Effects of motivational music on work output and affective responses during sub-maximal cycling of a standardized perceived intensity.
Elliott, Dave ; Carr, Sam ; Savage, Dave 等
Researchers (e.g., Triandis, 1977) examining the antecedents of
exercise adherence have suggested that affective responses might exert
an influence on individuals' intention to exercise. According to Godin and Shephard (1990) the affective dimension typically refers to
individuals' emotional responses to the thought of adopting
behaviors and feelings elicited by the change in behavior (e.g., is the
behavior perceived as pleasant/unpleasant or interesting/ boring?).
Crucial to the development of such affective patterns are previous
experiences of a given behavior, with positive experiences engendering
positive feelings and a subsequent desire to repeat the behavior (Godin,
1994). Hence, it is important that participation in exercise sessions is
a positive experience for individuals (Godin, 1994).
In ensuring that exercise is shaped into a positive experience,
researchers (e.g., Hardy & Rejeski, 1989) have suggested that
attention should be paid to both "how one feels" and
"what one feels" during the exercise experience. The former
refers to individual subjective feelings during exercise that may be
characterized by moods or feelings of pleasure or displeasure (Hardy
& Rejeski, 1989). The latter refers to subjective estimates of
physical work intensity, also considered to be of major importance to
those concerned with exercise prescription (Rejeski, 1985). It is
suggested that both these factors play a role in exercise adherence
(Rejeski, 1985). One strategy that has shown potential in positively
influencing both of these subjective components during exercise
experiences is the application of music. For example, from assessment of
participants' mood during exercise, Boutcher and Trenske (1990),
Brownley, McMurray, and Hackney (1995), and Karageorghis and Terry
(2000) have all indicated that music appears to elicit positive
affective feelings during exercise. Hence, improved affective responses
as a result of the application of music during exercise could increase
the likelihood of individuals attending exercise sessions.
However, some authors (e.g., Perkins & Epstein, 1988) have
contended that attendance may be an insufficient measure of adherence.
According to Perkins and Epstein (1988), factors such as the duration
and intensity of exercise also require consideration. That is,
individuals may be required to reach objectively defined levels of
exercise intensity or duration in specific exercise sessions in order
for adherence to a given exercise program to be evident. As such, the
possibility exists that individuals may, for example, fulfil attendance
criteria but may not be meeting the specified intensities or duration.
Related to this, a body of evidence suggests that music may also be a
viable aid for individuals' to reach specified levels of exercise
intensity. For example, Szabo, Small and Leigh (1999) and Karageorghis
and Jones (2000) found that music could improve progressive cycle
endurance. However, such findings are not conclusive, with numerous
studies also suggesting that music has little impact on work rates
(e.g., Dorney, Goh, & Lee, 1992; Becker, Chambliss, Marsh &
Montemayor, 1995).
In response to such contradictions, Karageorghis and Terry (1997)
specified the need to address the methodological designs employed to
examine musical effects. They suggested that an awareness of factors
such as type of music employed, socio-cultural influences and
appropriate dependent measures was required. Accordingly, Karageorghis,
Terry and Lane (1999) emphasized the need for theory-driven research in
this area and proposed the Conceptual Model for the Prediction of
Responses to Motivational Asynchronous Music in Exercise and Sport. The
model indicates that particular attention should be given to both
musical factors (i.e., volume, tempo, rhythm, melody, lyrics) and
personal factors (i.e., socio-cultural background and personal meaning)
when selecting musical accompaniment to exercise. Attending to these
factors may allow music to be termed motivational (i.e., a piece of
music that inspires or stimulates physical activity). It is claimed that
if music can be described as being motivational and consideration is
given to appropriate methodological design, research outcomes will be
more consistent and supportive of the hypothesis that music can
positively influence physical activity.
Further developments have led to the formulation of an operational
rating inventory labeled the Brunel Music Rating Inventory (BMRI;
Karageorghis, Terry & Lane, 1999). It is suggested that the BMRI
provides sport scientists and practitioners with a valid method of
evaluating the motivational qualities of music in accordance with the
predictions of Karageorghis and his colleagues.
Research employing the above framework, in conjunction with the
BMRI, in sport and exercise situations has proved to be encouraging.
