A typology of marathon runners based on cluster analysis of motivations.
Ogles, Benjamin M. ; Masters, Kevin S.
It is not inherently obvious why anyone engages in marathon
running, yet each year in the United States thousands participate in
these events. An analysis of the costs of marathon participation reveals
many factors that seem to pose serious obstacles to the completion of a
marathon. First, training for a marathon is a significant undertaking.
Months of day in and day out running with sometimes lengthy individual
training sessions are required to prepare for a marathon. Numerous hours
and miles of training are necessary, especially if the participant
anticipates running the entire distance. This level of training is
clearly beyond what is necessary to acquire the basic health benefits of
regular exercise (Blair et al., 1996; Blair et al., 1995; Blair et al.,
1989; "Physical Activity", 1995) and necessitates that work,
meal, family, and social schedules be organized to accommodate the
regimen. Additionally, other recreational activities are missed and time
with family and friends is likely to be reduced. All of this is
particularly significant when considering the difficulty that many,
perhaps most, people have with maintaining minimal aerobic exercise routines.
Though the monetary costs of marathon running are not onerously high compared to other activities, there are numerous expenses including
shoes, running attire, race entry fees, and often travel. The physical
and psychological costs are also noteworthy. Runners may, particularly
in the initial stages, experience fatigue following training runs. This
may interfere with work or other important activities. Additionally,
while logging numerous miles, runners increase the probability of
sustaining an injury and thus enduring the subsequent complicating sequelae (e.g., medical bills, pain, time and energy for rehabilitation,
etc.). Psychologically, the hours of training may be boring, lonely, and
monotonous. Even if the runner survives the training ordeal in good
psychological and physical condition, there is no guarantee of a
satisfactory performance in the marathon itself. Nervousness about the
race, a disagreeable meal, lack of sleep, illness, travel to a distant
city, an injury during the marathon, or simply a p oor performance may
all contribute to a negative experience. In fact, few human activities
have potential costs of this magnitude with such uncertain outcomes.
Yet, many people voluntarily engage in marathon running on a regular
basis! This paradox generates an interesting and provocative
psychological research question-What motivates individuals to endure the
apparent punishment of training for and participation in a marathon?
Several previous investigators (e.g., Carmack & Martens, 1979;
Clough, Shepherd, & Maughan, 1989; Curtis & McTeer, 1981; Gill
et al., 1996; Johnsgard, 1985a, 1985b; Masters & Lambert, 1989;
Ogles & Masters, 2000; Summers, Machin, & Sargent, 1983; Summers
et al., 1982; Thornton & Scott, 1995; Vitulli & DePace, 1992;
Ziegler, 1991) have studied this topic as it pertains to running in
general and marathon running in particular. Several motivating reasons
for marathon running have been identified such as mood control,
self-concept enhancement, fitness/health, challenge, psychological
well-being, competition, weight control, social status, etc. In short, a
variety of potential motivational explanations have been generated for
marathon runners.
Given this rather impressive list of possible reasons for marathon
running, it seems unlikely that marathon runners are a motivationally
homogeneous group. Instead, individual marathon runners may be
characterized by different motives for running. For example, some
runners may be driven by a need to compete against other participants
whereas other runners are driven to improve their own performance (i.e.,
goal orientation). Similarly, health concerns may be of primary
importance to some while social considerations could be preeminent for
others. In addition, marathon runners may have multiple reasons for
running. As a result, patterns or profiles of motivations may be
examined to identify differences among runners using a combination of
motivational variables.
If marathon runners can be differentiated on the basis of their
motives for running, they may also differ in their training patterns or
on demographic variables. For instance, certain clusters of motivational
factors may characterize older as opposed to younger runners, men versus
women, those who train harder, etc. However, many of the previous
studies have methodological difficulties that prevent more sophisticated
multivariate analysis or the identification of subtypes of runners using
profiles. For example, several studies did not use samples of marathon
runners. Others relied upon responses to open-ended questions about
reasons for running. Finally, some studies utilized questionnaires of
unknown psychometric quality. As a result, no study has provided a
statistically based clustering of the motives for marathon running that
empirically determines groups or types of marathon runners.
