An examination of perfectionism and self-esteem in intercollegiate athletes.
Gotwals, John K. ; Dunn, John G.H. ; Wayment, Heidi A. 等
Perfectionism has been shown to have adaptive or maladaptive influences upon cognition, affect, and behavior (e.g., Davis, 1997;
Frost, Heimberg, Holt, Mattia, & Neubauer, 1993; Hamachek, 1978;
Terry-Short, Owens, Slade, & Dewey, 1995). In the domain of sport
psychology, the construct of perfectionism is extremely relevant because
it appears to have the potential to either help or hinder athletes in
their pursuit of achievement goals (Dunn, Causgrove Dunn, &
Syrotuik, in press). However, only a handful of researchers have
examined perfectionism in the context of sport (i.e., Coen & Ogles,
1993; Dunn et al., in press; Frost & Henderson, 1991; Gould, Tuffey,
Udry, & Loehr, 1997; Gould, Udry, Tuffey, & Loehr, 1996; Hall,
Kerr, & Matthews, 1998).
Based on professional intuition gained through years of experience
working in the field of sport psychology, Hardy, Jones and Gould (1996)
proposed that "many of the most effective world class athletes are
perfectionist in their orientations" (p. 243). However, in a series
of publications on former elite figure skaters, Scanlan, Stein, and
Ravizza (1989, 1991) noted that skaters who strove for perfection tended
to enjoy their competitive experiences, yet skaters who felt the need to
skate flawlessly often experienced stress. Similarly, Zinsser, Bunker,
and Williams (2001) concluded that there "is always value in
striving for perfection [in sport], but nothing is gained by demanding
perfection" (p. 302). Given the potentially adaptive and
maladaptive functions of perfectionism, understanding and identifying
correlates of perfectionism in sport becomes an important research
endeavor (Dunn et al., in press).
Hamachek (1978) argued that there are two types of perfectionism:
neurotic perfectionism (henceforth labeled maladaptive perfectionism)
and normal perfectionism (henceforth labeled adaptive perfectionism).
Maladaptive perfectionists have a tendency to set excessively high
personal standards for performance and are preoccupied with and overly
critical of their mistakes (Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate,
1990). Blatt (1995) suggested that maladaptive perfectionists also have
a strong need to avoid failure and often feel vulnerable to the
possibility of public criticism. Consequently, maladaptive
perfectionists tend to view social and performance domains (and
particularly significant others) as threatening, overly demanding, and
non-supportive. Frost and Henderson (1991) suggested that the focus on
mistakes (and the possibility of negative social evaluation) may cause
athletes with maladaptive perfectionism to lose focus of important
task-relevant thoughts soon after committing an error during
competition. Th ese researchers further speculated that this loss in
concentration might ultimately lead to a drop in performance level.
Given that flawless performances rarely occur in sport, athletes with
maladaptive perfectionist orientations will seldom be satisfied with
their performances and will have a tendency to view themselves as
failures (cf. Burns, 1980).
In contrast to individuals who adopt a maladaptive perfectionist
orientation, adaptive perfectionists set high personal performance
standards while maintaining the ability to view themselves as successful
even when these standards are not fully achieved (Hamachek, 1978). In
other words, adaptive perfectionists "are able to accept both
personal and environmental limitations" (Blatt, 1995, p. 1006) when
pursuing their performance goals, and "feel free to be less precise
[i.e., 'less perfect'] as the situation permits"
(Hamachek, p. 27). Given their ability to accept personal limitations
and environmental obstacles, adaptive perfectionists are more likely
(than maladaptive perfectionists) to experience a sense of satisfaction
following their achievement oriented efforts (Blatt, 1995), even when
the 'perfect performance' is not realized. It seems reasonable
to speculate that this adaptive form of perfectionism is the
perfectionist orientation that many sport psychologists propose as being
a trademark feature of h igh-performance athletes (e.g., Hardy et al.,
1996; Henschen, 2000). Given the limited amount of research on
perfectionism in sport, understanding the correlates of both types of
perfectionism will aid sport psychologists' efforts to intervene
with maladaptive perfectionists.
