首页    期刊浏览 2024年11月30日 星期六
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Examining current-ideal discrepancy scores and exercise motivations as predictors of social physique anxiety in exercising females.
  • 作者:Sabiston, Catherine M. ; Crocker, Peter R.E. ; Munroe-Chandler, Krista J.
  • 期刊名称:Journal of Sport Behavior
  • 印刷版ISSN:0162-7341
  • 出版年度:2005
  • 期号:March
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:University of South Alabama
  • 摘要:Researchers have emphasized females' reports of negative psychosocial affect related to body image, since they place higher meaning and importance on physical characteristics and perceptions of appearance than do males (Harter, 1999; Pliner, Chaiken, & Flett, 1990). It has been observed that the pressures to achieve a thin body shape amongst females may arise from comparisons of their own figures with the perceived female figures thought to be 'ideal' to same and opposite sex peers (Bordo, 1993; Cohn & Adler, 1992; Heinberg & Thompson, 1995). These comparisons of current and ideal body shapes may lead to heightened body-related anxiety. According to Harter (1999), real-ideal discrepancies are also motivations for behavior change, such as behaviors focused on altering body shape and size.
  • 关键词:Anxiety;Motivation (Psychology);Women athletes

Examining current-ideal discrepancy scores and exercise motivations as predictors of social physique anxiety in exercising females.


Sabiston, Catherine M. ; Crocker, Peter R.E. ; Munroe-Chandler, Krista J. 等


Body image is generally defined as a multidimensional construct purporting to identify an individual's perceptions and attitudes towards her/his body and physical characteristics (Bane & McAuley, 1998; Banfield & McCabe, 2002). The multidimensional measurement of body image includes cognitive, affective, and behavioral modalities (Bane & McAuley, 1998). A weakness in the literature is that body image research has typically been detached from foundational conceptual and theoretical frameworks (Bane & McAuley, 1998; Kulbok & Cox, 2002). However, associations between sociocultural frameworks, self-esteem theories, self-presentation and social anxiety perspectives, and behavioral motivations are inherent in recent portrayals of body image as a multidimensional construct (Bane & McAuley, 1998; Banfield & McCabe, 2002). Therefore, an individual's body image and possible implications of psychosocial functioning and related social physique anxiety should be examined using sound theoretical frameworks.

Researchers have emphasized females' reports of negative psychosocial affect related to body image, since they place higher meaning and importance on physical characteristics and perceptions of appearance than do males (Harter, 1999; Pliner, Chaiken, & Flett, 1990). It has been observed that the pressures to achieve a thin body shape amongst females may arise from comparisons of their own figures with the perceived female figures thought to be 'ideal' to same and opposite sex peers (Bordo, 1993; Cohn & Adler, 1992; Heinberg & Thompson, 1995). These comparisons of current and ideal body shapes may lead to heightened body-related anxiety. According to Harter (1999), real-ideal discrepancies are also motivations for behavior change, such as behaviors focused on altering body shape and size.

One method of assessing females' perceptions of the ideal body, in addition to perceptions of what others find attractive, is the Figure Rating Scale (FRS; Stunkard, Sorenson, and Schulsinger, 1983). The FRS consists of silhouette drawings of adult body figures ranging from extremely thin to very large. Individuals are asked to indicate the body figure that represents their current body shape, the body shape most desired (which is considered the ideal body figure), the body shape they perceive to be most attractive to the opposite gender, and the body shape that is perceived to be ideal to individuals of the same gender (Cohn & Adler, 1992). The FRS is purported to measure the cognitive dimension of body image (i.e., Bane & McAuley, 1998), and is frequently used as a measure of personal body dissatisfaction. This is obtained by assessing the discrepancy between the individual's current body shape and their perceived ideal body shape (i.e., Lavine, Sweeney, & Wagener, 1999; Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe, & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999).

There is a multitude of research employing an individual's current and ideal discrepancy scores. However, very few studies have examined the discrepancy scores between an individual's perceived current body shape and the body shapes they perceive to be most attractive by same gender or opposite gender peers as a measure of cognitive body image (Colin & Adler, 1992). These discrepancy scores take into account the apparent perceptions of other individuals, which are considered antecedents to body image disturbances, changes in overall self-esteem, and subsequent health behaviors (i.e., Leary, 1992; Harter, 1999; Heinberg & Thompson, 1995).

Using the FRS as a measure of perceived ideal female body shapes allows the researcher to identify discrepancies that may lead to unhealthy behaviors, such as eating disturbances, and excessive or avoidance of physical activity (Thompson et al., 1999). Although the FRS is a global-like measure, whereby individuals can only identify entire shapes rather than specific aspects of the body, it does provide an indication of whether there are differences between the body shape perceived to be current, ideal, and the shapes perceived to be desired by others. Females who report a current body shape that falls short of the perceived ideal body shape desired by themselves, males, or by other females may express body-related anxiety in social because they do not measure up to the perceived ideal body and physical attractiveness standards. Partial links between perceptions of body shape and social physique anxiety have been reported (Crawford & Eklund, 1994; Eklund & Crawford 1994).