In part to solve this problem, Masters, Ogles, and Jolton (1993)
developed the Motivations of Marathoners Scales (MOMS). The test was
based on the earlier research findings mentioned above and includes nine
scales grouped within four broad categories of motives for participating
in a marathon. The items and scales were rationally developed and then
supported by empirical analyses including assessments of retest reliability and internal consistency along with demonstrations of factor
and construct validity (Masters & Ogles, 1995; Masters, Ogles, &
Jolton, 1993; Ogles, Masters, & Richardson, 1995). Since the MOMS is
comprehensive in scope, of adequate psychometric quality, and yields
nine different motivational scale scores, it is legitimate to
incorporate it into studies that utilize multivariate analyses, such as
cluster analysis. These techniques may then be able to reveal types of
marathon runners distinguishable by their motivation profiles.
This study was designed to statistically group marathon runners
based on their MOMS profiles. It was hypothesized that marathon runners
are not a homogeneous group in terms of their motives for running and
that definable and interpretable subgroups could be identified using
cluster analysis. Further, once definable clusters were identified they
would be com
Pared on the basis of demographic and training variables to
determine how cluster membership relates with these characteristics.
Method
Participants
Marathon runners participating in one of six Midwestern marathons
were recruited during marathon pre-race registration. While registering
they were asked to take home and complete a training and demographic
questionnaire along with the MOMS and to return both in the mail. The
final sample consisted of 1242 men and 277 women for a total of 1519
participants. They ranged in age from 15 to 79 years and were
predominately Caucasian (95%). Their average running career was 9.37
years (SD6.09) and ranged from 0 to 35 years. Best finishing times in
previous marathons averaged 3 hours and 33 minutes (SD=35.32 minutes)
and ranged from 2 hours 15 minutes to 7 hours and 4 minutes. Prior to
the marathon, runners were training an average of 44.11 miles per week
(SD= 17.26). About 20% of the runners were participating in their first
marathon.
Instruments
Demographic and training questionnaire. The demographic and
training questionnaire inquired regarding age; gender; miles, hours and
days of training per week; years of running; percent of time running
alone; number of previous marathons; and best and average finish times
in previous marathons.
Motivations of Marathoners Scales (MOMS; Masters, Ogles, &
Jolton, 1993). The MOMS consists of 56 items that are rated using a I to
7 scale as to the degree to which the runner considers them a reason for
training and running a marathon. The items are further divided into nine
internally consistent scales representing four broad categories of
motives. The broad categories, scales, and example items are presented
in Table I. As indicated above, adequate evidence for the internal
consistency (alphas range from .80 to .93), test-retest reliability
(r's range from .71 to .90), factorial and construct validity of
the scales has been presented elsewhere (Masters & Ogles, 1995;
Masters, Ogles, & Jolton, 1993; Ogles, Masters, & Richardson,
1995).
Analysis
To discover natural groupings of runners based on their endorsement
of motives for running, a cluster analysis was performed using the nine
scales of the MOMS. As a method of assessing the robustness of the
cluster solution, the sample was randomly divided into halves and each
half was analyzed separately. The entire sample was then cluster
analyzed followed by statistical comparisons of the solutions for the
half samples with the whole sample solution. The cluster groups were
described graphically based on the average rating on each of the scales.
Then a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted using
the cluster solution as the independent variable (cluster group
membership) and the nine motives as the dependent variables to describe
motivational differences among the clusters. Additional evidence of
cluster validity was obtained by examining differences in demographic
and training variables that coincide with the motivational profiles for
each of the clusters.
