Gender, age, and professional-level differences in the psychological correlates of fear of injury in Italian gymnasts.
Cartoni, Anna Claudia ; Minganti, Carlo ; Zelli, Arnaldo 等
The role that psychological factors play in influencing an
athlete's performance is a topic that has received substantial
attention in the psychological sport literature (Sonstroem, 1974,
Corbin, Stewart & Blair, 1981 : Feltz & Landers, 1983; Kerr,
1987; Lirgg, 1991 ; Moritz & Feltz, 2000). This literature shows
that anxiety and fear can have a detrimental influence on performance,
whereas self-confidence, self-efficacy and positive thinking can improve
performance.
An individual's psychological outcomes, actions, and
achievements have social foundations and are embedded in the network of
cultural and societal influences he or she may experience (Caprara &
Cervone, 2001). For instance, societies may have different expectations
about the traits, abilities, and prospective achievements of men and
women and, therefore, vary in the degree to which they may enforce
homogenous gender conceptions. Gender differences may thus play a role
in the context of the relations linking psychological factors and
performance. Some literature suggests that gender differences are the
product of environmental influences and that males' socialization is substantially different from females' socialization
(Greendorfer, 1977; Lewko & Ewing, 1980). In this cultural
conditioning view, males are socialized to be more competitive,
aggressive, and dominant than females and are more encouraged to take on
risks and face up to fears (Gill, 1988; Ginsburg & Silverman, 2000;
Kerr & Vlaminkx, 1997). Literature also shows that these differences
in socialization may already arise in early infancy and continue
throughout childhood and adolescence. In the very early years, boys tend
to be more active than girls (Eaton & Enns, 1986) and to engage in
different games and play-time activities (Fisher-Thompson, 1990;
Giuliano, Popp & Knight, 2000). In later years, teachers, coaches,
and peers may contribute to differential socialization by treating boys
and girls in very different ways, by having different expectations about
them, and by providing different practice opportunities (Thomas &
French, 1985). Over time, gender differences may crystallize and
influence boys' and girls" attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors
in significant ways. For instance, Eccles and Harold (1991) found that
adolescent boys valued sports and believed in their sport abilities more
than girls did. Other research has also pointed out that girls are less
likely to participate in activities promoting the development of motor
skills associated with sports (Thomas J.R. & Thomas K.T., 1988).
In several studies of physical activity, cultural conditioning has
been considered to account for males' relatively low levels of
anxiety and high levels of self-efficacy (Barberini, Dotto, Bortoli
& Robazza, 1996: Clifton & Gill, 1994; Jones & Swain, 1992;
Lirgg 1991 ; Moritz & Feltz, 2000). Nonetheless, while psychologists
have typically investigated the fear of failure and the fear of success
(e.g. Anshel, 1991; Conroy, 2001: Lazarus, 1991; Martens, Burton,
Vealey, Bump & Smith, 1990), the study of fear of physical injury
and the possible ways of coping with this emotion have received little
attention in the sports literature. Fear of injury might be common among
athletes, but there are no studies or data to confirm this claim.
In the present study, we examined fear of physical injury among
gymnasts at varying levels of technical expertise or competence. Several
considerations contributed to our choice of participants. Athletes in
gymnastics are required to execute movements on the floor and on
apparatus that are a challenge to gravity. The body is called upon to
carry out movements that can arouse emotions such as fear, worry, and
anxiety, often related to the risk of physical injury. For some
gymnasts, fear may become a major psychological barrier that prevents
learning and success. It may prevent the learning of new skills and
delay the improvement of skills already learned. Furthermore, in the
case of inexperienced youngsters, fear can even influence learning more
broadly by reducing motivation to train and by evoking withdrawal from
activities at an early stage. Finally, fear of physical injury may lead
to a specific behavioral block, that is, a refusal to execute a specific
technical movement.