Social physique anxiety is rooted in self-presentation and social anxiety frameworks (Hart, Leary, & Rejeski, 1989; Leafy, 1992; 1999). Self-presentation is an individual's attempt to control and monitor the impressions others form of them (Leafy, 1999). Due to the importance of physical appearance in Western cultures, positive impressions related to attractiveness and body shape/size are coveted by females, and self-presentation strategies are prolific (Leafy, 1999; McAuley & Burman, 1993). Consequently, all individual's degree of body-related anxiety can be influenced by the perceived or known impressions of others and the individual's subsequent attempt to control these impressions. Social physique anxiety arises when individuals are motivated to make desired (physical) impressions on others but doubt their capabilities to do so (Hart et al., 1989). Since social physique anxiety is a reported affective component of body image (Bane & McAuley, 1998), it is important to understand the relationships among discrepancies in perceptions of body shapes and affective responses that could have negative impacts on females" psychological and physiological health.

Apart from possible relationships between perceptions of attractive female body shapes and body-related anxiety, individuals who report a greater discrepancy between their current and perceived ideal body shapes may also identify more self-presentational concerns as motivation for physical activity. As such, these individuals may be more likely to report participating in physical activity in order to change aspects related to their appearance and shape.

Research examining the relationship between social physique anxiety and physical activity behavior has yielded ambiguous results. Predictions alluding to social physique anxiety as a precursor to behavioral responses, such as physical activity avoidance or withdrawal, have only been partially supported (Hart et al., 1989; Leary, 1999; Sabiston, Sedgwick, Farrell, et al., 2003). In recent reports, social physique anxiety had low or insignificant relations with physical activity (Crawford & Eklund, 1994; Crocker, Sabiston, Forrester et al., 2003; Kantz, Hardy, & Ainsworth, 1997), competence in physical activity (Crocker et al., 2003; Crocker, Snyder, Kowalski, & Hoar, 2000; Kowalski, Crocker, & Kowalski, 2001), physiological measures (Treasure, kox, & Lawton, 1998), and preference for exercise settings (Eklund & Crawford, 1994) in adolescent and adult females. However, Frederick and Morrison (1996) found that social physique anxiety was related to activity type preference, whereby individuals who reported higher body-related anxiety were more likely to participate in fitness-type activities rather than individual and team sports. Given these research findings, the strength and implication of the associations between social physique anxiety and exercise behavior are not entirely understood.

Despite the inconclusive results noted above, an expected yet equivocal relationship between body-related anxiety and physical activity is evident. Females often report exercise motivations that originate from self-presentational concerns, which include weight concerns, appearance, and body shape and tone (Bane & McAuley, 1998; Crawford & Eklund, 1994; Frederick & Morrison, 1996; Leary, 1999; Marten & Shaffer, 2002). According to Ingledew and Sullivan (2002), body image and body mass index (BMI) were significant predictors of appearance-related exercise motives among older adolescent females. Specifically, individuals who had higher BMI's and/or expressed greater body-related anxiety (measured as lower body image scores) were more likely to exercise in order to become thinner or alter their attractiveness. These findings support research linking exercise strategies to weight and appearance (Silberstein et al., 1988).

To investigate the reasons that individuals report as motives to engage in physical activity, Silberstein et al. (1988) developed and tested the Reasons for Exercise Inventory (REI). The researchers identified weight and appearance as primary motives for females' engagement in physical activity. Since the development of the REI, some researchers have used the instrument and challenged the psychometrics of the scale (Cash, Novy, & Grant, 1994; Smith, Handley, & Eldredge, 1998). Using factor analysis techniques, Cash et al. (1994) determined that the original 7-subscale instrument actually comprised only 4 factors that were guided by theory and methodology. These four factors were subsequently labeled "'weight/ appearance management", "fitness/health management", "stress/emotion management", and "socializing". Smith et al. (1998) reported that the inventory's factor structure might not be generalizable to males and females. Despite the ambiguous scale dimensions and psychometrics, self-presentational exercise strategies consistently emerge among female adolescents and adults (Cash et al., 1994; Crawford & Eklund, 1994; Silberstein et al.. 1988).

Several theorists have stated that the sociocultural pressures for females to achieve and/or maintain an 'ideal' figure are likely the basis for the emphasis on self-presentational motives (i.e., Frederickson & Roberts, 1997; Leary, 1999; Monteath & McCabe, 1997). Therefore, it is important to determine whether perceptions of ideal female figures are related to weight and appearance exercise motives. Females are motivated to achieve perceived ideal body shapes by exercising to lose weight and gain muscle tone, thereby improving their appearance (Bordo, 1993; Page & Fox, 1997; Silberstein et al., 1988). However, the relationships between exercise motivations I especially weight and appearance), self-presentational anxiety (such as social physique anxiety), and perceptions of attractive female body shapes (the discrepancy between an individual's current body shape and the female shapes thought to be considered ideal by oneself and others) have not been examined.