Contemporary perfectionism theorists conceptualize and measure
perfectionism as a multidimensional construct. The two most prominent
measures of perfectionism cited in the extant literature are the
similarly named Multidimensional Perfectionism Scales (MPS) developed
independently by Frost et al. (1990) and Hewitt and Flett (1991). 1 To
date, perfectionism in sport has been measured almost exclusively by
Frost et al.'s instrument (i.e., the Frost-MPS).2 The Frost-MPS
contains six subscales labeled Personal Standards, Concern Over
Mistakes, Doubts About Actions, Parental Criticism, Parental
Expectations, and Organization. Personal standards perfectionism
reflects the extent to which individuals set extremely high standards of
personal performance. The concern over mistakes dimension describes an
individual's tendency to react negatively to personal mistakes in
the performance environment. The parental expectations and parental
criticism dimensions reflect the degree to which individuals perceive
that their pa rents set excessively high achievement goals for them, and
perceive that their parents are overly critical in evaluating their
achievement efforts. The doubts about actions dimension reflects the
degree to which an individual feels that tasks are never completed to
satisfaction, and the organization dimension represents an
individual's preference for order and organization (Frost et al.,
1990).
Empirical research indicates that the concern over mistakes,
parental criticism, parental expectations, and doubts about actions
subscales are generally associated with maladaptive functioning (for a
review see Enns & Cox, 2002). In contrast, research has been more
equivocal about the functional nature of the personal standards and
organization subscales. For example, Frost and colleagues (1990)
reported that personal standards was positively correlated with
self-efficacy among female undergraduates (r = .53), yet when the
variance in this correlation was controlled for using a partial
correlation, personal standards was also significantly correlated with
depression (r = .29). Similarly, an examination of perfectionism among
female intercollegiate athletes conducted by Frost and Henderson (1991)
found that personal Standards perfectionism was strongly correlated with
the tendency for athletes to focus on both achieving success (r = .68)
and the possibility of failure (r = .37).
Research examining the adaptive versus maladaptive nature of the
organization dimension has also produced equivocal findings. For
example, Frost and colleagues (1990) found that organization was
negatively correlated with the frequency of procrastination behaviors on
academic tasks among a sample of 106 female college students (r = -.37).
However, high levels of organization have been associated with burnout in youth tennis (Gould, Udry, et al., 1996). Thus, it appears that
personal standards and organization have the potential to function as
adaptive or maladaptive components of perfectionism. In order to better
understand when personal standards or organization operate as indicators
of adaptive or maladaptive perfectionism, it is important to examine the
levels of these variables in the context of other perfectionism
dimensions. For example, it may be that high personal standards is
adaptive when coupled with low levels of concern over mistakes, parental
expectations, and parental criticism (see Parker, 19 97). Conversely,
high personal standards may be maladaptive when coupled with high levels
of concern over mistakes, parental expectations, and parental criticism
(see Hall et al., 1998).
Given the ambiguous findings regarding the functional role of
personal standards and organization, examining all perfectionism
dimensions simultaneously may shed further light on the correlates of
maladaptive and adaptive perfectionism. The benefits of this approach
were highlighted by Parker (1997) in a study of 820 academically
talented sixth grade children who completed the Frost-MPS together with
measures of clinical maladjustment, global self-esteem, and personality.
A hierarchical cluster analysis produced three perfectionism groups that
Parker (p. 555) labeled "non-perfectionists", "healthy
perfectionists" (i.e., adaptive perfectionists), and
"unhealthy perfectionists " (i.e., maladaptive
perfectionists). Children in the healthy perfectionism cluster had high
levels of organization, moderate levels of personal standards and
parental expectations, and low levels of parental criticism, doubts
about actions, and concern over mistakes. The results of personality
assessments indicated that these students w ere conscientious, goal and
achievement oriented, predictable, and socially at ease" (p. 555).