Specifically, motivational music (as identified by BMRI ratings) has
been found to increase aerobic endurance (Karageorghis & Jones,
2000), isometric muscular endurance (Karageorghis & Lee, 2001), and
in-exercise affective states (Karageorghis & Terry, 1999), to reduce
perceptions of effort (Karageorghis & Terry, 1999), and to induce
pre- and in-task flow states (Karageorghis & Terry, 1999;
Karageorghis & Deeth 2002).
In summary, evidence exists to support the notion that, where
appropriate consideration is given to relevant factors, music can create
a more positive exercise experience and may also positively influence
participants' work output. However, these factors may function
antagonistically. That is, participants need a positive exercise
experience, yet may also need to increase objective intensity in order
to meet specific intensity demands. Given that research has suggested
that in-exercise work intensity may be negatively associated with
in-exercise feelings and mood (e.g., Hardy & Rejeski, 1989), it may
be that as objective intensity increases then a potential forfeiture is
individuals' feeling states. Unlike previous studies that have been
conducted in this area the current study did not employ objective
methods in order to standardize exercise intensity (e.g., Boutcher &
Trenske, 1990; Brownley, McMurray, & Hackney, 1995). Rather, this
study standardized perceptions of effort so that participants perceived
that they were working at a given intensity. This way, an important
affective component of the subjective exercise experience had been
controlled, whilst potentially allowing for a degree of variation in
objective output.
Subsequently, we aimed to discern whether, although participants
were working at a given perceived intensity, they would actually
register greater work-output with the application of motivational music
without registering any conscious awareness of increases in objective
effort (due to the fact that they were exercising at a standardized
perceived intensity). In addition, we also aimed to ascertain whether
any increases in work-output elicited by motivational music would relate
to a detriment in feeling states.
Method
Music selection
Initially, 14 musical selections were identified in accordance with
the following criteria: Tempo. Tempo is considered to be one of the most
powerful elements of music (Gundlauch, 1935; Karageorghis & Terry,
1997). If tempi of tracks are not reasonably similar then differential
responses to musical selections may well be a response to this component
alone and not the additional components of the model proposed by
Karageorghis et al. (1999). That is, internal validity of the BMRI
rating procedures could be compromised in that individuals'
responses to items may simply reflect their preference for a given tempo
and not the motivational quality of the specific musical selection under
scrutiny. According to Iwanaga (1995) individuals tend to prefer tempi
that are congruent with their heart rate. In order to ascertain a
probable heart rate range for this study, a pilot study was conducted.
Specifically, six participants were required to cycle at an RPE (Borg,
1971) of '13' for 12-minutes and average heart rates during
exercise were recorded. It was identified that participants' mean
heart rate during steady state exercise was approximately 132 beats per
minute. Although, LeBlanc, Colman, McCrary, Sherrill and Malin (1988)
disagree with Iwanaga's contentions, these researchers have
suggested that tempi of approximately 134 beats per minute are
considered to be favorable. Hence, for this study tracks possessing a
tempo of approximately 130bpm were selected. Although tempi were
reasonably similar, the accentuation of the tempo differed between
selections. It was therefore assumed that this would influence the
motivational qualities of this element of the music.
Musical idiom. Karageorghis and Terry (1997) are critical of
including contrasting idioms into music research because subject bias
towards or against a particular style may have a powerful effect on
responses to music. Again, such bias is likely to compromise the
internal validity of the BMRI rating procedures. In this study tracks
were chosen so that they all possessed a consistent electronic bass beat
and were therefore categorized as being dance music.
Cultural appropriateness and familiarity. For the reasons stated
above, measures were taken to ensure a degree of cultural
appropriateness and familiarity within the musical selections.
Specifically, all musical selections had appeared in the British music
charts within the last year and all participants involved in the study
were required to have a "liking" of the musical idiom used.