We thus investigated gender, age, and professional-level
differences in gymnasts' fear of physical injury and its relations
to anxiety and self-efficacy for motor performance. This interest was
motivated on at least three separate grounds. First, psychological
literature has recently highlighted the importance of examining emotions
not as general or trait-like phenomena, but rather as self-regulatory
mechanisms that are influenced by and may influence specific,
context-bound, cognitive experiences (Bandura, 1999; Caprara &
Cervone, 2001; Mischel & Shoda, 1999). In line with this theoretical
perspective, gymnasts' fear of physical injury can reasonably be
the expression of a complex pattern of individual experiences. This
pattern could involve both general dispositions such as a gymnast's
tendency to experience anxiety, and specific self-evaluation processes
such as the degree to which an athlete is confident in his or her
ability to perform. Second, the notion of cultural conditioning
inherently embraces the possibility that anxiety, self-efficacy, and
fear of physical injury may operate differently in different individuals
who are at different ages or levels of professional and technical
competence. Thus, male and female (or low vs. high professional-level)
gymnasts may differ not only at average group levels but also in terms
of the relations linking anxiety, self-efficacy, and fear of physical
injury (i.e., an hypothesis about moderating group effects). Third, the
study of group differences in gymnasts' fear of injury represents a
novel step in sport research conducted within the Italian culture.
In the present investigation, Italian male and female gymnasts
completed a series of questionnaires measuring trait anxiety,
self-efficacy, and fear of physical injury. The overall purpose of the
study was to measure these constructs and examine how their average
levels and interrelations varied with gymnasts' gender, age, and
professional-level. We considered anxiety as an individual's
general trait-like disposition to experience apprehension and tension
across many different situations. Self-efficacy refers instead to
one's level of confidence in performing a given activity or
behavior well (Bandura, 1999). Following the work of Robazza and Bortoli
(1994), we studied self-efficacy for physical abilities by measuring
gymnasts' confidence in performing motor or physical tasks.
As to the issue of mean group differences on the variables of
interest, we hypothesized, in line with previous studies (Bortoli &
Robazza, 1993; Clifton & Gill, 1994; Lirgg, 1991), that male
gymnasts would express greater confidence in performing physical
activities and less anxiety than female gymnasts. Furthermore, based on
cultural stereotypes depicting women as less willing to take risks (Kerr
& Vlaminkx, 1997), we also hypothesized that female gymnasts would
experience more fear of physical injury than male gymnasts. Finally, in
line with the hypothesis of contextual or cultural effects, we explored
whether fear of physical injury varied with gymnasts' level of
experience. Two possibilities seemed plausible. On one hand, high
technical competence or experience with competition might protect
gymnasts from experiencing fear of physical injury. Thus, greater levels
of competence/competition might be related to lesser fear of physical
injury. On the other hand, however, high technical competence or
experience with competition tends to be associated with relatively more
difficult physical exercises that put gymnasts at risk for serious
injuries or accidents. Thus, greater levels of competence/competition
might be related to stronger fear of physical injury.
Finally, as to the issue of group differences in the relations
among the variables of interest (i.e., the possibility of moderating
group effects), we explored the possibility that self-efficacy and
anxiety predicted tear of physical injury differently in young versus
old, male versus female, and high- versus low-experienced, gymnasts.
Method
Participants
One hundred eighty-six Italian gymnasts for whom parental consent
was obtained participated in the present research. Eighty-three males
and one hundred and three females, all of whom had been practicing
gymnastics for at least two years, were contacted while attending summer
camps and asked to complete a series of three questionnaires.
Participants were assessed individually in one testing session at the
end of their daily training. The gymnasts' age ranged from 8 to 19
years (M = 12.96; SD = 3.32). For the purpose of examining age
differences, the participants were divided into three age groups that
represented, respectively, late childhood (8-10 years-old), early
adolescence (11-14 years-old), and adolescence (15-19 years-old).
Participants were also divided into two groups of regional and national
professional-levels, respectively. This classification took into
consideration both the difficulty level of the exercises that the
gymnasts typically performed, and the type of professional competition
(regional vs. national or international) in which they typically
participated. These criteria led to a classification of 97
national-level gymnasts and 89 regional-level gymnasts. Table 1 shows
the sample composition across gender, age, and technical levels.