Results
Cluster Solutions
Cluster analysis (Ward's method), for each half and the entire
sample resulted in similar solutions. When choosing solutions based on
the increase in error sum of squares and the interpretability of the
solutions, all three samples (the two halves and whole sample) yielded
five cluster solutions with the clusters labeled Running Enthusiasts,
Lifestyle Managers, Personal Goal Achievers, Personal Accomplishers, and
Competitive Achievers (see Figure I). A chi-square analysis comparing
the cluster classification of individuals using the half sample
solutions with the whole sample solution was significant, [X.sup.2] (16,
N= 1479) 1999.38, p<.0001. Two-thirds of the subjects were assigned
to the same cluster group in both the half sample and whole sample
solutions. Further, the Kappa for the frequency table was .57 which is
also statistically significant and demonstrates good agreement between
classifications based on the half and whole sample solutions. Therefore,
subsequent analyses are based on the whole sample solution.
Motivational Differences Among Clusters
To verify differences among the clusters on the nine MOMS scales
and to provide descriptive data concerning the clusters, a MANOVA with
cluster group membership as the independent variable and the nine MOMS
scales as the dependent variables was conducted. As expected, the
MANOVA, approximate F(36, 5858) = 98.95, p<.001, and all of the
univariate tests were significant (ps<.001; see Table 2). Post hoc tests to identify specific differences among cluster groups for each of
the nine MOMS scales were then conducted. On the Health Orientation,
Weight Concern, and Recognition scales, all five cluster groups were
significantly different from each of the remaining cluster groups - that
is, every pairwise comparison was significant. On the remaining scales
only those clusters that were not significantly different from one
another will be described. Lifestyle Managers and Personal Accomplishers
did not differ on the Affiliation scale. Lifestyle Managers and
Competitive Achievers did not differ on the Life Meaning or Se lf-esteem
scales. Lifestyle Managers and Running Enthusiasts did not differ on the
Psychological Coping scale. Personal Accomplishers did not differ from
Lifestyle Managers, and Competitive Achievers did not differ from
Running Enthusiasts on the Personal Goal Achievement scale. Finally,
Lifestyle Managers, Personal Goal Achievers, and Personal Accomplishers
did not differ from one another on the Competition scale.
To aid in the interpretation of the cluster profiles, all scales
that had an average at or above the midpoint (4.0) were considered
primary motives endorsed by the cluster group. These scales are found in
Table 3 under the "motivational characteristics" heading.
Table 3 also describes the clusters based on the training and
demographic analyses to be presented below.
Demographic and Training Differences Among Clusters
In order to further assess the validity of the five cluster groups,
we conducted two chi-square analyses (gender by cluster group membership
and endorsement of training twice in one day by cluster group
membership) to consider differences among the clusters in terms of
gender and training intensity. In addition, seven one-way analyses of
variance (followed by post hoc Scheffe tests when appropriate) using the
cluster groups as the independent variable and demographic and training
characteristics as the dependent variables were conducted. These
analyses were conducted to ascertain differences among cluster groups
using the following variables: age, years running, miles and days per
week in training, percent of time training alone, average marathon
completion time, and number of previous marathons.
Both the gender by cluster group and the training twice in one day
by cluster group chi-square analyses were significant, [X.sup.2] (4, N =
1476) = 25.59, p<.0001; [X.sup.2] (4, N = 680) = 22.49, p<.0001.
Examination of the 2 [X.sup.2] contingency tables revealed an
interesting pattern of differences among the cluster groups in terms of
proportion of men/women in the groups and proportion of individuals who
sometimes train twice in one day in the groups. The gender by cluster
group contingency tables revealed that Running Enthusiasts and Lifestyle
Managers did not differ in terms of the proportion of men and women.
Similarly, there were no differences in the proportion of men (women)
among Personal goal Achievers, Personal Accomplishers, and Competitive
Achievers. Running Enthusiasts and Lifestyle Managers, however, were
significantly different from Personal goal Achievers, Personal
Accomplishers, and Competitive Achievers in every comparison. Men were
more likely to be Personal goal Achievers, Personal Accom plishers, or
Competitive Achievers while women were disproportionately represented in
Running Enthusiasts and Lifestyle Managers.
An examination of all possible 2 X 2 contingency tables for the
training twice in one day by cluster group analysis indicated that
Competitive Achievers were more likely to train twice in one day than
any of the other cluster groups. In addition, Lifestyle Managers were
less likely to train twice in one day than Running Enthusiasts and
Competitive Achievers. There were no other differences among the groups.