Rating selected tracks for their motivational qualities
The 14 identified musical selections were therefore reasonably
similar in terms of their tempo, musical idiom, cultural
appropriateness, and familiarity. To assess any generalizable
differences in the motivational qualities of these musical selections
the BMRI (Karageorghis et al., 1999) was employed. Specifically, the
13-item BMRI is based around the Revised Conceptual Framework for
Prediction of Responses to Motivational Asynchronous Music in Exercise
and Sport (Karageorghis et al., 1999) and consists of sub-scales
designed to assess the motivational qualities of rhythmic response,
musicality, cultural impact, and association of various musical
selections. Participants are required to rate the motivational quality
of each of the 13items on a 10-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1
(Not at all motivating) to 10 (Extremely motivating). Examples of items
from each scale are "Tempo (beat)" (assesses rhythmic response
sub-scale), "Melody" (assesses musicality), "The
artist" (assesses cultural impact), and "Association of music
with a film/video" (assesses association). The instrument has been
demonstrated to possess adequate reliability, validity, and factor
structure (Karageorghis et al., 1999).
Participants for music rating procedures
Participants involved in the music rating procedures were 55
undergraduates attending Universities in the UK. Specifically, there
were 29 males (M = 21.6 years, SD = 2.3) and 26 females (M = 20.9 years,
SD = 2.5). All participants were of similar cultural background in that
they originated from the UK and had a "liking" of mainstream
dance music.
Equipment for music rating procedures
The 14 initially identified musical selections were recorded onto a
TDK, FE ferric 90 audiotape. Music was played to participants through a
Tandberg R822 Audio Tutor cassette player. Musical selections were rated
for their motivational quotient through the implementation of the BMRI
(Karageorghis et al., 1999).
Music rating procedures
Initially it was ensured that all participants expressed a
"liking" of mainstream dance music. Any individual expressing
a dislike was asked to abstain from the rating procedure. Each subject
involved was then provided with a booklet containing 14 copies of the
BMRI (Karageorghis et al., 1999). In accordance with the recommendations
of Karageorghis et al. (1999), participants were informed that each
musical selection should be rated with the specific task of sub-maximal
cycling in mind. A brief explanation of the rating scale was provided
including further instructions regarding the meaning of each of the 13
items. Participants were then asked if they had any questions regarding
the procedure. During the procedure participants were asked to refrain
from conferring with peers. The first musical selection was then
presented, after which the tape was stopped to allow each individual to
complete the rating procedure. The subsequent musical selections were
rated in an identical fashion with respect to their motivational
qualities.
Scoring the motivational qualities of the musical selections
Upon completion of the rating procedure the motivational quotient
for each musical selection was calculated by following the procedure
suggested by Karageorghis et al. (1999). Specifically, participants each
obtain a raw score for how motivating they find each sub-scale (rhythmic
response, musicality, cultural impact, association) for each musical
selection. The raw score on each sub-scale is then multiplied by a
weighting factor identified by Kareageorghis et al. (1999). This is so
that the hierarchical importance of each sub-scale is reflected in the
final motivational quotient. The four weighted sub-scales are then
summed in order to identify a total motivational quotient for each
musical selection for each participant. Mean sample motivational
quotients were then calculated for each of the 14 tracks.
The median motivational quotient for the 14 musical selections in
this study was 15.06 and quotients for individual tracks ranged from
7.05 (SD = 4.0) to 24.65 (SD = 4.63). The four musical selections with
the highest motivational quotients were recorded in full onto an
audiotape (TDK, FE ferric 90). This cassette was deemed to reflect
"'motivational" music, as identified by the BMRI rating
procedure. Generally, scores of the motivational music selections tended
to be beyond the middle of the range for each weighted subscale score in
accordance with the recommendations of Karageorghis et al. (1999).
However, on the association dimension, two of the motivational tracks
tended to be closer to the center of the range. Similarly, the four
selections with the lowest motivational quotients were recorded onto an
identical audiotape. This music was deemed to reflect
"oudeterous" music. The four motivational musical selections
had quotients ranging from 18.17 (SD = 6.01) to 24.65 (SD = 4.22) and
the average motivational quotient for these four tracks was 20.92 (SD =
1.63). The four oudeterous musical selections had quotients ranging from
7.05 (SD = 4.0) to 10.96 (SD = 4.83) and the average motivational
quotient for these four tracks was 9.12 (SD = 2.80). The average
motivational quotients for motivational vs. oudeterous music were
significantly different (t = 5.37, p < .01), indicating that the
motivational qualities of the two musical compilations were
significantly disparate.