Instruments
Self-Efficacy for Physical Abilities. This questionnaire (SEM-S,
Bortoli & Robazza 1996 cited in Barberini, Dotto, Bortoli &
Robazza, 1996, see Appendix) measured a person's view of his/her
own physical abilities. The questionnaire included 10 items, comprising
four positive self-efficacy items (e.g., "I have good control of my
movements in all situations"), three negative self-efficacy items
(e.g., "I have little confidence in my motor abilities"), and
three self-efficacy items for "demanding situations" (e.g.,
"I can quickly resolve the problems that I may encounter during
competitions"). Items were answered on a five-point scale (not at
all = 1 ;just a little = 2; somewhat = 3; a lot = 4; quite a lot = 5).
The scores for negative items were reversed, and a self-efficacy total
score was obtained by summing scores across all items. High scores
indicated a high level of self-efficacy in physical abilities. The
reliability of the instrument in measuring systematic individual
differences in physical self-efficacy was high (Cronbach's [alpha]
= .79).
Trait Anxiety. This questionnaire (Busnelli, Dall'Aglio, &
Faina, 1974) included twenty items measuring trait anxiety in daily life
situations (e.g., "Are you afraid when you have to go to the
doctor'?"). Items were answered on a 3-point scale (never = 1;
sometimes = 2; almost always = 3). The internal consistency of the items
was acceptable (Cronbach's [alpha] = .72). (1)
Gymnastics Fear Inventory. This questionnaire was developed for
this study on the basis of information obtained in a focus group
including gymnasts and experts on gymnastics. In the present study,
athletes answered several questions concerning their fears of getting
hurt during physical activities and exercise. The athletes answered the
following five questions: 1) "Do you consider yourself to be an
apprehensive gymnast?", 2) "Are you afraid of getting
hurt?", 3) "Are you afraid of trying out new
exercises'?", 4) "Are you afraid of trying out an
exercise that you already know?", 5) "Does it ever happen that
you imagine (or think) of getting hurt before canting out an
exercise?". Item scores ranged from 1 to 4 (never = 1 ; hardly at
all = 2; quite often = 3; very much = 4). The total score was obtained
by summing scores across the five items. The internal consistency of
these five items was quite acceptable (Cronbach's [alpha] = .77).
Results
Mean Group Differences in Anxiety, Physical Self Efficacy, and Fear
of Physical hi jury
We first performed a series of three 2 x 2 ANOVAs to examine
possible gender, age, and professional-level differences in the mean
scores of fear, anxiety, and self-efficacy for physical abilities,
respectively. In these analyses, gender (male, female), age (late
childhood, early adolescence, and adolescence), and professional-level
(regional, national) were entered as fixed factors.
Table 2 shows the mean scores and standard deviations for anxiety,
fear of physical injury, and self-efficacy across gender, age, and
professional-level groups. On average, male gymnasts were significantly
less anxious, F(1, 174) = 50.82, p<.001, partial [[eta].sup.2] = .23,
power = 1.0, and more efficacious, F(1, 174) = 5.33, p < .02, partial
[[eta].sup.2] = .03, power = .63 than were female gymnasts. Regarding
fear of physical injury, female gymnasts tended to be slightly more
fearful of physical injury than male gymnasts, but this difference did
not reach statistical significance, F(1, 174) = 3.48, p = .06, partial
[[eta].sup.2] = .02, power = .46.
On average, children were significantly more anxious than late
adolescents, F(1, 174) = 3.16, p = .04, partial [[eta].sup.2] = .03,
power = .60, with differences particularly marked and statistically
significant between the youngest and the oldest gymnasts (p > .01).
Finally, there also was a marginally significant joint effect of
participants' age and professional-level on athletes' fear of
physical injury, F(2, 174) = 2.80, p = .06, partial [[eta].sup.2] = .03,
power = .54. In particular, while the youngest gymnasts were the most
fearful among the regional-level athletes, the oldest gymnasts were the
most fearful of physical injury among the more experienced,
national-level, athletes.
Group differences in the Relations among Fear, Anxiety, and
Physical Self-Efficacy
In addition to examining group differences in the average levels of
gymnasts' anxiety, self-efficacy, and fear of physical injury, we
evaluated whether gender, age, or professional-level moderated the
relations among these variables. That is, we were interested in
examining whether the magnitude of the relations varied significantly
across gender, age, or professional-level groups. Illustratively, this
analysis would, for instance, reveal the extent to which anxiety or
self-efficacy would predict fear in male gymnasts but not in female
gynmasts. This analysis would then serve the purpose of examining
whether psychological processes linking anxiety, self-efficacy, and fear
of physical injury generalize across groups.