The purpose of this study was to examine the predictors of social physique anxiety (SPA) in exercising females, while controlling for physical characteristics of weight and height (BMI). The sample was limited to exercising females due to the consistent reports that SPA is not a critical variable in determining physical activity level (frequency or duration) but appears to be related to the motives for exercise as well as perceptions of body shape (Crawford & Eklund, 1994; Kowalski et al., 2001; Lantz et al., 1997). It was hypothesized that discrepancies between an individual's current perceived body shape and their ideal, and the female figure thought to be most attractive to both male and female peers would significantly predict SPA. Furthermore, exercise motivations pertaining to weight and appearance, would also significantly predict SPA. Based on previous research, BMI may be significantly related to SPA and was controlled in the analyses (Crawford & Eklund, 1994; Crocker et al.. 2003; Eklund & Crawford, 1994; Kowalski et al., 2001).

Methods

Participants

Three hundred and seventy three undergraduate females were recruited from a range of classes at the University of Windsor in Ontario. Canada, and volunteered to participate in the study. University-aged females were targeted since they have been known to place strong emphasis on the importance of physical attractiveness, body image, and social acceptance (Altabe & Thompson, 1993; Bordo, 1993; Thompson et al., 1999). From the sample, only those individuals who reported exercising at least once a week were included in the analyses (n=296). Therefore, 79% of the recruited sample was included in this research. The participants reported a mean age of 20.53 (SD=2.91) years, mean height of 1.66 (SD=.07) meters, and mean weight 61.84 (SD=10.39) kilograms. Mean body mass index (BMI) was 22.56 (SD=3.23) kg/[m.sup.2]. This BMI is consistent with previous research using undergraduate participants (i.e., Crawford & Eklund, 1994; Kowalski et al., 2001). The participants indicated that main exercise activities included: biking, individual sports, running, swimming, team sports, and weight training.

Instruments

Figure rating scale (FRS). The FRS is a reliable and valid instrument of cognitive body image for adult and adolescent females (Bane & McAuley, 1998; Candy & Fee, 1998; Thompson et al., 1999). The instrument measures perceived body shapes using a 9-point continuum of female body figure diagrams ranging from extremely thin to extremely overweight. Individuals are asked to identify their current body size (CFR), an ideal or desired body shape (IFR), an indication of the body shape thought to be most desirable to males (MFR), and the perceived body shape desired by female peers (FFR) on the FRS. Refer to Stunkard et al. (1983) or Cohn and Adler (1992) to see the female silhouette images.

Three cognitive body image scores were derived for each participant by subtracting the ideal figure rating, the body shape perceived to be most attractive to males, and the perceived body shape ideal for female peers from the current figure rating (Cohn & Adler, 1992; Lavine et al., 1999). For descriptive purposes, the raw score was employed; however, the absolute value of the discrepancy between the CFR and each of the three reported figure ratings was employed in further analyses. The direction of discrepancy was not considered as important as the magnitude of the discrepancy (Crawford & Eklund, 1994).

Social physique anxiety scale (SPAS). The SPAS (Martin, Rejeski, Leary, McAuley, & Bane, 1997) is a 9-item self-report inventory that measures social physique anxiety as an affective modality of body image. Participants were asked to indicate the degree to which statements are characteristic of them on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from (1) not at all to (5) extremely. Individuals who scored higher (e.g., maximum score is 45) on the SPAS indicated a fear of being judged and observed by others, thus expressing the importance of others' opinions. The SPAS has demonstrated construct validity, test-retest reliability, and internal consistency (Martin et al., 1997; Motl & Conroy, 2000).

Reasons for exercise inventory (REI). The REI is a 24-item, 7-subscale self-report inventory purporting to evaluate motives for engaging in exercise activities (Silberstein et al., 1988). The seven REI subscales include exercising for weight control, fitness, health, body tone, overall physical attractiveness, mood enhancement, and enjoyment. Participants rated the importance of each exercise motive from (1) not at all important to (7) extremely important. Mean scores were determined for each motive, and the higher scores identified the main reason(s) individuals engaged in exercise. According to Tabachnick and Fidell (2001), the small number of items within each subscale could contribute to instability for this inventory. However, acceptable reliabilities on each of the scales have been reported (Silberstein et al., 1988).

Procedures

Following approval from the University Behavioral Research Ethics Board, female participants were recruited during class time from a variety of undergraduate courses. The principal investigator briefed the students on the study objectives and procedures, distributed consent forms, and the interested female students were asked to remain behind after class. It was emphasized that participation in the study was voluntary and would not influence the individual's grade in the course.