Experimental Details
Participants
Participants in the experiment were 18 sport and exercise science
undergraduates attending universities in the UK. Specifically, there
were 8 males (M = 22.1 years SD = 1.4) and 10 females (M = 21.7 years,
SD = 0.7). All participants were homogenous in terms of cultural
background (i.e., originated in the UK and had a "liking" of
mainstream dance music) and had previous experience of performing
sub-maximal exercise on cycle ergometers.
Participants were required to perform in three sub-maximal exercise
trials (no music--where participants were required to listen to the
sound of a "blank tape", oudeterous music, and motivational
music) on a "Monark 818E Ergomedic ergometer cycle,"
calibrated and standardized to a resistance of 10N. The study was
conducted over a three-week period, with participants performing one
exercise trial per week. Each experimental trial was 12 minutes in
duration (excluding a three minute warm up and one minute cool down).
Trial order was counterbalanced to control for order effects.
Specifically, of the nine pairs of participants, the first six pairs
were allocated to one of six possible counterbalancing orders. The
remaining three pairs of participants were then randomly allocated to
one of the six counterbalancing orders. All trials were conducted at a
similar time of day and participants were asked to refrain from eating
or drinking caffeine-based products one hour prior to testing.
Participants were tested in pairs, however cycle ergometers were
placed so that they were unable to observe each other. Trials were
conducted within a laboratory environment to eliminate external
extraneous variables. During each trial, participants were required to
wear headphones and music (or a blank tape in the no music condition)
was played through a "Tandberg R822 Audio Tutor" cassette
player. Specifically, two pairs of "HD 2020" headphones were
consistently used and volume intensity was standardized to a level of
70% of maximum volume throughout the experiment. To reduce personal goal
setting effects data concerning distance traveled during each trial was
withheld from participants until a final debriefing session.
Borg's (1971) Rate of Perceived Exertion Scale was applied in
order to ensure that the same perceived sub-maximal intensity of
exercise was consistently maintained during all trials. That is,
participants were specifically instructed to maintain a specified RPE
throughout the duration of each trial in order to observe whether any
objective increases in work output were made without being subjectively
recognized by participants (due to the fact that they were exercising at
a standardized RPE). Noble and Robertson (1996) have suggested that
without practice, participants may have difficulty linking semantic
anchors with numeric values in scales such as Borg's RPE. Hence, in
this study it was ensured that all participants had prior knowledge of
the Borg scale and that instructions and rating practice were provided
prior to trials.
Measures
Rate of perceived exertion. As identified above, rate of perceived
exertion was maintained through the application of Borg's (1971)
15-point RPE scale. Specifically, the RPE consists of perceived
workloads ranging on a semantic continuum from very, very light to very,
very hard. The RPE has been demonstrated as a valid and reliable measure
of perceived exertion during exercise (e.g., Borg, 1971; Skinner,
Hustler, Bergsteinova, & Buskirk, 1973).
Distance cycled The distance cycled by participants in each trial
was also recorded on the built-in cycle computer of the Monark 818E
Ergomedic Ergometer in order to register any objective increases in
workload between trials. As identified above, participants were unable
to view the distance traveled for each trial in order to avoid
goal-setting effects.
Affect. Affect was recorded using the 11-point Feeling Scale
developed by Rejeski (1985) in order to monitor any differences in
affective patterns between trials. The scale ranges from -5 (feeling
very good) to +5 (feeling very bad) with semantic anchors at 2-point
intervals. Specifically, participants affect scores were measured at
three-minute intervals (3, 6, 9, & 12 minutes) throughout the
exercise trials. At the end of each trial the total affect score was
obtained by simply summing the four measures of affective states for a
given trial. Hence, participants affect scores could range from a low of
-20 to a high of 20. The scale has been found to be a valid and reliable
measure of affective states during exercise performance (Hardy &
Rejeski, 1989).
Measurement procedures
An initial ten-minute introductory session introduced participants
to the various rating scales involved in this study. Participants were
given instructions regarding the correct use of the scales and given a
brief practice regarding the use of Borg's (1971) RPE scale in
order to maintain a consistent perceived intensity throughout all
trials.
All exercise trials then followed an identical measurement
procedure. Specifically, prior to testing participants were asked to
adjust the cycle seat to a comfortable and appropriate height. They were
then asked to perform a 2-minute warm-up at an RPE level of
'9', after which a further 1-minute warm-up was performed at
an RPE level of '11'. Following the warm-up the RPE scale was
placed within participants' view and they were asked to apply the
headphones.