Overall, fear correlated significantly and negatively with physical
self-efficacy (r = -.44, p < .01) and positively with anxiety (r =
.33, p < .01). Furthermore, physical self-efficacy correlated
negatively with anxiety (r = -.43, p < .01). Table 3 shows both these
overall correlations and the correlations computed for the different
gender, age, and professional-level groups. As a whole, correlations did
not vary much with the gymnasts' gender and level of professional
experience, suggesting that the processes through which these
psychological factors operate might not be affected in significant ways
by differing cultural and socialization experiences. Correlational
differences were, however, more marked across age groups. In particular,
it seemed that the correlations between trait anxiety and fear of
physical injury were substantial and positive among the youngest
gymnasts and decreased gradually across age to then become virtually
zero among the oldest gymnasts. An opposite trend emerged instead for
the negative correlation between sell-efficacy and fear of physical
injury (i.e., the oldest group showed the strongest correlation). Thus,
while correlations among the three psychological constructs did not seem
to vary with gender and professional-level, they seem to vary with
gymnasts' age.
In order to evaluate these possibilities further, we performed a
hierarchical regression analysis in which fear of physical injury was
first predicted by the main effects accrued by gender,
professional-level, and gynmasts' age (step 1), then by the main
effects accrued by self-efficacy and anxiety (step 2), and thirdly by
the interactive effects created by multiplying each factor in step 1
with each of the two predictors used in step 2 (step 3). At each
hierarchical regression step, the estimation was performed via a
stepwise-forward method. Overall, the presence of statistically
significant regression effects for any of the six interaction terms
included in step 3 would prove that the relations between fear of
physical injury and self-efficacy or anxiety were different across
gender, age, or professional-level groups.
This hierarchical regression yielded a statistically significant
model of effects at each step, F(1,184) = 4.29, p =.04 at step 1,F(2,
183) = 24.68, p = .001 at step 2, and F(3, 182) = 18.16, p = .001 at
step 3. Overall, the final model significantly explained 23% of the
variance in gymnasts' fear of physical injury, and predictors at
each step provided statistically significant increments in explained
variance.
In line with the visual inspection of the bivariate correlations
reported in table 3, there were no interaction terms that entered the
final model. Thus, the relations linking fear, self-efficacy, and
anxiety did not vary across gymnasts' gender, age, and
professional-levels. There, however, were statistically significant main
effects for self-efficacy and anxiety. In particular, gymnasts who were
more confident in their abilities experienced less fear of physical
injury ([beta] = -.43, p <. 001), whereas gymnasts who were more
anxious experienced greater fear of physical injury ([beta] = .28, p<
.001).
Discussion
To the best of our knowledge, the present research is the first
study that examined the relations linking self-efficacy for physical
performance, anxiety, and fear of physical injury in an Italian sample
of male and female gynmasts. The study examined these relations as a
function of gender and age, as well as differing levels of
gymnasts' professional experience. The study responded to important
theoretical and research issues. First, psychological literature
suggests that the study of emotions must be embedded in an investigation
of individuals' cognitive and self-regulatory processes (Bandura,
1999: Higgins, 1990; Mischel & Shoda, 1999). The study of how
self-efficacy for physical performance is related to fear of physical
injury responded to this important theoretical call. Second,
individuals' peculiar socialization and cultural experiences may
shape these processes and there may, theretbre, exist important
individual differences in how emotion, cognition, and self-regulation
operate. In this study, we addressed this issue by examining whether the
relations linking self-efficacy, anxiety, and fear were significantly
different in the diverse gender, age, and professional-level groups
(i.e., the hypothesis of moderating effects).