Means and standard deviations for the seven one-way analyses are
presented in Table 4. Only number of years running was not significantly
different among the groups. Omnibus F-tests were significant for the
remaining six dependent variables suggesting differences among the
motivational clusters in terms of their age, F(4, 1472) = 10.04,
p<.001; average marathon completion time, F(4, 1051) 19.62,
p<.001; number of days training, F(4, 666) = 7.01, p<.00l; miles
per week of training, F(4, 1460) = 9.55, p<.001; percent of time
training alone, F(4, 1470) = 12.29, p<.001; and number of previous
marathons, F(4, 1432) = 5.34, p<.001. Post-hoc tests at the p<.05
level indicated that competitive Achievers were significantly younger
than
Running Enthusiasts, Lifestyle Managers, and Personal
Accomplishers. In addition, Personal Goal Achievers were significantly
younger than Running Enthusiasts. In short, Competitive Achievers and
Personal Goal Achievers tended to be younger runners whereas the oldest
runners were Running Enthusiasts. In addition to being younger,
Competitive Achievers also had faster finishing times in previous
marathons. Post-hoc tests indicated that Competitive Achievers were
significantly faster than Running Enthusiasts, Lifestyle Managers, and
Personal Accomplishers. On the other hand, Lifestyle Managers were
significantly slower in previous marathon times than all other groups.
Competitive Achievers also ran more days per week than Lifestyle
Managers and Personal Accomplishers. Lifestyle Managers trained
significantly fewer miles per week than the remaining groups. They were
also more likely to train alone. Their percentage of time training alone
was significantly higher than Running Enthusiasts, Personal
Accomplishers, a nd Competitive Achievers. In contrast, Running
Enthusiasts trained more frequently with other runners when compared to
Lifestyle Managers, Personal Goal Achievers, and Personal Accomplishers.
Running Enthusiasts had participated in more marathons than Lifestyle
Managers, Personal Goal Achievers, and Personal Accomplishers. Finally,
there were no differences among the groups in terms of the average
number of years training.
Discussion
Marathon runners are indeed a heterogeneous group when considering
their motives for running a marathon. Their motives are sufficiently
diverse that the runners can be statistically clustered based on their
self-report motives for marathon running. Cluster analysis of
motivational profiles for this large sample of runners produced five
distinct groups or clusters of marathon runners. Although five clusters
do not account for all the variation among motivational profiles, it is
clear that five broad categories of runners provide sufficient evidence
that marathon runners are a heterogeneous group. These five groups are
not only distinguishable by their pattern of endorsement of motives for
running but also by training and demographic variables, in addition, the
duplication of findings across random samples lends preliminary evidence
for the reliability of the cluster findings. A brief description of each
cluster will more clearly demonstrate the various types of marathon
runners groups by motives.
Cluster #1 was labeled Running Enthusiasts. This group accounted
for 16% of the sample and endorsed all nine of the MOMS scales as
reasons for running. Perhaps this group simply responded with an
affirmative response set, the so-called "yea sayers."
Alternatively, the data also support an interpretation indicating that
these runners are veteran disproportionately female, marathoners who may
have come to value many aspects of the running experience. They ranked
health orientation, self-esteem, and personal goal achievement as their
highest motives. However, they were also the only cluster to rate
affiliation and recognition motives above the midpoint. Additionally,
their training profile shows that they were more likely to run with
other runners, It may be that it is their general endorsement of running
motives, including the social aspect, that has enabled them to continue
running these long distances over the years. On the other hand, there
could be a cohort effect showing that older, disproportionately fem ale,
runners are positively influenced by many reasons for marathon running.
At any rate, they appear to find many aspects of the running experience
to be reinforcing.
Cluster #2 we labeled Lifestyle Managers and they accounted for 25%
of the sample. They endorsed health orientation, self-esteem, weight
concern, psychological coping, personal goal achievement, and life
meaning motives. They generally appear to be motivated by an interest in
improving their physical and psychological well-being. This pattern
suggests that, at least to some extent, running for these individuals
may also be a method of handling negative emotions. At the same time,
they are not motivated by social and competitive reasons for running. As
might be expected based on their motive profile, this group runs slower
and trains less intensively. They are more likely to train alone and to
be female.