Dependent upon the condition, the appropriate audiotape (no
music/oudeterous music/ motivational music) was installed into the
cassette player and participants were instructed to pedal for 12 minutes
at a specific RPE of '13'. An RPE of '13' was
selected because it corresponds to a heart rate indicative of
sub-maximal exercise (ACSM, 1998). During each trial, measures of
affective states (assessed by the Feelings Scale; Rejeski, 1985) were
taken at three-minute intervals (3, 6, 9, & 12 minutes), where
participants simply pointed to which number most closely reflected their
current affective state. In addition, at each three-minute interval
participants were required to confirm that they were still cycling at an
RPE of '13.' Completion of the 12-minute trial was followed by
a 1-minute cool-down at an RPE of '9.' After the final trial,
participants were debriefed and their results were disclosed to them.
Results
Descriptive statistics
Descriptive statistics across the three conditions (no music,
oudeterous music, and motivational music) are displayed in Table 1.
These descriptive statistics show means and standard deviations for
distance traveled, and affect across the three conditions.
Repeated measures ANOVAs
Firstly, total distance traveled was analyzed with conditions (no
music vs. oudeterous music vs. motivational music) as a
within-participants factor. The sphericity assumption was not met, so
the Huynh-Feldt correction was applied. The main effect for conditions
was significant (F(1.54, 23.03) = 4.22,p < .05, (d = .22). Post-hoc
comparisons were conducted using the Sidak adjustment for multiple
comparisons. Comparisons revealed that participants in the motivational
music condition (M = 7.11 km, SD = .85), compared to the no music
condition (M = 6.41 km, SD = .92), had traveled significantly further (p
< .05, d = 0.74). No significant differences were identified between
the motivational music and oudeterous music, and between the oudeterous
music and no music conditions. This suggests that motivational music
elicited significantly greater objective work output than no music even
though participants were exercising at the same perceived intensity
level.
Secondly, participants' sum totals of affective scores over
the 12-minute conditions were analyzed in the same way with conditions
(no music vs. oudeterous music vs. motivational music) as a
within-participants factor. This was to identify whether the increases
in objective intensity that was elicited by motivational music would
have any adverse influence on affective patterns. This time the
sphericity assumption was met, so the Huynh-Feldt correction was not
applied. The main effect for conditions was significant (F(2, 30) =
29.72, p < .01, d = .67). Post-hoc comparisons, using the Sidak
adjustment for multiple comparisons, revealed that participants in the
motivational music (M = 12.19, SD = 4.84.) and oudeterous music
conditions (M = 11.19, SD = 6.05), compared to the no music condition (M
= 6.13, SD = 5.99), reported significantly higher levels of affect
during exercise (motivational music =p < .05, d = 1.07; oudeterous
music = p < .05, d = .89). However, no significant differences were
identified between the motivational music and oudeterous music
conditions.
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to examine the effect of motivational
music on objective work output at a standardized perceived exertion and
the subsequent effects on in-exercise affective states. The study was
conducted in the context of the theoretical assumptions proposed by
Karageorghis et al. (1999).
Motivational music elicited a significant increase in distance
traveled when compared to the no music condition. No significant
difference in distance traveled was found between oudeterous music and
no music conditions. This finding provides some support for the
assumptions proposed by Karageorghis et al. (1999) suggesting that where
appropriate considerations are given to the motivational characteristics
of musical selections, positive impacts on perceptions of exertion are
stimulated. Specifically, in this study the application of motivational
music (compared to no music) appeared to engender an enhanced perception
of what constituted cycling at an RPE of '13' in participants.
Additionally, given that participants were more objectively
productive when exposed to motivational music, we aimed to examine
whether there appeared to be any subsequent negative effects on
affective states that might be brought about by increased objective
intensity. Specifically, results suggested that motivational music
stimulated improvements in affective states during exercise trials
compared to the no music condition, indicating that the increased
objective output stimulated by motivational music was not accompanied by
a decrease in affect. In fact, affective states appeared to be
significantly elevated by the application of motivational music. Hence,
overall, results of this study suggest that the application of
motivational music can stimulate individuals to reach higher objective
sub-maximal exercise intensities whilst registering no conscious
awareness of an increase in effort and also experiencing heightened
levels of positive affective states during the experience. Of course,
this finding should be interpreted with caution in terms of attributing
the cause of affective improvements directly to the motivational music.