In line with these guiding interests, the results of the study were
interesting in several respects. On average, Italian female gymnasts
were more anxious and felt less confident to do well in physical
performance than male gynmasts. This finding is quite in line with
research that has highlighted the role of differing cultural norms for
males and females and that has documented how males receive more
encouragement in sport than females from family members, peers,
teachers, and coaches (Giuliano et al., 2000). Despite its apparent
inconsistency with these results, the additional finding that male and
female gymnasts did not differ on their average levels of fear of
physical injury did not seem so surprising, in retrospect. The physical
risks associated with the sport of gynmastics are particularly evident
to whoever wants to practice this sport and, as such, gymnastics
probably tends to attract athletes who are willing to overcome these
risks. This may explain the lack of gender differences in fear. In order
to evaluate this hypothesis, it would be quite informative to measure,
with appropriated tests, the different views about risk of male and
female athletes in several sports and compare these views to those
obtained for male and female subjects who do not practice any sport.
Finally, very young gymnasts, reported greater anxiety and fear of
physical injury than adolescent gymnasts, although this age difference
seemed to hold especially for those Italian gymnasts who are in the
early stage of their professional career and typically participate in
regional competitions.
Despite these group differences, the pattern of bivariate relations
linking fear of physical injury to self-efficacy and to anxiety were not
different in male and female gymnasts, in children and adolescents, and
in gymnasts who were at different levels of competence or competition
type. This finding emerged both from a visual analysis of the
correlations among the psychological constructs reported in table 3 and
from a formal statistical regression analysis. It is first important to
point out that, despite relying exclusively on self-report data, the
correlations among the psychological constructs were all in the expected
direction and varied to some extent across variables, thus supporting
indirectly the discriminant validity of the measurements used in the
study.
The correlations suggested that tear of physical Injury may
partially rise in athletes who tend to experience anxiety and who do not
have or engage in self-reassuring cognitions about their own abilities.
Of course, the cross-sectional and correlational nature of the study can
not provide conclusive data on this possibility. More importantly,
however, the correlation and regression findings, together, clearly
showed that the magnitude of these relations did not vary much across
groups, be they gender or age groups as well as groups formed on the
basis of the gymnasts' level of competence/competition. These
findings suggest that the experience of fear of physical injury follows
a similar pattern in different individuals and that the intervening
mechanisms that could account for this experience, be they a general
tendency towards anxiety or lack of self-reassuring self-efficacy
cognitions, have the same importance or play the same role in male and
female, young and older adolescent, and regional-level and
national-level gymnasts. These findings also suggest that the different
processes of socialization that male or female gymnasts experience in
their younger years with respect to physical activity, exercise, or
sport do not produce significant differences in how they psychologically
confront and deal with their daily performances, fears, or physical
exercise tasks.
These conclusions call for caution, however. In addition to the
correlational nature of the study, which poses clear limits in the
validity of any conclusion concerning processes and modelling effects,
the present findings also yielded small effect sizes for the tests of
group mean differences in the psychological variables of interest, with
the exception--perhaps--of the differences in anxiety reported by male
and female gymnasts. This finding raises the question of whether gender,
age, and professional competence/competition of Italian gymnasts are the
most useful factors to examine differences in the athletes'
psychological experiences concerning their sport discipline and its
physical demands. Future studies should be designed to address and
clarify these issues.
Despite these limitations, however, we believe that the present
study supports the general notion that an adequate motor and sport
education may aid in building or reinforcing people's
self-confidence and, perhaps, in correcting or ameliorating their
negative emotional or thinking tendencies. Consistent with this notion,
Bandura (1977; 1999) suggested that the acquisition of abilities and
their practice permit one to confront difficult situations and may lead
to an increasingly more positive perception of the environment, in such
a way as to render it less dangerous. In the end, we feel it would be
desirable and important to broaden our investigation to include in-depth
research on the cognitive and behavioural aspects correlated with fear
of physical injury, such as, for example, self-talk and visual imagery.
This research would provide further insights on how the professional
performance of athletes and gymnasts significantly depend on
psychological processes and dimensions of individual differences.