Cluster #3 we labeled the Personal Goal Achievers and they
accounted for 12% of the sample. These runners are primarily influenced
by improving their running speed and performing up to the best of their
capabilities. It is important to note, however, that their pursuit of
goals is ipsative, i.e., they are not motivated by competition with
other runners, only with themselves. They tended to be younger and
faster males who trained more miles per week than other groups.
Cluster #4 we labeled the Personal Accomplishers arid they
accounted for 28% of the sample. These runners endorsed health
orientation, personal achievement, and self-esteem as primary motives.
They are similar to Lifestyle Managers except that they do not endorse
weight concern, life meaning, and psychological coping motives. This
group is more concerned with accomplishment, perhaps best thought of in
a general sense as positive participation. Whereas Lifestyle managers
seems to have a strong element of managing negative aspects such as
weight or troubling emotions, this is not found with the Personal
Accomplishers. Their training and performance profiles tended to be very
average, and they accounted for the largest percentage of runners in the
sample.
Finally, Cluster #5 was labeled the Competitive Achievers and it
accounted for 17% of the sample. These runners primarily endorsed
personal goal achievement, self-esteem, health orientation, life meaning
and competition, as their motives. They tended to be younger males who
ran faster and trained more days per week. They were also more likely to
train twice in one day. In fact, 60% of the runners in this group
indicated that they sometimes ran twice a day. They differ from Personal
Goal Achievers in several ways, but perhaps most significantly is their
relative endorsement of competition with others as a motive.
It is interesting that Running Enthusiasts and Lifestyle Managers
tended to be disproportionately female (ca. 25% female - when the
proportion of women in the sample was 18%) while Personal Goal
Achievers, Personal Accomplishers, and Competitive Achievers were
disproportionately male (ca. 87% male and 13% female). In both cases,
these findings are consistent with previous research. For example, the
Running Enthusiasts and Lifestyle Managers endorsed a wide variety of
motives for running. Several previous studies have suggested that women
perceive more benefits from marathon running than men perceive (e.g.,
Ogles, Masters, & Richardson, 1995). Benefits cited in the
literature include improvements in self-esteem, greater opportunities to
meet people and improved social lives, relief from depression, feeling
less shy, better weight control, increased commitment to running, more
relaxation and energy, and greater physical attractiveness (Clough,
Shepherd, & Maughan, 1989; Curtis & McTeer, 1981; Gill, et al.,
1996 ; Masters & Lambert, 1989; Ogles, Masters, & Richardson,
1995; Porter, 1985; Summers, Machin, & Sargent, 1983; Ziegler,
1991).
One difference between the Running Enthusiasts and the Lifestyle
Managers is the degree to which social reinforcers were characteristic
of their motivational profile. Specifically, the Running Enthusiasts
were more strongly influenced by affiliation and recognition motives
than were the Lifestyle Managers and, correspondingly, were more likely
to train with someone than were the Lifestyle Managers. This finding
demonstrates that while for some marathon runners the social aspects of
running are important, such is certainly not the case for all of them.
Consequently, characterizations of social motivations, particularly for
female marathon runners, must be made cautiously. Certainly statements
indicating that they are motivated by social reasons would be overly
simplistic.
The Personal Goal Achievers, Personal Accomplishers, and
Competitive Achievers tended to be male. These clusters differ in
several ways but have in common at least a mild endorsement of either
goal achievement (i.e., competition with oneself) or competition with
others. Many researchers have noted (Gill, 1986; Gill & Deeter,
1988; Gill et al., 1996; Ziegler, 1991) that males tend to score higher
on measures of competitiveness than do females. Thus, it is not
surprising that motivational clusters containing some type of
competition include a disproportionate representation of males.