It may be that any affective changes brought about by motivational music
are a consequence of increased objective intensity and not a direct
consequence of musical application. However, the aim of this study was
not to identify causal links between variables but simply to examine any
alterations in affective states that occurred in conjunction with
musical application.
However, although motivational music increased distance traveled
and positive affect when compared to no music, comparison of
motivational music to oedeterous music revealed no significant
differences in terms of distance traveled and affective states. Further,
both music conditions elicited a significant increase in affective
states compared to no music. In terms of affect, these results suggest
that when sufficient consideration is afforded to factors such as tempo,
familiarity, and cultural appropriateness, music need not necessarily be
"motivational" in order to bring about improvements. These
findings may concur with studies suggesting that music categorized as
simply being "up-beat" produced significant improvements in
affective response (e.g., Lee, 1989; Wales, 1986). However, such
findings are in contrast to the findings of Karageorghis and Terry
(1999) who found that motivational music had the most positive effect on
participants' affective states and that appropriately selected
oudeterous music failed to enhance affect.
A number of possibilities exist that may account for the similarity
between affective responses to both music conditions. Firstly, the
motivational music in this study may not have been sufficiently
motivating. The BMRI allows a highest motivational quotient of 33.33.
Although oedeterous and motivational musical selections had
significantly different quotients, the mean quotient for motivational
music was 20.92. Perhaps the music in this study was not
"motivational" enough to differentiate it from oedeterous
music. Secondly, trials in this study were 12 minutes in length and it
is possible that this duration may not have been long enough to elicit
elevated feelings of boredom or discomfort. For example, motivational
music may have proved to be more beneficial in elevating affective
states in exercise trials of a longer duration.
Before conclusions can be drawn, a number of limitations require
acknowledgement. The authors recognize that the twelve-minute duration
of exercise trials within this study may compromise ecological validity.
However, externally imposed time constraints prevented the
implementation of trials of a longer duration. Despite this,
recommendations presented by the ACSM (1998) have suggested that there
may be significant public health benefits elicited by regular bouts of
intermittent exercise of approximately 10-15 minutes duration. Hence,
the results of this study may have ecological validity in the context of
such guidelines. This study also neglected to determine whether gender
influenced music rating procedures and subsequent responses during
exercise performance. Future research might address these factors.
In conclusion, the results of this study suggest that when
sufficient consideration is given to the selection of musical
accompaniment to exercise, affective benefits may ensue. However,
results also suggest that when compared to no music, the use of
motivational music may be superior to oedeterous music in terms of
eliciting increased exercise intensity. These findings may have
implications within the exercise environment given the importance of
affect and intensity in relation to exercise adherence.
Table 1. Means and standard deviations for distance travelled and
affective states in each experimental condition
Condition M Distance SD M Affect SD
Motivational music 7.11 km .85 12.19 4.84
Oudeterous music 6.87 km 1.05 11.19 6.05
No music 6.41 km .92 6.13 5.99
References
American College of Sports Medicine (1998). ACSM's resource
manual for guidelines for exercise testing and prescription (Third ed.).
London: Williams & Wilkins.
Becker, N., Chambliss, C., Marsh, C., & Montemayor, R. (1995).
Effects of mellow and frenetic music and stimulating and relaxing scents
on walking by seniors. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 80, 411-415.
Boutcher, S.H. & Trenske, M. (1990). The effects of sensory
deprivation and music on perceived exertion and affect during exercise.
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 12, 167-176.
Borg, G. (1971). The perception of physical performance. In R.J.
Shepard (Ed.) Frontiers of fitness (pp. 280-294). Springfield, IL:
Charles C. Thomas.
Brownley, K.A., McMurray, R.G., & Hackney, A.C. (1995). Effects
of music on physiological and affective responses to graded treadmill
exercise in trained and untrained runners. International Journal of
Psychophysiology, 19, 193-201.
Dorney, L., Goh, E.M.K., & Lee, C. (1992). The impact of music
and imagery on physical performance and arousal: Studies of
co-ordination and endurance. Journal of Sport Behaviour, 15, 21-33.
