Using sex and gender role orientation to predict level of sport fandom.
Warm, Daniel L. ; Waddill, Paula J. ; Dunham, Mardis D. 等
Although sport fandom and spectating are often viewed as male
dominated activities, recent data indicate that the number of women
interested in the pastime may be on the rise (Wann, Melnick, Russell,
& Pease, 2001). In fact, females now comprise at least 40 percent of
National Hockey League, National Football League, and Major Soccer
League fans (Hofacre, 1994; Meyers, 1997; Mihoces, 1998). Consequently,
social scientists have recently directed their attention toward the
relationship between gender and sport fandom. Often, these efforts have
been aimed at documenting the gender gap in sport fandom. Empirical
evidence suggests that males are more likely to be interested and
involved in sport as fans than are females (Anderson & Stone, 1981;
Lieberman, 1991 ; McPherson, 1975; Murrell & Dietz, 1992; Prisuta,
1979; Wann, in press; for a review, see Warm et al., 2001).
A second line of inquiry into gender differences in random has
focused on sport fan socialization and motivation. With respect to sport
fan socialization, McPherson (I 975) found gender differences in the
impact of various socialization agents. Specifically, his work indicated
that peers, family, and school were the most powerful agents for boys
while family, peers, and the community was most influential for girls.
Recently, Warm and his associates (2001) replicated McPherson's
research. In contrast to McPherson, however, these authors found that
schools were the most prominent agent of sport fan socialization for
girls, a finding that may be a consequence of Title IX and the increased
funding given to female youth sports at the scholastic level in the 25
years since McPherson's study. As for motivational differences,
Wann and his colleagues have used the Sport Fan Motivation Scale to
identify several consistent gender differences (Wann, 1995; Warm,
Schrader, & Wilson, 1999). For instance, males are more likely than
females to be motivated by eustress (i.e., positive stress), self-esteem
needs, and the aesthetic qualities of sport. Wann et al. have found that
females are more likely to view sport as an opportunity to spend time
with their family, a finding replicated by Dietz-Uhler, Harrick, End,
and Jacquemotte (2000).
Other investigators have examined differential perceptions of male
and female fans. For instance, Dietz-Uhler, End, Jacquemotte, Bentley,
and Hurlbut (2000) recently asked a group of individuals to consider the
behaviors of a hypothetical male and female fan. The participants
reported large differences in their perceptions as the male fan was
expected to be engaged in significantly more fan behaviors (e.g.,
watching sport on television) than the female fan. Another recent
investigation found that males with strong interest in fandom gave
particularly positive ratings to a female target who was described as
possessing a similarly high interest in sport (Wann, Schinner, &
Keenan, 2001).
Thus, research on the relationship between gender and sport fandom
is beginning to take several interesting and informative directions.
However, because past work had focused solely on the relationship
between anatomical sex and sport fandom, an area that has been neglected
involves the potential relationship between gender role orientation and
fandom. Gender role orientation involves the extent to which individuals
view themselves as masculine, feminine, or androgynous, that is,
possessing nearly equal levels of both masculine and feminine qualities
(Hoffman and Borders, 2001, have suggested using the terms
instrumentality and expressiveness to replace masculine and feminine,
respectively). Although social scientists had yet to investigate the
relationship between gender role orientation and fandom, a number of
investigators had examined the gender role orientations of athletes
(e.g., Martin & Matin, 1995; Roloff & Solomon, 1989; Swain &
Jones, 1991). LeUnes and Nation (I 989) and Gill (1992) each conducted a
review of this literature and concluded that female athletes tended not
to express a feminine sex role orientation. Rather, masculine sex role
orientations were much more common among these persons.
A more recent study of the gender role orientation of athletes was
conducted by Lantz and Schroeder (1999). These authors examined the
relationship between one's identification with the role of athlete
and gender role orientation by asking participants Io complete the
Athletic Identification Measurement Scale (Brewer, Van Raalte, &
Linder, 1993) and the Bem Sex Role Inventory (Bern, 1974). Correlational
analyses indicated that identification with the role of athlete was
positively correlated with masculinity and negatively correlated with
femininity. The current investigation was designed to extend Lantz and
Schroder's work to sport fans. We expected similar relationships
between gender role orientations and identification to emerge with fans,
hypothesizing that there would be a positive correlation between level
of sport fandom and level of masculinity but a negative relationship
between level of fandom and level of femininity. This prediction was
based on research indicating that affective, cognitive, and behavioral
reactions of sport fans often mimic those displayed by the athletes
themselves. For instance, athletes and fans exhibit similar
attributional biases and precompetition anxiety patterns (Wann &
Dolan, 1994; Wann, Schrader, & Adamson, 1998). Thus, it seemed
reasonable to expect fans and athletes to report similar relationships
with respect to gender role orientation.