Overall, however, the cluster groups were similar in their relative
under emphasis of competitive and social motives when compared to
health, personal achievement, and self-esteem reasons. Although the rank
order of the latter three reasons differed among clusters, they were
consistently ranked as the most important. The psychological motives
were likewise viewed as slightly more important than the competitive and
social motives for all five clusters. This suggests that although
marathon events are large, ostensibly competitive, social gatherings,
most of the "competitors" are motivated to participate for
personal life enhancing reasons that are not externally competitive, or
even social, in nature. Further supporting this is the finding that 53%
ofthe sample was classified as either Lifestyle Managers or Personal
Accomplishers. Both groups are clearly more motivated by personal
accomplishment or psychological factors than competing with others,
achieving recognition, or finding social affiliation.
As more is learned about the motivation of marathon runners we come
to a better understanding of why it is that so many individuals endure
the costs in order to participate in these events. Hopefully this
knowledge will be useful in encouraging participation in all types of
exercise, not just marathon running. It is recommended that researchers
continue investigating the motivation of endurance exercisers by
studying specific samples in specific events. For example, knowledge of
the differences between elite versus non-elite runners is important.
Similarly, are the motivational patterns of marathon runners similar to
those who participate in triathlons or longer running or cycling races?
Longitudinal studies of motivations, while they are difficult to
conduct, will help us to learn more about what, if any, changes there
are in motivational patterns across the exercise/life cycle. This
finding could have significant practical implications. Finally, more
research investigating the motivational types displayed in this study is
also important. For example, we hypothesize that the lifestyle Managers
may experience more psychological and emotional difficulty than do the
Personal Accomplishers. The degree to which running and training is used
to cope with negative emotions may also differ among the groups. Further
investigation of how the motivational profile types compare on standard
psychological assessments would add to our understanding of the
psychological aspects of marathon running and marathon runners.
[FIGURE OMITTED]
Table 1
General categories, Scales and Sample Items for the Motivations of
Marathoners Scales.
General Category
Scale Sample Items
I. Physical Health Motives
Health Orientation to improve my health, to prolong my
life, to become more physically
fit, to reduce my chance of having
a heart attack, to stay in physical
condition
Weight Concern to look leaners, to help control my
weight, to reduce my weight
II. Social Motives
Affiliation to socialize with other runners, to
meet people, to visit with friends,
to share a group identity with
runners
Recognition to earn respect of peers, people
look up to me, brings me
regnition, to make my family or
friends proud of me
III. Achievement Motive
Competition to compete with others, to see how
high I can place, to get a faster
time than my friends, to beat
someone I've never beaten before
Personal Goal Achievement to improve my running speed, to
complete with myself, to push
myself, to beat a certain time,
to run faster
IV. Psychological Motives
Psychological Coping to become less anxious, to distract
myself from daily worries, to
improve my mood, to concentrate on
my thoughts, to solve problems
Self-Esteem to improve my self-esteem, to feel
more confident, to feel proud of
myself, to feel a sense of
achievement, to feel mentally in
control of my body
Life Meaning to make my life more purposeful,
to make myself feel whoel, to feel
a sense of belonging with nature,
to feel at peace with the world
Table 2
Means and Standard Deviations for the Nine MOMS Scales by Cluster Group
Membership
Cluster 1 Cluster 2
Running Lifestyle
Enthusiasts Managers
n = 238 n = 380
MOMS' Scale X SD X SD
Competition 4.46 1.38 2.19 0.95
Personal Goal
Achievement 5.80 0.76 4.64 1.16
Psychological
Coping 4.85 1.19 4.66 1.08
Self-esteem 5.84 0.79 5.29 0.89
Life Meaning 4.82 1.15 4.14 1.07
Health
Orientation 6.10 0.68 5.56 1.05
Weight
Concern 5.37 0.97 5.00 1.26
Recognition 4.37 1.20 2.87 1.40
Affiliation 4.69 1.14 2.46 1.02
Cluster 3 Cluster 4
Goal Personal
Achievers Accomplishers
n = 187 n = 418
MOMS' Scale X SD X SD
Competition 2.08 1.23 2.31 1.17
Personal Goal
Achievement 3.96 1.44 4.46 1.24
Psychological
Coping 1.50 0.58 2.72 0.92
Self-esteem 3.26 1.28 4.04 1.12
Life Meaning 1.60 0.69 2.34 0.90
Health
Orientation 2.70 1.13 4.91 1.20
Weight
Concern 1.86 0.90 3.82 1.63
Recognition 2.02 1.07 2.39 1.15
Affiliation 2.13 0.97 2.50 1.20
Cluster 5
Competitive
Achievers
n = 256
MOMS' Scale X SD
Competition 3.99 1.36
Personal Goal
Achievement 5.53 0.89
Psychological
Coping 3.71 1.28
Self-esteem 5.40 0.82
Life Meaning 4.03 1.22
Health
Orientation 4.55 1.13
Weight
Concern 2.70 1.03
Recognition 3.91 1.19
Affiliation 3.92 1.15
Table 3
Names and Characteristics for the Cluesters Groups.