Appendix
Self-Efficacy for Physical Abilities
1. I have good control of my movements in all situations
2. I have limited motor abilities
3. I can quickly resolve the problems that I may encounter during
competitions
4. I have little confidence in my motor abilities
5. My physical abilities allow me to perform complex and difficult
movements well
6. I think nay movements are slow and awkward
7. I have a good level of motor ability
8. I have trouble at making important decisions during competitions
9. I have excellent reflexes
10. When I am in difficult situations, I have little control of the
actions I must perform
Table 1
Sample composition across gender, age and professional-level groups
Age Males Females National level Regional level Total
8-10 23 31 24 30 54
11-14 31 42 32 41 73
15-19 29 30 41 18 59
Total 83 103 97 89 186
Table 2
Mean and Standard Deviations for-Anxiety: Self-Efficacy,
and Fear by Athletes'Gender; Age and Professional-Level
Anxiety Self-efficacy Fear
All cases 11.86 (5.90) 37.93 (5.14) 9.06 (2.48)
Males 8.75 (4.60) 38.99 (5.15) 8.73 (2.39)
Females 14.36 (5.69) 37.08 (4.98) 9.33 (2.54)
Age 8-10 13.57 (5.82) 37.74 (4.95) 8.89 (2.72)
Age 11-14 11.64 (5.82) 37.45 (5.03) 8.92 (2.53)
Age 15-19 10.56 (5.82) 38.69 (5.42) 9.41 (2.20)
National level 11.81 (5.90) 37.88 (5.12) 9.42 (2.45)
Regional level 11.91 (5.95) 37.99 (5.18) 8.67 (2.48)
Table 3
Bivariate Zero-Order Correlations linking Physical
Self-Efficacy, Anxiety, and Fear of Physical Injury as a
Function of Gymnasts' Gender, Age, and Professional-Level
Correlations between
Fear and Fear and Anxiety and
Anxiety Self-Efficacy Self-Efficacy
All cases (n = 186) .33 ** -.44 ** -.43 **
Males (n = 83) .36 ** -.40 ** -.43 **
Females (n = 103) .23 * -.46 ** -.36 **
Age 8-10 (n = 54) .57 ** -.33 * -.27 *
Age 11-14 (n = 73) .29 * -.49 ** -.48 **
Age 15-19 (n = 59) .00 -.54 ** -.23 ***
National level (n = 97) .38 * -.41 ** -.43 **
Regional level (n = 89) .31 ** -.47 ** -.43 **
Note: * p.<.05; ** p.<.01; *** p.<.10
(1) In order to verify the construct validity of the Italian trait
anxiety measure used in this study (Busnelli, Dall'Aglio, &
Faina. 1974), children's, preadolescents', and
adolescents" scores were correlated with these participants'
respective scores on the trait scale of the STAI inventory (State Trait
Anxiety Inventory, Form Y, Spielberger, 1983). In this regard, it is
important to note that the STAI inventory was not designed or validated
as a measure of trait anxiety in elementary school children, as the
present study would have required. We expected that the correlations
between the two trait measures would be positive and statistically
significant in the two older groups and non-significant in the youngest
subgroup. The results of these analyses confirmed our hypotheses. In
particular, the two measures correlated positively and significantly
among the middle school athletes (.37) and particularly well among the
high school athletes (.43). On the contrary, the correlation between the
two measures was statistically non-significant in the youngest group
(.25). Thus, these correlation analyses supported the validity of the
trait measure used in the present study.
References
Anshel, M. H. (1991). Causes of drug abuse in sport: a survey of
intercollegiate athletes. Journal of Sport Behavior, 14, 283-307
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of
behavioural change. Psychological Reviews; 84, 191-207
Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of personality. In D.
Cervone & Y. Shoda (Eds.), "The coherence of personality:
Social cognitive bases of consistency variability, and
organization" 185-241. New York: Guilford Press.
Barberini, G., Dotto, A., Bortoli, L., & Robazza C. (1996).
Self-efficacy, ansia ed emozioni nello sport giovanile [Self-efficacy,
anxiety and emotions in youth sport]. Movimento, 12, 54-56
Bortoli, L., & Robazza, C. (1993). Le aspettative personali di
efficacia nell'apprendimento motorio e nella prestazione [The
individual efficacy expectations in motor learning and per formance].
Didattica del movimento, 86/87, 48-56
Busnelli, C., Dall'Aglio, E., & Faina, E(1974).
Questionario Scala d'Ansia per l'Eta Evolutiva, manuale di
istruzioni [Trait anxiety questionnaire for young, instruction manual].
Organizzazioni Speciali, Firenze
Caprara, G.V., & Cervone, D. (2001). Personality: Determinants,
Dynamics, & Potentials. Cambridge University Press.