A second purpose of the present study was to compare the extent to
which anatomical sex and gender role orientation predict level of
fandom. As noted, a number of studies have found that males are more
likely to be heavily involved in sport fandom than females. Should
gender role orientation be related to fandom as expected, it would allow
for a direct test of whether anatomical sex or gender roles best predict
sport fandom (or, perhaps both are uniquely predictive of random). Such
an examination would have important implications for sport marketers as
they attempt to define their target audience. However, because the
relationship between random and gender roles has as yet been untested
and because there were no past data or theory on the relative importance
of gender roles versus anatomical sex in predicting fandom, no direct
hypotheses for these analyses were formulated. Rather, these analyses
were examined within the framework of a research question asking,
"Do anatomical sex and gender role orientation differ in their
ability to predict sport fandom?"
Method
Participants
Participants were 336 (112 male; 2:24 female) university students
earning extra course credit in exchange for participation. They had a
mean age of 26.08 years (SD = 7.82, range = 18 to 60).
Materials and Procedure
Upon entering the testing room and providing their consent to
participate, participants (tested in small groups) were asked to
complete a questionnaire packet containing three sections. The first
section contained demographic items assessing age and anatomical sex.
The second section contained the Sport Fandom Questionnaire (SFQ; Warm,
in press). The SFQ contains five Likert-scale items assessing level of
sport fandom. Response options to the SFQ range from 1 (strongly
disagree) to 8 (strongly agree). A sample item contained in the SFQ
reads "Being a sport fan is very important to me." Higher
ratings are associated with greater fandom. Warm reports strong internal
consistency, test-retest reliability, and criterion validity for the
SFQ.
The third section of the questionnaire packet contained the Bern
Sex Role Inventory (BSRI; Bern, 1974). The BSRI contains 60 Likert-scale
items, 20 each measuring level of femininity, level of masculinity, and
neutral (i.e., filler) items. Participants are asked to indicate the
extent to which the traits are self-descriptive. Sample feminine items
include "flatterable", "feminine" and
"tender". Sample masculine items include
"assertive", "masculine", and "dominant".
Sample neutral items include "helpful", "happy", and
"conventional". Response options range from 1 (never or almost
never true) to 8 (always or almost always true). Although some authors
have challenged the psychometric quality of the BSRI (e.g., Locksley
& Colten, 1979; Pedhazur & Tetenbaum, 1979), many sport
scientists have successfully employed these measures in their sport
research (e.g., Lantz & Schroeder, 1999; Roloff & Solomon, 1989;
Swain & Jones, 1991). Although the BRSI has often been used to
classify participants into various gender role categories (e.g.,
"androgynous" or "undifferentiated") via methods
such as median splits performed on subscale scores, the current
investigation attempted to avoid misclassifications by treating the
femininity and masculinity scales as continuous (i.e., participants
received an overall femininity and masculinity score and were not
classified into specific groups).
After the participants completed their packet, they returned it to
a researcher who handed them a debriefing statement. This statement
disclosed the hypotheses of the study and contained information on
contacting the researcher for a final report of the project. Once the
participants had received the debriefing statement, they were excused
from the testing session. The sessions lasted 20 minutes.
Results
A standard multiple regression analysis was performed on the
dependent variable of sport random (as measured by SFQ score) with sex,
Bern masculinity score, and Bern femininity score as predictor variables
(see Table 1 for means and standard deviations by sex). The BSRI scores
are quite consistent with past research in sport psychology (e.g.,
Colley, Roberts, & Chipps, 1985). The correlation matrix and
descriptive statistics for the variables used in the multiple regression
analysis are shown in Table 2.
Table 3 displays the unstandardized (B) and standardized ([beta])
regression coefficients, the squared semipartial correlations (sr2), and
the R, [R.sup.2], and adjusted [R.sup.2] values. Masculinity ([sr.sup.2]
= .13) contributed the most unique variability to the prediction of
random, followed by sex ([sr.sup.2] = .05). Sex and level of masculinity
in combination contributed another .06 in shared variability. Although
level of femininity was significantly correlated with sport random (r =
-.14), it did not contribute significantly to the regression when sex
and masculinity were also taken into account. Altogether, 24% (23%
adjusted) of the variability in sport random was predicted by knowing
biological sex and degree of masculinity. R for regression was
significant, F(3,332) = 34.67, MSE = 3.71, p < .001. These results
indicate that although biological sex accounts for a significant
proportion of unique variability in fan identification (with men
generally expressing greater random than women), masculinity makes an
even larger contribution. In short, regardless of sex, the greater the
degree of masculine characteristics a man or woman possesses, the
stronger the sport random.