Cluster Name Motivation
Characteristics
1. Running Endorse all 9 motives
Enthusiasts
(16%)
2. Lifestyle Personal Goal Achievement;
Managers Self-esteem; Health
(25%) Orientation; Psych. Coping;
Weight Concern;
Life Meaning
3. Personal Goal Personal Goal Achievement
Achievers
(12%)
4. Personal personal Goal Achievement;
Accomplishers Self-esteem; Health
(28%) Orientation
5. Competitive Personal Goal Achievement;
Achievers Self-esteem; Health
(17%) Orientation; Competition;
Life Meaning
Cluster Name Training and Demographic
Characteristic
1. Running Older, more marathon
Enthusiasts participation, more likely to
(16%) run with other runners and
disproportionately female
2. Lifestyle More likely to train alone,
Managers run slower marathons, trains
(25%) fewer miles and days, less
day, disproportionately female
3. Personal Goal Somewhat younger, faster
Achievers times, training more miles,
(12%) disproportionately male
4. Personal Rated near the middle on
Accomplishers most variables ("average"),
(28%) disproportionately male
5. Competitive Younger, faster marathon
Achievers times, train more days, more
(17%) likely to train twice in one
day, disproportionately male
Table 4
Means And Standard Deviations For One Demographic And Six Training
Variables By Cluster Group Membership.
Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3
Running Lifestyle Personal Goal
Enthusiasts Managers Achievers
n = 238 n = 380 n = 187
Variable X (SD) X (SD) X (SD)
Age 40.9 (10.89) 38.8 (9.06) 37.3 (8.31)
Average
finish time 220.5 (33.37) 235.6 (33.77) 219.2 (37.06)
Days run
per week 5.4 (1.10) 5.0 (1.20) 5.4 (1.13)
Miles
per week 44.6 (15.68) 40.2 (14.88) 47.3 (17.15)
% training
alone 70.1 (30.73) 85.4 (23.37) 78.6 (26.30)
Number of
Marathons 9.9 (25.03) 5.1 (15.37) 4.9 (7.25)
Years running 9.3 (7.20) 9.2 (6.15) 9.8 (6.23)
Cluster 4 Cluster 5
Personal Competitive
Accomplishers Achievers
n = 418 n = 256
Variable X (SD) X (SD)
Age 38.9 (9.43) 35.8 (9.47)
Average
finish time 225.18 (35.47) 208.12 (29.84)
Days run
per week 5.0 (.31) 5.6 (1.23)
Miles
per week 44.1 (17.94) 47.7 (18.23)
% training
alone 78.3 (28.01) 76.5 (27.21)
Number of
Marathons 5.1 (10.20) 7.8 (15.32)
Years running 8.5 (5.38) 9.7 (5.26)
References
Blair, S. N., Kampert, J. B., Kohl, H. W. 3rd., Barlow, C. E.,
Macera, C. A., Paffenbarger, R. S. Jr., & Gibbons, L. W. (1996).
Influences of cardiorespiratory fitness and other precursors on
cardiovascular disease and all-cause mortality in men and women. Journal
of the American Medical Association, 276, 205-210.
