed for the sport of
volleyball.
As for the number of athlete leaders, the results in the present
study distinguishing between team and peer leaders expanded the
unidimensional approach (examining athlete leadership in general)
employed by Loughead and Hardy (2005) who found that approximately 27%
of athletes were nominated as athlete leaders by their teammates.
Together the two studies' results suggest that while theoretically
every team member can be a leader (Northouse, 2001), it is clear that
not everyone assumes or carries out a leadership role on their team.
Future research should examine whether there is an ideal number of
athlete leaders for a team and if the quantity of team and/or peer
leaders influences individual (e.g., satisfaction) and team level
outcomes (e.g., team effectiveness, cohesion).
As previously noted, the third purpose of the present study was to
determine the stability of athlete leadership over the course of a
season. In general, the results indicated that those athletes who were
nominated as either team or peer leaders at the beginning of the season
(i.e., Time 1) were highly likely to remain a leader near the end of the
season (i.e., Time 2) regardless of leadership function (task, social,
external). Although this would suggest that leadership is static in
nature, closer examination of the results indicated that while there is
a core group of athletes who fulfill a leadership role throughout the
season, there is some turnover. That is to say that some team and peer
leaders were only nominated at the beginning of the season and a smaller
number only nominated near the end of the season. Given the dynamic
nature of groups (Forsyth, 1998), it follows that a team's
leadership also changes to some extent. This could be related to the
fact that depending on the team's need at a particular moment in
time, athlete leadership may change or remain stable. For instance,
teams that have an excellent chance of capturing a championship may
decide to play their veterans more than their rookies. Consequently,
there would be very little change in athlete leadership. On the other
hand, a team that will not be participating in post-season competition
may decide to play more of the freshman or junior players; thus
increasing the likelihood of these players emerging as either team or
peer leaders.
One limitation to the present study was that the findings were
based on a sample of varsity collegiate team sports. Consequently, it is
recommended that future research examine athlete leadership at other
levels of competition. In particular, the investigation of athlete
leadership in more developed or established teams (e.g., professional
sport) is needed. It would be particularly interesting to examine
personal characteristics, such as team tenure or experience, in relation
to team leaders at the professional level as it is quite possible for a
newly acquired player with several seasons of experience playing at the
same level to immediately step into a leadership position. Nonetheless,
the results of this study offer preliminary support for the presence of
team and peer leaders who fulfill task, social, and external leadership
functions. It is hoped that the results of the present study will
stimulate systematic research in this emerging area.
References
Carton, A. V., Hausenblas, H. A., & Eys, M.A. (2005). Group
dynamics in sport (3rd ed.). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information
Technology.
Chelladurai, P. (1994). Leadership. In R. N. Singer, M. Murphey,
& L. K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology
(pp. 647-671). New York: MacMillan.
Chelladurai, P., & Riemer, H. A. (1998). Measurement of
leadership in sport. In J. L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in sport and exercise
psychology measurement (pp. 227-253). Morgantown, WV: Fitness
Information Technologies.
Conway III, L. G, & Schaller, M. (1998). Methods for the
measurement of consensual beliefs within groups. Group Dynamics: Theory,
Research, and Practice, 2, 241-252.
Fleishman, E. A., Mumford, M. D., Zaccaro, S. J., Levin, K Y.,
Korotkin, A. L., & Hein, M. B. (1991). Taxonomic efforts in the
description of leader behavior: A synthesis and functional
interpretation. Leadership Quarterly, 2, 245-287.
Forsyth, D. R. (1998). Methodological advances in the study of
group dynamics. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 2,
211-212.
Glenn, S. D., & Horn, T. S. (1993). Psychological and personal
predictors of leadership behavior in female soccer athletes. Journal of
Applied Sport Psychology, 5, 17-34.
Gould, D., Hodge, K, Peterson, K., & Petlichkoff, L. (1987).
Psychological foundations of coaching: Similarities and differences
among intercollegiate wrestling coaches. The Sport Psychologist,
1,293-308.
Kogler Hill, S. E. (2001). Team leadership. In P. G. Northouse
(Ed.), Leadership: Theory and practice (2hd ed.) (pp. 161-187). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Lee, M. J., Coburn, T., & Partridge, R. (1983). The influence
of team structure in determining leadership function in association
football. Journal of Sport Behavior, 6, 59-66.
Loughead, T. M., & Hardy, J. (2005). An examination of coach
and peer leader behaviors in sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 6,
303-312.