The questionnaires were administered in person by the researcher, and full instructions were provided, in writing, on the front page of the questionnaire booklet. Any further questions were addressed prior to the commencement of the assessment. The students were then asked to complete all four sections of the questionnaire booklet (i.e., demographic data, FRS, SPAS, and REI instruments). The questionnaires took approximately 25 minutes to complete and return to the researcher.

Results

Scale structures and reliabilities The FRS ([alpha]=.85) and the SPAS ([alpha] = .89) exhibited acceptable internal consistency. Due to the concern over the factor structure for the REI, a Maximum Likelihood exploratory factor analysis with oblique rotation was conducted on the scale using SPSSx (version 11). The number of factors extracted was based on the requirement that a factor's eigenvalue be greater than 1, and examination of the Scree plot. Item loadings were deemed acceptable if equal to or greater than .50 on a single rotated factor (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). The resultant factor structure of the REI comprised four factors that were labeled Fitness/Health (factor 1), Weight/ Appearance (factor 2), Stress/Emotion (factor 3). and Socialization (factor 4). This structure and the subsequent labeling are consistent with previous research (Cash et al., 1994). Three items were deleted due to nonunique and/or insufficient loadings on any single factor (Item 10: To have fun: Item 12: To maintain my current weight; and Item 17: To increase my energy level). Refer to Table 1 for the factor structure of the REI. The revised REI subscales had acceptable internal consistency (Fitness/health: [alpha] =.88; Weight/Appearance: [alpha] = .86: Stress/ Emotion: [alpha] = .77; and Socializing:[alpha] =.86).

Descriptive statistics for the FRS, SPAS, and REI are presented in Table 2. These values are consistent with previous research (Cash et al., 1994; Crawford & Eklund, 1994: Eklund & Crawford, 1994: Kowalski et al., 2001: Lavine et al., 1999). The relationships among the variables were examined using Pearson correlation coefficients and are presented in Table 3. The social physique anxiety measure had low to moderate correlations with BMI (r=.28), absolute discrepancy scores on the FRS (CFR-IFR: 1=.47; CFR-MFR: r=.31; CFR-FFR: r=.24), and the Weight/Appearance subscale of the REI (r=.59). The Fitness/Health subscale of the REI was not significantly correlated with SPAS, which is similar to previous research (Crawford & Eklund, 1994). As expected, the Stress/Emotion and Socialization subscales of the REI demonstrated no significant relationships with any of the other variables.

Main analyses

Following an investigation of the assumptions of normality and outliers, a two-step forced entry multiple regression analysis was conducted to test the hypothesis that social physique anxiety would be related to self-presentational variables. The dependent variable was the SPAS measure, and the predictors that were entered into the model were BMI, followed by the absolute discrepancy scores on the FRS, and the subscales of the REI. The order of variable entry was based on predictions. BMI, entered to control for physical characteristics, was significant and accounted for 7.8% of the variance in SPAS. The final model accounted for close to 45% of the variance in SPAS. The CFR-IFR ([beta] = .23) and CFR-MFR ([beta] =.14) discrepancy scores, and the Weight/Appearance ([beta]=.52) and Fitness/Health ([beta]=-.10) subscales of the REI were significant individual predictors in the final model. However. since significant correlations between several variables were noted, we were cautious of moderately to highly interrcorrelated variables in the model. In an attempt to obtain a more parsimonious model, a second multiple regression analysis was conducted. The discrepancy score between the participant's current and ideal body shape and weight/appearance reasons for exercise were significant individual predictors of SPA and accounted for 43% of the variance (see Table 4). The second more robust model accounted for similar variance in SPA.

Discussion

This research study examined the predictors of social physique anxiety within a multidimensional model of body image and self-presentation theoretical frameworks. This study allowed for an extension of the social physique anxiety research with connections to both subjective and objective perceptions of the ideal female body, and motives for exercise and physical activity.

The novelty of this research is the consideration of females' subjective and objective perceptions of the ideal female figure. It is well documented that perceptions of others are important determinants of behavior and emotions related to the self (Harter, 1999; Leary & Kowalski, 1995: Leary, 1992). According to some researchers, sociocultural pressures towards thinness that females experience are attributed to male preferences for thin, physically attractive figures and the prolific mediated female body (Bordo, 1993; Frederick & Roberts, 1997; Markula, 1995; Myers & Biocca, 1992). This study examined the perceptions of the ideal female figure, as perceived by oneself, and the perceived ideal for same and opposite sex peers. The discrepancy between these perceived body shapes and reported current shapes was also examined to determine the relationship with social physique anxiety. As predicted, the discrepancy score between females' current body shapes and that perceived to be most attractive to males was a significant predictor of SPA. However, the relationship appeared to be significant only in the presence of other predictors. Therefore, these relationships require further investigation.