Children in the unhealthy perfectionism cluster had high scores on the
personal standards, concern over mistakes, doubts about actions,
parental criticism, and parental expectations subscales of the Frost-MPS
and moderate scores on the organization dimension. Individuals in this
cluster were described as being anxious, disagreeable, moody, defensive
and socially detached. Thus, using this multi-dimensional analysis, the
personal standards and organization subscales were identified as factors
in both the adaptive and maladaptive clusters. In contrast to earlier
research that examined correlates of each Frost-MPS subscale separately,
this analysis revealed the importance of simultaneously examining the
relative presence of different subscales.
It is also worth noting that Parker (1997) found significantly
higher levels of self-esteem (as measured by Rosenberg's [1965]
Self Esteem Scale) in the healthy perfectionism group than in the
unhealthy perfectionism group. This finding supports Blatt's (1995)
contention that adaptive perfectionists are able to maintain (or foster)
high levels of self-esteem because they are better able than maladaptive
perfectionists to experience or perceive success and satisfaction
following performance efforts (regardless of whether the 'perfect
performance is actually achieved). In the context ofsport, Zinsser and
associates (2001) have similarly posited that "athletes who believe
they should be perfect will blame themselves for every defeat, every
setback...[and consequently] their self-concept will likely suffer"
(p. 302).
Self-esteem can be defined as a trait-like (i.e., dispositional)
evaluative attitude that people have towards themselves (Coopersmith,
1967) in reference to their own value, importance, and self-worth
(Blascovich & Tomaka, 1991). However, researchers also propose that
self-esteem can be regarded as a state-like construct that fluctuates
from moment to moment in response to situational circumstances and
events (e.g., Heatherton & Polivy, 1991). Identifying correlates of
self-esteem in sport is important given that research has consistently
shown self-esteem to be associated with a wide variety of cognitive,
affective, and behavioral factors in a variety of settings (Leary,
Tambor, Terdal, & Downs, 1995). For example, research in nonsport
settings has shown that people with low self-esteem tend to be less
happy (Freedman, 1978), experience more anxiety (Baumeister, 1993), and
exert less effort on tasks (McFarlin, Baumeister, & Blascovich,
1984) than people with high self-esteem. In the competitive sport enviro nment, Kerr and Goss (1997) found that elite adolescent artistic
gymnasts with low self-esteem lacked a sense of personal control over
their lives. Other sport research conducted by Berry and Howe (2000)
found that low self-esteem female university varsity athletes were more
prone to experiencing competitive anxiety and were more likely to adopt
behaviors that were symptomatic of eating disorders than their higher
self-esteem counterparts.
According to Sonstroem and colleagues (Sonstroem, Harlow, Gemma,
& Osborne, 1991; Sonstroem & Morgan, 1989), global self-esteem
is primarily determined by an individual's level of perceived
competence and self-acceptance. Sonstroem and associates (1991; p. 350)
defined perceived competence as an individual's "feelings of
mastery and control over the self and environment" in a specific
domain. In turn, Sonstroem and colleagues theorized that perceived
competence would be influenced by an individual's level of
self-efficacy (Bandura, 1986)3/4defined as "a statement of
expectancy about one's ability to accomplish a specific task"
(Fox, 1997, p. xiii). In contrast, self-acceptance is defined as an
individual's respect for her/himself in the face of admitted faults
(Wylie, 1979). Thus, it is predicted that individuals who feel competent
in a particular performance domain and who respect themselves regardless
of success or failure will likely have high levels of self-esteem in
that domain (Sonstroem et al., 1991; Sonstroem & Morgan, 1989).