Discussion
It was hypothesized that there would be a positive correlation
between level of sport random and masculinity but a negative
relationship between level of random and femininity. The hypothesis
involving masculinity was supported. However, the hypothesized
relationship between random and femininity was not found. Although there
was a small (and significant) negative correlation between the two
variables, the regression analyses revealed that there was no
independent contribution of level of femininity to degree of sport
random. Thus, the results indicate that sports fans are somewhat similar
to athletes with regard to the impact of gender role orientation (Gill,
1992; LeUnes & Nation, 1989). That is, both athletes and fans tend
to express a masculine gender role orientation. However, while athletes
tend not to express a feminine role, sport fans and nonfans do not
differ significantly on this variable.
Our results indicate that to fully understand the relationship
between demographic variables and involvement in sport as a fan,
individuals must consider both anatomical sex and masculinity. Each was
found to have a significant unique contribution to level of fandom.
Comparisons of the relative contributions of each revealed that
masculinity accounted for greater than twice the variability of
anatomical sex, suggesting that masculinity is the better predictor of
random. Although previous research documenting sex differences in sport
fandom was replicated (e.g., Anderson & Stone, 1981; Lieberman,
1991; Warm, in press), the current data indicate that simply focusing on
effects of anatomical sex leads to an oversimplification of the
relationship between demographic variables and sport fandom.
Similarly, past research on sex differences in sport fan motivation
could be advanced by comparing the variance in specific motives
accounted for by anatomical sex and gender roles. For instance, as noted
above, males tend to be motivated by eustress, self-esteem, and
aesthetics while females tend to be motivated by family needs
(Dietz-Uhler et al., 2000; Warm, 1995; Warm et al., 1999). Based on the
findings reported here that gender role orientation is a better
predictor of general fandom than anatomical sex, one could predict that
a similar pattern of effects would emerge with sport fan motivation.
That is, researchers may find that masculinity is a better predictor of
differences in motives such as eustress and family needs than is
anatomical sex. Further, motives that have not been related to sex in
past research (e.g., using sport fandom as an escape or for economic
gain), may be related to gender role orientation (i.e., masculinity).
The finding that masculinity is a better predictor of fandom than
anatomical sex has important implications for sport marketers and
advertisers. The findings presented here suggest that sport marketers
should target masculine audiences (regardless of the gender composition
of the audience), in addition to audiences that tend to be
male-dominant. For instance, advertisements and commercials appearing
during sporting events that portray scantly-clad women (e.g., the
Swedish Bikini Team ads) may well attract the attention of male fans but
do little, if anything, for masculine female fans. These persons may
need other marketing strategies to attract their attention (of course,
additional research is needed to determine the precise nature of these
strategies). That is, armed with the knowledge that masculinity is an
important predictor of fandom, sport marketers should determine which
advertising strategies are most effective for masculine persons
(regardless of gender). This suggestion also holds true for sport
teams' attempts to market their product to fans. Their strategies
will be more effective and reach a larger audience if they are targeted
at masculine persons of both genders, rather than simply towards males.
In light of the fact that women continue to comprise a larger portion of
the sport fan pool (see Wann et al., 2001), such an all-inclusive
strategy would be particularly advisable.
Table 1
Mean and Standard Deviations for Level of Masculinity and Femininity by
Anatomical Sex.
Males Females
Gender role M SD M SD
Level of:
Masculinity 110.46 14.82 100.94 14.26
Femininity 91.70 14.93 103.58 11.77
Table 2
Correlation Matrix and Descriptive Statistics for Variables Used in
Multiple Regression Analysis.
1 2 3 4 M
1.LSFQ -- -.34 ** .43 ** -.14 * 4.34
2.Sex -- -.30 ** .40 ** --
3.Masculinity -- -.28 ** 104.12
4.Femininity -- 99.62
SD
1.LSFQ 2.2
2.Sex --
3.Masculinity 15.11
4.Femininity 14.05
Notes: SFQ = Sport Fandom Questionnaire. Sex was coded as 1 = male, 2 =
female.
* p<.05, ** p<.01.
Author's Note
Portions of the paper were presented at the annual meeting of the
North American Society for the Sociology of Sport, Colorado Springs, CO,
November, 2000.
References
Anderson, D., & Stone, G. P. (1981). Responses of male and
female metropolitans to the commercialization of professional sport 1960
to 1975. International Review of Sport Sociology, 16(3), 5-20.
Bern, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155-162.
Brewer, B. W., Van Raalte, J. L., & Linder, D E. (1993).