The current-ideal female body discrepancy score, which was a significant predictor of SPA in the final model, has been used widely as a measure of body size/shape satisfaction or as a cognitive body image construct (i.e., Bane & McAuley, 1998; Crawford & Eklund, 1994; Lavine et al., 1999). The magnitude and direction of the discrepancy between the female's current body shape and the shape she reportedly desired is similar to other empirical research in the area (i.e., Cohn & Adler, 1992; Lavine et al., 1999; Silberstein et al., 1988). Consistently, females report an ideal body shape that is smaller than their current body shape, and this discrepancy is discussed as normative discontent among female adolescents and adults (Markey, Tinsley, Ericksen, Ozer, & Markey, 2002; Thompson et al., 1999). In the current research study, the absolute discrepancy between the current and ideal body shapes is smaller than the discrepancy between the current and the perceived ideal female body most attractive to males, which is again smaller than the difference between the current and the perceived ideal female body desired by other females. Therefore, it is not the magnitude of the discrepancies between the current and perceived ideal body shapes but the context of the discrepancy.

In deciphering between perceived ideal female figures, one must question the quantification of a subjective ideal figure. The individual's perceived ideal is likely the product of situational and external influences in addition to one's own perceptions of what is achievable and desirable (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997; Sparkes, 1997). Therefore, the measure of an individual's subjective ideal figure may also be representative of the objective perceptions of others. Nevertheless, there were differences between the measures of ideal female figures, which indicate some level of distinctness. More research is necessary to determine the factors that contribute to a female's perceived ideal figure.

Physical activity is a primary strategy for altering one's body shape and size, which usually emanates from current-ideal discrepancies and motivated by strategies related to self-presentation (Harter, 1999; Silberstein et al., 1988). The significant correlations between the discrepancy scores and the weight/appearance motives for exercise demonstrate this contention. The fitness/health motives on the REI demonstrated low correlations with the CFR-IFR discrepancy score, whereby an inverse relationship was noted. The closer the match between the individual's current and ideal body shapes indicated a higher fitness/health motivation for exercise.

The fitness/health variable was a significant predictor of SPA in the initial modeling process, however only in the presence of the other predictors. The low-moderate significant correlations between the fitness/health motive and all other reasons for exercise may be due to underlying fitness and health motives for exercise that likely exist concomitantly with all other motives. For instance, individuals may report exercising for weight/appearance reasons while accepting that they will become more fit and healthy as a secondary benefit. Also, females report fitness and health motives for exercise most frequently, perhaps due to the known and highly publicized links between exercise and fitness/health or the reporting of these reasons for exercise due to the social acceptability of the response. Further research is necessary to understand these intricate relationships.

The weight/appearance motives for exercise was significantly and highly associated with SPA. Previous research has identified that physical appearance-related variables demonstrate strong correlations with SPA (Crawford & Eklund, 1994; Crocker et al., 2003; Crocker et al., 2000; Frederick & Morrison, 1996; Kowalski et al., 2001). In this study, female undergraduate students who reported exercising primarily for weight and/or appearance expressed greater body-related anxiety. This relationship needs to be examined in more depth, especially considering the various meanings that weight and appearance hold as motivations for exercise. For instance, some females desire a muscular and toned body shape that motivates them to exercise, which may trigger body-related anxiety as a result of perceptions of insufficient muscle mass. This condition has recently been introduced in the literature as muscle dysmolphia (i.e., Goodale, Watkins, & Cardinal, 2001). According to Goodale et al. (2001), muscle dysmorphia is connected to various physical and psychosocial emotional and behavioral consequences, such as anxiety and distress. Aside from desires for muscularity, other females describe their weight and appearance in terms of thinness. Desires for thinness may also lead to body-related anxiety as a result of perceptions of excess adiposity (i.e., body dysmorphia, see Thompson et al., 1999). Therefore, the potential differences between descriptions of weight/ appearance as muscular versus thin should be examined in order to better understand the relationship between these exercise motivations and social physique anxiety.

The relationship between weight/appearance exercise strategies and social physique anxiety also needs to be analyzed further to determine the longitudinal effects of exercising primarily for weight/appearance. Future studies may apply motivation theories and frameworks to the relationship between exercise motives and SPA to determine whether individuals' physical activity strategies fluctuate between external forms of motivation such as self-determined identified and introjected regulation (i.e., Deci & Ryan, 1995). These forms of motivation that could be influenced differentially by experiences of SPA have demonstrated various health and fitness consequences in the sport and exercise literature (i.e., Duda, Chi, Newton, Walling, & Catley, 1995; Vallerand & Fortier, 1998; Vlachopoulos, Karageorghis, & Terry, 2000).