Blair, S. N., Kohl, H. W. 3rd., Barlow, C. E., Paffenbarger, R. S.
Jr., Gibbons, L. W., & Macera, C. A. (1995). Changes in physical
fitness and all-cause mortality. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 273, 1093-1098.
Blair, S. N., Kohl, H. W. 3rd., Paffenbarger, R. S. Jr., Clark, D.
G., Cooper, K. H., & Gibbons, L. W. (1989). Physical fitness and
all-cause mortality: A prospective study of health men and women.
Journal of the American Medical Association, 262, 2395-2401.
Carmack, M. A., & Martens, R. (1979). Measuring commitment to
running: A survey of runners' attitudes and mental states. Journal
of Sport Psychology, 1, 25-42.
Clough, P. J., Shepherd, J., & Maughan, R. (1989). Motives for
participation in recreational running. Journal of Leisure Research, 21,
297-309.
Curtis, J., & McTeer, W. (1981, January). The motivation for
running. Canadian Runner, 18-19.
Gill, D. L. (1986). Competitiveness among females and males in
physical activity classes. Sex Roles, 15, 233-247.
Gill, D. L.,& Deeter, T. E.(1988). Development of the Sport
Orientation Questionnaire. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport,
59, 191-202.
Gill, D. L., Williams, L., Dowd, D. A., Beaudoin, C. M. (1996).
Competitive orientations and motives of adult sport and exercise
participants. Journal of Sport Behavior, 19, 307-318.
Johnsgard, K. (1985a). The motivation of the long distance runner:
I. Journal of Sports Medicine, 25, 135-139.
Johnsgard, K. (1985b). The motivation of the long distance runner:
II. Journal of Sports Medicine, 25, 140-143.
Masters, K. S., & Lambert, M. J. (1989). The relations between
cognitive coping strategies, reasons for running, injury, and
performance of marathon runners. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology 11, 161-170.
Masters, K. S., & Ogles, B. M. (1995). An investigation of the
different motivations of marathon runners with varying degrees of
experience. Journal of Sport Behavior, 18, 69-79.
Masters, K. S., Ogles, B. M., & Jolton, J. A. (1993). The
development of an instrument to measure motivation for marathon running:
The Motivations of Marathoners Scales (MOMS) Research Quarterly for
Exercise and Sport, 64, 134-143.
Ogles, B. M., & Masters, K. S. (2000). Older versus younger
adult male marathon runners: Participative motives and training habits.
Journal of Sport Behavior, 23(3), 1-14.
Ogles, B. M., Masters, K. S., & Richardson, S. A. (1995).
Obligatory running and gender An analysis of participative motives and
training habits. International Journal of Sport Psychology 26, 233-248.
Physical Activity and Cardiovascular Health. NIH Consensus
Statement. (1995). Bethesda MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services.
Porter, K. (1985). Psychological characteristics of the average
female runner. The Physician and Sportsmedicine, 13, 171-175.
Summers, J.J., Machin, V. J., & Sargent, G. I. (1983).
Psychosocial factors related to marathon running. Journal of Sport
Psychology, 5,314-331.
Summers, J. J., Sargent, G. I., Levely, A. J., & Murray, K. D.
(1982). Middle-aged, non elite marathon runners: A profile. Perceptual
and Motor Skills, 54,963-969.
Thornton, E. W., & Scott, S. E. (1995). Motivation in the
committed runner: Correlations between self-report scales and behaviour.
Health Promotion International, 10,177-184.
Vitulli, W. F., & DePace, A. N. (1992). Manifest reasons for
jogging and for not jogging. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75,111-114.
Ziegler, S. G. (1991). Perceived benefits of marathon running in
males and females. Sex Roles, 25,119-127.
Author Note
Order of authorship is random, both authors contributed equally to
this project. An earlier version of this article was presented at the
American Psychological Association Conference, Chicago, August 1998.
Address Correspondence To: Benjamin M. Ogles, Ph.D., Department of
Psychology, 200 Porter Hall, Ohio University, Athens, OH 45701. E-mail:
ogles@ohio.edu.