Athletic identity: Hercules' muscles or Achilles' heel?
International Journal of Sport Psychology, 24, 237-254.
Colley, A., Roberts, N., & Chipps, A. (1985). Sex-role
identity, personality, and participation in team and individual sports
by males and females. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 16,
103-112.
Dietz-Uhler, B., End, C., Jacquemotte, L., Bentley, M., &
Hurlbut, V. (2000). Perceptions of male and female sport fans.
International Sports Journal, 4, 88-97.
Dietz-Uhler, B., Harrick, E. A., End, C., & Jacquemotte, L.
(2000). Sex differences in sport fan behavior and reasons for being a
sport fan. Journal of Sport Behavior, 23, 219-231.
Gill, D. L. (1992). Gender and sport behavior, in T. S. Horn (Ed.),
Advances in sport psychology (pp. 143-160). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Hofacre, S. (1994). The women's audience in professional
indoor soccer. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 3, 25-27.
Hoffman, R. M., & Borders, L. D. (200 l). Twenty-five years
after the Bern Sex-Role Inventory: A reassessment and new issues
regarding classification variability. Measurement and Evaluation in
Counseling and Development, 34, 39-55.
Lantz, C. D., & Schroeder, P. J. (1999). Endorsement of
masculine and feminine gender roles: Differences between participation
in and identification with the athletic role. Journal of Sport Behavior,
22, 545-557.
LeUnes, A. D., & Nation, J. R. (1989). Sport psychology: An
introduction. Chicago: Nelson-Hall. Lieberman, S. (1991,
September/October). The popular culture: Sport in America--a look at the
avid sports fan. The Public Perspective: A Roper Center Review of Public
Opinion and Polling, 2(6), 28-29.
Locksley, A., & Colten, M. E. (1979). Psychological androgyny:
A case of mistaken identity? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 37, 1017-1031.
Martin, B. A., & Martin, J. H. (1995). Comparing perceived sex
role orientations of the ideal male and female athlete to the ideal male
and female person. Journal of Sport Behavior, 18, 286-301.
McPherson, B. (1975). Sport consumption and the economics of
consumerism. In D. W. Ball & J. W. Loy (Eds.), Sport and social
order: Contributions to the sociology of sport (pp.243-275). Reading,
MA: Addison-Wesley.
Meyers, B. (1997, August 28). Feminine touches planned but blood
and guts remain. USA Today, pp. A1, A2.
Mihoces, G. (1998, May 7). Women checking in more as NHL fans. USA
Today, pp. C1, C2.
Murrell, A. J., & Dietz, B. (1992). Fan support of sports
teams: The effect of a common group identity. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 14, 28-39.
Pedhazer, E. J., & Tetenbaum, Y. J. (1979). Bern sex role
inventory: A theoretical and methodological critique. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 996-1016.
Prisuta, R. H. (1979). Televised sports and political values.
Journal of Communication, 29, 94-102.
Roloff, M. E., & Solomon, D. H. (1989). Sex typing, sports
interests, and relational harmony. In L. A. Wenner (Ed.), Media, sports,
and society (pp. 290-311). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Swain, A., & Jones, G. (1991). Gender role endorsement and
competitive anxiety. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 22,
50-65.
Wann, D. L. (in press). Preliminary validation of a measure for
assessing identification as a sport fan: The Sport Fandom Questionnaire.
International Journal of Sport Management.
Wann, D. L. (1995). Preliminary validation of the Sport Fan
Motivation Scale. Journal of Sport & Social Issues, 19, 377-396.
Wann, D. L., & Dolan, T. J. (1994). Attributions of highly
identified sports spectators. Journal of Social Psychology, 134,
783-792.
Wann, D. L., Melnick, M. J., Russell, G. W., & Pease, D. G.
(2001). Sport fans: The psychology and social impact of spectators. New
York: Routledge Press.
Wann, D. L., Schinner, J., & Keenan, B. L. (2001). Males'
impressions of female fans and nonfans: There really is "Something
About Mary". North American Journal of Psychology, 3, 183-192.
Wann, D. L., Schrader, M. P., & Adamson, D. R. (1998). The
cognitive and somatic anxiety of sport spectators. Journal of Sport
Behavior, 21, 322-337.
Wann, D. L., Schrader, M. P., & Wilson, A. M. (1999). Sport fan
motivation: Questionnaire validation, comparisons by sport, and
relationship to athletic motivation. Journal of Sport Behavior, 22,
114-139.
Address Correspondence To: Daniel L. Warm, Department of
Psychology, Murray State University, Murray, KY 42071 or to
dan.wann@murraystate.edu