The strengths of this study include the use of valid instruments and multidimensional body image theoretical frameworks, and a moderate sample of exercising female undergraduate students. A limitation is that the Reasons for Exercise Inventory (REI; Silberstein et al., 1988) did not emanate from theoretical foundations. Nevertheless, suggestions for the application of self-presentation and self-determination theories were noted, and future research should examine these possibilities and the subsequent properties of the scale. Also, the FRS schematics of body shapes do not account for muscle mass, which could be a limitation to this research. Given the increased desires for females to attain a muscular/toned body shape along with thinness (Bordo, 1993; Thompson et al., 1999), the FRS-type scales should include measures of muscularity. Raudenbush and Meyer (2003) have recently conducted a study with male intercollegiate athletes using a scale of increased muscularity, and found that desires for muscle mass varied based on the individual's sport preference and use of dietary supplements. This type of research with females is also warranted in order to advance our understanding of exercise motivation and possible antecedents of body-related anxiety. Finally, there was some concern regarding the use of self-report measures that tap perceptions rather than actual physical states. However, researchers have consistently maintained that the perceptions of body size and shape are more strongly related to weight and body image concerns than actual body size and have a greater influence on exercise and eating behaviors (Bane & McAuley, 1998; Davis, 1997; Thompson et al., 1999).

Aside from these limitations, this study highlighted interesting relationships between perceptions of ideal body shapes, physical activity motives, and social physique anxiety. It is important to continue to investigate relationships associated with body-related anxiety to gain an understanding of the antecedents and consequences of this form of anxiety, and the inherent health implications. Specific future recommendations include examining whether physical activity strategies are a coping function related to experiences of SPA, whereby the direction of the relationship between the motives and social physique anxiety should be investigated. Furthermore, the perceptions of others are integral to the study of body image and physical self research and should not be overlooked. The current state of body image literature is saturated with individual (objective) measures that should be combined with subjective concerns such as the perceptions of other male and females' ideal female body shapes. The importance of these perceptions should also be investigated in greater depth. It appears, based on this research, that an individual's ideal shape and the female body shape thought to be most attractive to others are distinct entities.

Based on the findings reported here, perceptions of body shape and reported motivations for physical activity are associated with social physique anxiety. The integral relationships with and predictors of SPA are influenced by self-presentation. However, the specific health implications related to sell-presentation are not well understood, and should become a focus in sport and exercise research. It is also recommended that practitioners in the sport, exercise, and health domains should focus on an individual's perceptions of themselves and others rather than merely on the physical characteristics of weight and appearance. It is possible that changes in perceptions of the ideal female body would also lead to changes in the experience of social physique anxiety and other body image issues, as well as physical fitness behaviors. The need for integrated applied and practical research is essential at this point in body image-related studies and possible interventions pertaining to the physical self.
Table 1

Factor structure of the Reasons for Exercise Inventory

REI Factors and Items Item
 Loading

Fitness/Health

 4. To improve my muscle tone .71
 5. To maintain my physical .72
 well-being
 6. To improve my endurance, 75
 stamina.
11. To improve my strength .71
13. To improve my flexibility, .65
 coordination
16. To increase my resistance .52
 disease

21. To improve my overall health .66
24. To improve my cardiovascular .77
 fitness

Stress/Mood

 3. To improve my mood .67
22. To cope with stress, anxiety .81
23. To cope with sadness, .76
 depression

Weight/Appearance

 1. To be slim .79
 2. To alter a specific area .71
 on my body
 7. To improve my overall .62
 body shape
 8. To lose weight .75
 9. To be sexually desirable .77

15. To be attractive to to .74
 illness and members of
 the opposite sex
18. To redistribute my weight .50
20. To improve my appearance .81

Socializing

14. To socialize with friends .87
19. To meet new people .87

Table 2

Descriptive statistics for the FRS, SPAS, and the REI measures.

Variable Mean SD

CFR 3.21 * 0.99
IFR 2.50 ** 0.66
MFR 2.43 *** 0.69
FFR 2.04 0.66
CFR-IFR 0.73 0.84
CFR-MFR 0.76 1.08
CFR-FFR 1.17 1.12
[absolute value of CFR-IFR] 0.80 0.68
[absolute value of CFR-MFRS] 1.08 0.74
[absolute value of CFR-FFR] 1.33 0.85
SPAS 26.83 7.31
REI: W/A 4.67 1.18
REI: F/H 5.28 1.01
REI: S/E 4.50 1.41
REI: SOC 3.47 1.72

Note: CFR=Current figure rating; IFR=Ideal figure rating;
MFR=Male figure rating; FFR=Female figure rating; [absolute
value of] = absolute discrepancy score: REI:W/A=Weight/
Appearance; REI: F/H=Fitness/Health; REI: S/E=Stress/Emotion;
REI: SOC=Socializing. * CFR significantly different than IFR,
MFR, FFR (p < .05) ** IFR significantly different than FFR
(p < .05) *** MFR significantly different than FFR (p < .05)

Table 3

Pearson correlation coefficients for BMI, the FRS, SPAS, and REI.