Godin, G. (1994). Social cognitive models. In R.K. Dishman (Ed.)
Advances in exercise adherence (pp. 113-137). Champaign: Illinois, Human
Kinetics.
Godin, G., & Shephard, R.J. (1990). Use of attitude-behaviour
models in exercise promotion. Sports Medicine, 10, 103-121.
Gundlach, R.H. (1935). Factors determining the characteristics of
musical preferences. American Journal of Psychology, 47, 624-643.
Hardy, C.J. & Rejeski, J. (1989). Not what but how one feels:
The measurement of affect during exercise. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology 11, 304-317.
Iwanaga, M. (1995). Relationship between heart rate and preference
for tempo of music. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81, 435-440.
Karageorghis, C. I., & Deeth, I. P. (2002). (In Press). Effects
of asynchronous motivational and oudeterous music on perceptions of
flow. Journal of Sport Sciences.
Karageorghis, C.I., Drew, K.M., & Terry, P.C. (1996). Effects
of pre-test stimulative and sedative music on grip strength. Perceptual
and Motor Skills. 83, 1347-1352.
Karageorghis, C.I. & Terry, P.C. (1997). The psycho-physical
effects of music in sport and exercise: A review. Journal of Sport
Behaviour, 20, 54-68.
Karageorghis, C.I. & Jones, J. (2000). Effects of synchronous
and asynchronous music in cycle ergometry. Journal of Sports Sciences,
18, 16-26.
Karageorghis, C.I., & Lee, J. (2001). Effects of asynchronous
music and imagery on an isometric endurance task. Proceedings of the
2001 World Congress on Sport Psychology.
Karageorghis, C.I. & Terry, P.C. (1999). Affective and
psycho-physical responses to asynchronous music during sub-maximal
treadmill running. Proceedings of the 1999 European College of Sports
Science Congress, Rome, Italy, (p.218).
Karageorghis, C.I., Terry, P.C., & Lane, A.M. (1999).
Development and initial validation of an instrument to assess the
motivational qualities of music in exercise and sport: The Brunel Music
Rating Inventory. Journal of Sports Sciences, 17, 713-724.
LeBlanc, A., Colman, J., McCrary, J., Sherrill, C., & Malin, S.
(1988). Tempo preference of different age music listeners. Journal of
Research in Music Education, 36, 28-45.
Lee, K.P. (1989). The effects of musical tempos on psycho-physical
responding during sub-maximal treadmill running. Masters thesis,
Pennsylvania State University. Microform Publications, University of
Oregon, Eugene, OR (Univer. Microfiche No. UNIV ORE: UO89 205).
Noble, B., & Robertson, R.J. (1996). Perceived Exertion.
Champaign: Illinois, Human Kinetics.
Perkins, K., & Epstein, L. (1988). Methodology in exercise
adherence research. In R.K. Dishman (Ed.) Exercise adherence: Its impact
on public health (pp. 399-417). Champaign: Illinois, Human Kinetics.
Rejeski, W.J. (1985). Perceived exertion: An active or passive
process? Journal of Sport Psychology, 7, 371-378.
Skinner, J.S., Hustler, R., Bergsteinova, V., & Buskirk, E.R.
(1973). The validity and reliability of a rating scale of perceived
exertion. Medicine and Science of Sports. 5, 97-103.
Szabo, A., Small, A., & Leigh, A. (1999). The effects of slow
and fast rhythm classical music on progressive cycling to voluntary
physical exhaustion. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness,
39, 220-225.
Triandis, H.C. (1977). Interpersonal behavior. Monterey, CA:
Brooks/Cole.
Wales, D.N. (1986). The effects of tempo and disposition in music
on perceived exertion, brain waves and mood during aerobic exercise.
Masters thesis, Pennsylvania State University. Microfilm Publications,
University of Oregon, Eugene, OR. (University Microfiche No. UNIV ORE:
UO86 251-252).
Dave Elliott
St. Martin's College
Sam Carr
University College Northampton
Dave Savage
Chester College
Address Correspondece To: Sam Carr, Division of Human Movement and
Sport Sciences, University College Northampton, Park Campus, Boughton
Green Road, Northampton, UK. E-mail: sam.carr@northampton.ac.uk or
samuelcarr@hotmail.com.