Clifton, R.T., & Gill, D.L.(1994).Gender differences in
self-confidence on a feminine-type task. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 16, 150-162
Conroy, D.E.(2001). Fear of failure: an exemplar for social
development research in sport. Quest. 53(2), 165-183
Corbin, C.B., Stewart, M.J., & Blair, W.O. (1981).
Self-confidence and motor performance of preadolescent boys and girls
studied in different feedbaback situations. Journal of Sport Psychology
3, 30-34
Eaton, W.O., & Enns, L.R.(1986). Sex differences in human motor
activity level. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 19-28
Eccles, J.S., & Harold, R.D. (1991). Gender differences in
sport involvement: applying the Eccles' expectancy-value model.
Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 3, 7-13
Feltz, D.L., & Landers, D.M. (1983). The effects of mental
practice in motor skill learning and performance: A meta-analysis.
Journal of Sport Psychology. 5, 25-27
Fisher-Thompson, D.(1990). Adult sex typing of children's
toys. Sex Roles, 23, 291-303
Gill, D. (1988). Gender differences in competitive orientation and
sport participation. International Journal of Sport Psychology,
19,145-159
Ginsburg, G.S., & Silverman, W.K. (2000). Gender role
orientation and fearfulness in children with anxiety disorders. Journal
of Anxiety Disorders, 14(1), 57-67
Giuliano, T.A., Popp, K.E., & Knight, J.L.(2000). Footballs
versus barbies: childhood play activities as predictors of sport
participation by women. Sex roles. 42,(3/4), 159-181
Greendorfer, S.L. (1977). Role of socializing agents in female
sport involvement. Research Quarterly, 48, 304-310
Higgins, E.T. (1990). Personality, social psychology, and
person-situation relations: Standards and knowledge activation as a
common language. In L.A. Pervin (Ed.), "Handbook of personality:
Theory and Research" 301-338 New York: Guilford Press.
Jones, G., & Swain, A.(1992). Intensity and direction as
dimensions of competitive state anxiety and relationships with
competitiveness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 74, 467-473
Kerr, J.(1987). Structural phenomenology, arousal and performance.
Journal of Human Movement Studies, 13, 211-215
Kerr, J., & Vlaminkx, J. (1997). Gender differences in the
experience of risk. Personality & Individual Differences, 22(2),
293-295
Lazarus, R.S.(1991). Cognition and motivation in emotion. American
Psychologist, 46, 352-367
Lewko, J.H., & Ewing, M.E.(1980). Sex differences and parental
influence in sport involvement of children. Journal of Sport Psychology,
2, 62-68
Lirgg, C.D.(1991).Gender differences in self-confidence in physical
activity: A meta-analysis of recent studies. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 8, 294-310
Martens, R., Burton, D., Vealey. R.S., Bump, L.A., & Smith,
D.E.(1990). Development and validation of the Competitive State Anxiety
Inventory-2. Competitive anxiety in sport. Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics, 117-190
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1999). Integrating dispositions and
processing dynamics within a unified theory of personality: The
cognitive-affective personality system. In L.A. Pervin, & O.P. John
(Eds.), "Handbook of personality: Theory and Research",
197-218. New York: Guilford Press.
Moritz, S., & Feltz, D.(2000). The relation of self-efficacy
measures to sport performance: a meta-analytic review. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, sep., 280-288
Robazza, C., & Bortoli, L.(1994). Apprendimento di abilita
motorie per il superamento dell'ansia [Learning of motor skills to
overcome anxiety]. Movimento, 10,64-66
Sonstroem, R. (1974). Attitude testing examining certain
psychological correlates of physical activity. Research Quarterly, 45,
93-103
Spielberger C.D. (1983). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, Form Y
Manual. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto. CA
Thomas J.R., & French, K.E.(1985). Gender differences across
age in motor performance: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 98,
260-282
Thomas, J.R., & Thomas, K.T.(1988). Development of gender
differences in physical activity. Quest, 40, 219-229
Address Correspondence To: Anna Claudia Cartoni,University
Institute of Motor Sciences of Rome, Piazza Lauro de Bosis 15, 00194
Rome, Italy
Anna Claudia Cartoni, Carlo Minganti and Arnaldo Zelli
University Institute of Movement Science of Rome