 1 2 3 4 5

1. BMI --
2. CFR-IFR .54 * --
3. CFR-MFR .46 * .60 * --
4. CFR-FFR .57 * .61 * .66 * --
5. SPAS .28 * .47 * .31 * .24 * --
6. REI:W/A .23 * .36 * .13 * .14 * .59 *
7. REI:F/H -.13 * -.20 * -.11 -.11 -.07
8. REI:S/E -.03 -.11 -.04 -.03 -.07
9. REI: SOC -.09 -.11 -.08 -.04 -.09

 6 7 8 9

1. BMI
2. CFR-IFR
3. CFR-MFR
4. CFR-FFR
5. SPAS
6. REI:W/A --
7. REI:F/H .15 * --
8. REI:S/E .04 .57 * --
9. REI: SOC .02 .33 * .32 * --

Note: * p <.05

Table 4

Final regression analysis predicting SPA

Step Variable B SE B [beta]

1
 REI: W/A 3.67 .29 .591 *
2
 REI: W/A 2.99 .30 .483 *
 CFR-IFR 3.28 .52 .301 *

Step [R.sup.2] [R.sup.2]

1 .35 * .35 *
2 .08 * .43 *

Note: * p < 0.05


References

Altabe, M., & Thompson, J. K. (1993). Body image changes during early adulthood. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 13, 323-328.

Bane, S., & McAuley, E. (1998). Body image and exercise. In J. L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in sport and exercise psychology measurement (pp. 311-322). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology.

Banfield. S., & McCabe, M. (2002). An evaluation of the construct of body image. Adolescence, 37, 373-393.

Bordo, S. (1993). Unbearable weight: Feminism, western culture, and the body. Berkeley, CA: University of California.

Candy, C., & Fee, V. (1998). Underlying dimensions and psychometric properties of the eating behaviors and body image test for preadolescent girls. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27, 117-127.

Cash, T. F., Novy, P. L., & Grant, J. R. (1994). Why do women exercise? Factor analysis and further validation of the Reasons for Exercise Inventory. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 78, 539-544.

Cohn, L. D., & Adler, N. E. (1992). Female and male perceptions of ideal body shapes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 16, 69-79.

Crawford, S., & Eklund, R. C. (1994). Social physique anxiety, reasons for exercise, and attitudes toward exercise settings. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 16, 70-82.

Crocker P, Sabiston C, Forrester S, Kowalski N, Kowalski K, McDonough M. (2003). Examining change in physical activity, dietary restraint, BMI, social physique anxiety and physical self-perceptions in adolescent girls. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 94, 332-337.

Crocker P, Snyder J, Kowalski K, & Hoar S (2000). Don't let me be fat or physically incompetent! The relationship between physical self-concept and social physique anxiety in Canadian high performance female adolescent athletes. Avante, 6, 1-8.

Davis, C. (1997). Body image, exercise, and eating behaviors. In K. Fox (Ed.), The physical self: From motivation to well-being. (pp. 143-175). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Duda, J., Chi, L., Newton, M., Walling, M., & Catley, D. (1995). Task and ego orientation and intrinsic motivation in sport, International Journal of Sport Psychology, 26, 40-63.

Eklund, R. & Crawford, S. (1994). Active women, social physique anxiety, and exercise. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 16, 431-448.

Fox, K. (1997). The physical self and processes in self-esteem development. In K. Fox (ed.), The physical self: From motivation to well-being (pp. 111-139). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Fredrickson, B., & Roberts, T. (1997). Objectification theory: Toward understanding women's lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173-206.

Goodale, K. R., Watkins, E L., & Cardinal, B. J. (2001). Muscle dysmorphia: Anew form of eating disorder? American Journal of Health Education, 32, 260-266.

Hart, E. A., Leafy, M. R., & Rejeski, W. J. (1989). The measurement of social physique anxiety. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 11, 94-104.

Hatter, S. (1998). The development of self-representations. In Damon, W., and Eisenberg, N. (Eds.) Handbook of Child Psychology (pp. 553-617), New York: Wiley.

Hatter, S. (1999). The Construction of the Self: A Developmental Perspective. New York, NY: Guilford.

Heinberg, L. J., & Thompson, J. K. (1995). Body image and televised images of thinness and attractiveness: A controlled laboratory investigation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 14, 325-338.

Ingledew, D., & Sullivan, G. (2002). Effects of body mass and body image on exercise motives in adolescence. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 3, 323-338.

Kowalski, N., Crocker, P., & Kowalski, K. (2001). Physical self and physical activity relationships in college women: Does social physique anxiety moderate effects? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 72, 55-62.

Kulbok, P., & Cox, C. (2002). Dimensions of adolescent health behavior. Journal of Adolescent Health, 31. 394-400.