Mabry, E. A., & Barnes, R. E. (1980). The dynamics of small
group communication. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Mosher, M. (1979). The team captain. Volleyball Technical Journal,
4, 7-8.
Neubert, M. J. (1999). Too much of a good thing or the more the
merrier? Exploring the dispersion and gender composition of informal
leadership in manufacturing teams. Small Group Research, 30, 635-646.
Northouse, P. G. (2001). Leadership: Theory and practice (2hd ed.).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Pescosolido, A. T. (2002). Emergent leaders as managers of group
emotion. Leadership Quarterly, 13, 583-599.
Rees, C. R. (1983). Instrumental and expressive leadership in team
sports: A test of Leadership Role Differentiation Theory. Journal of
Sport Behavior, 6, 17-27.
Rees, C. R., & Segal, M.W. (1984). Role differentiation in
groups: The relationship between instrumental and expressive leadership.
Small Group Behavior, 15, 109-123.
Riemer, H. A., & Chelladurai, P. (1995). Leadership and
satisfaction in athletics. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology,
17, 276-293.
Shaw, M. E. (1981). Group dynamics: The psychology of small group
behavior (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Tropp, K. J., & Landers, D. M. (1979). Team interaction and the
emergence of leadership and interpersonal attraction in field hockey.
Journal of Sport Psychology, 1,228-240.
Wheelan, S. A., & Johnston, F. (1996). The role of informal
member leaders in a system containing formal leaders. Small Group
Research, 27, 33-55.
Yukelson, D., Weinberg, R., Richardson, P., & Jackson, A.
(1983). Interpersonal attraction and leadership within collegiate sport
teams. Journal of Sport Behavior, 6, 28-36.
Todd M. Loughead
University of Windsor, Canada
James Hardy
University of Wales Bangor, U.K.
Mark A. Eys
Laurentian University, Canada
Address Correspondence To: Todd Loughead, Department of
Kinesiology, University of Windsor, 401 Sunset Avenue, Windsor, Ontario,
Canada, N9B 3P4 Email: loughead@uwindsor.ca Fax: (519) 973-7056
Table 1
List of Behavioral Characteristics of Task Social, and External
Leaders Provided to the Participants
Task Leaders Social Leaders External Leaders
* Helps focus the * Contributes to * Promotes the team well
team on its goals harmony within the within the community
team (e.g., city or campus
communities)
* Helps to clarify * Ensures teammates * Represents the team's
responsibilities for are unsolved and interests in meetings
teammates included in team with coaching staff or
events league organizers
* Assists in decision * Helps solve * Attempts to secure
making interpersonal necessary or desired
conflicts that may resources, support, and
arise within the recognition for the team
team (e.g, organizes &
participates in
fundraising)
* Offers instruction * Offers support * Buffers team members
to teammates when and is busted by from outside
required teammates distractions (e. g.,
media, financial/budget
issues)
Table 2.
Dispersion Coefficients for Team and Peer Leadership
Time 1 Time 2
Team Peer Team Peer
Leader Leader Leader Leader
Task Leadership .15 .35 .15 .36
Social Leadership .11 .47 .12 .45
External Leadership .08 .31 .10 .29
Table 3
Personal Characteristics of Team and Peer Leaders
Task Leadership Social Leadership
Time 1 Time 2 Time 1 Time 2
TLs PLs TLs PLs TLs PLs TLs PLs
Formalized role
Formal 65 34 72 34 57 26 56 27
Informal 35 66 28 66 43 74 44 73
Starting status
Starter 95 81 94 7 93 66 85 79
Non-starter 5 19 6 27 7 34 15 21
Tenure on team
First season 7 7 9 14 3 13 4 14
Second season 11 24 9 23 17 29 8 24
Third season 49 40 41 34 50 36 44 40
Fourth season 26 26 28 22 20 19 36 16
Fifth season 7 3 13 3 10 3 8 6
External Leadership
Time 1 Time 2
TLs PLs TLs PLs
Formalized role
Formal 79 37 81 42
Informal 21 63 19 58
Starting status
Starter 88 75 94 78
Non-starter 34 25 6 22
Tenure on team
First season 0 9 0 5
Second season 4 24 5 22
Third season 46 37 43 42
Fourth season 42 25 33 23
Fifth season 8 5 19 8
Note. The numbers included in the table are percentages.
TLs = team leaders and PLs = peer leaders.