Lantz, C., Hardy, C., & Ainsworth, B. (1997). Social physique anxiety and perceived exercise behavior. Journal of Sport Behavior, 20. 83-93.

Lazarus, R. (1999). Epistemology and metatheory, Stress and emotion: A new synthesis (pp 3-23). New York: Springer.

Leary, M. (1999). The social and psychological importance of self-esteem. In R. Kowalski and M. Leary (Eds.), The social psychology of emotional and behavioral problems. Interfaces of social and clinical psychology (197-221). Washington, DC: APA.

Leary, M. & Kowalski, R. (1995). Social Anxiety. New York: Guilford.

Lavine, H., Sweeney, D., & Wagner, S. H. (1999). Depicting women as sex objects in television advertising: Effects on body dissatisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1049-1058.

Markey, C., Tinsley, B., Ericksen, A., Ozer, D., & Markey, P. (2002). Preadolescents' perceptions of females' body size and shape: Evolutionary and social learning perspectives. Journal of Youth and adolescence, 31, 137-146.

Markula, P. (1995). Firm but shapely, fit but sexy, strong but thin: The postmodern aerobicizing female bodies. Sociology of Sport Journal. 12, 424-453.

Martin, K., Rejeski, W. J., Leary, M., McAuley, E., & Bane, S. (1997). Is the Social Physique Anxiety Scale really multidimensional? Conceptual and statistical arguments for a unidimensional model. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 19, 359-367.

Martin, P., & Shaffer, C. (2002). A comparison of female college athletes and nonathletes: eating disorder symptomatology and psychological well-being. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24, 33-41.

McAuley, E., & Burman, G. (1993). The Social Physique Anxiety Scale: Construct validity in adolescent females. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 25, 1049-1053.

Monteath, S., & McCabe, M. (1997). The influence of societal factors on female body image. The Journal of Social Psychology, 137, 708-727.

Motl, R., & Conroy, D. (2000). Validity and factorial invariance of the Social Physique Anxiety Scale. Medicine and Science in Sports attd Exercise, 32, 1007-1017.

Myers, E, & Biocca, F. (1992). The elastic body image: The effects of television advertising and programming on body distortions in young women. Journal of Communication, 42, 108-131.

Page, A., & Fox, K. (1997). Adolescent weight management and the physical self. In K. Fox (ed.), Thephysical self: From motivation to well-being(pp. 229-256). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Pliner, R, Chaiken, S., & Flett, G. (1990). Gender differences in concern with body weight and physical appearance over the life span. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 263-273.

Raudenbush, B., & Meyer, B. (2003). Muscular dissatisfaction and supplement use among male intercollegiate athletes. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 25, 161-170.

Sabiston, C.M., Sedgwick, W., Farrell, R., Crocker, P.R.E., Kowalski, K.C., & Stevens, D. (2003). Body-related anxiety and coping strategies used by Canadian female adolescents. In R. Stelter (Ed.) New Approaches to Exercise and Sport Psychology: Theories, methods and applications. Copenhagen, Denmark: University of Copenhagen.

Silberstein, L., Striegel-Moore, R., Timko, C., & Rodin, J. (1988). Behavioral and psychological implications of body dissatisfaction: Do men and women differ? Sex Roles, 19, 219-233.

Smith, B., Handley, R, & Eldredge, D. (1998). Sex differences in exercise motivation and body image satisfaction among college students. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 723-732.

Stunkard, A., Sorenson, T., & Schulsinger, F. (1983). Use of the Danish adoption register for the study of obesity and thinness. In S. Kety (Ed.), The genetics of neurological and Psychiatric disorders (pp. 115-120). New York, NY: Raven.

Tabachnick, B., & Fidell, L. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Thompson, A., & Chad, K. (2002). The relationship of social physique anxiety to risk for developing an eating disorder in young females. Journal of Adolescent Health, 31, 183-189.

Thompson, K., Heinberg, L., Altabe, M, & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1999). Exacting beauty: Theory; assessment, method, and treatment of body image disturbance. Washington, DC: APA.

Treasure, D., Lox, C & Lawton, B. (1998). Determinants of physical activity in a sedentary, obese female population. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 20, 218-224.

Vallerand, R., & Fortier, M. (1998). Measures of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in sport and physical activity: A review and critique. In J. L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in sport and exercise psychology measurement (pp. 81-101). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology.

Vlachopoulos, S., Karageorghis, C., & Terry, R (2000). Motivation profiles in sport: A self-determination theory perspective. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 387-397.

Address Correspondence To: Catherine Sabiston, School of Human Kinetics, University of British Columbia, Rm. 220B, War Memorial Gym, 6081 University Boulevard, Vancouver, BC.V6T 1Z1

Catherine M. Sabiston and Peter R.E. Crocker

University of British Columbia

Krista J. Munroe-Chandler

University of Windsor
联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有