When self-esteem is conceptualized in accordance with
Sonstroem's views (i.e., Sonstroem et al., 1991; Sonstroem &
Morgan, 1989), it makes theoretical sense that Parker (1997) found
higher levels of self-esteem among the sixth grade children who were in
the healthy (i.e., adaptive) perfectionism group than those in the
unhealthy (i.e., maladaptive) perfectionism group. Indeed, according to
perfectionism theorists, adaptive perfectionists have a tendency to
"rejoice in their skills, and appreciate a job well-done"
(Hamachek, 1978, p. 27), which would seem indicative of a person with
high self-acceptance. In contrast, maladaptive perfectionists are
expected to develop lower levels of global self-esteem because (a) they
frequently experience a strong sense of self-doubt (Blatt, 1995) which
is indicative of low perceived competence, and (b) are frequently overly
critical of their performance efforts (Hamachek) which is indicative of
low self-acceptance. Armed with their respective levels of competence
and self-ac ceptance, it is little wonder theorists propose that
adaptive perfectionists "tend to enhance their self-esteem"
(Hamachek, p. 27) while maladaptive perfectionists generally experience
"precipitous loss[es] in self-esteem" (Burns, 1980, p. 34)
following performance attempts.
We found ten published studies (i.e., Accordino, Accordino, &
Slaney, 2000; Campbell & Di Paula, 2002; Cheng, Chong, & Wong,
1999; Flett, Hewitt, Blankstein, & O'Brien, 1991; Koivula,
Hassm(n, & Fallby, 2002; Parker, 1997; Preusser, Rice, & Ashby,
1994; Rice, Ashby, & Slaney, 1998; Slaney, Rice, Mobley, Trippi,
& Ashby, 2001; Vohs, Bardone, Joiner, Abramson, & Heatherton,
1999) that examined the relationship between perfectionism and
self-esteem. Results of these studies consistently showed that
maladaptive dimensions of perfectionism were associated with lower
levels of self-esteem. In contrast, adaptive dimensions of perfectionism
were generally found to have no relationship with self-esteem. For
example, of the four studies that used the Frost-MPS to assess
perfectionism (i.e., Cheng et al., 1999; Parker, 1997; Rice et al.,
1998; Slaney et al., 2001), all found significant relationships between
maladaptive components of perfectionism (i.e., concern over mistakes,
doubts about actions, parental criticism, a nd parental expectations)
and low levels of self-esteem. Rice and colleagues (1998) also reported
a strong significant negative correlation (r = -.62) between a
maladaptive perfectionism factor (composed of concern over mistakes,
doubts about actions, parental expectations and parental criticism
subscales) and self-esteem following a factor analysis of data provided
by 173 male and female undergraduates. However, with the exception of
Parker's study, none of three other studies found significant
relationships between self-esteem and potentially adaptive dimensions of
perfectionism (i.e., personal standards and organization: cf. Frost et
al., 1993).
Of the studies reviewed, only Koivula et al. (2002) examined
perfectionism and self-esteem in sport. Using a sample of elite male and
female Swedish athletes (N = 178), Koivula et al. found that athletes
who reported high levels of self-esteem had significantly higher
personal standards, lower concern over mistakes, and lower doubts about
actions scores than athletes with low levels of self esteem. However,
Koivula et al. did not use the parental expectations, parental
criticism, and organization subscales of the Frost-MPS, thereby making
it impossible to create profiles of adaptive and maladaptive
perfectionism based on all six subscales. Indeed, with the exception of
Parker's (1997) research, none of the ten studies reviewed
considered all dimensions of perfectionism simultaneously when examining
their relationship with self-esteem. These limitations hinder our
understanding of the relationships between adaptive and maladaptive
profiles of perfectionism and self-esteem in sport. Consequently, the
purpose of the present study was to examine the relationship between
perfectionism and self-esteem in sport by considering all six dimensions
of the Frost-MPS simultaneously when creating perfectionism profiles. It
was hypothesized that a profile of maladaptive perfectionism would be
associated with low levels of self-esteem and that a profile of adaptive
perfectionism would be associated with high levels of self-esteem.
Method
Participants
Eighty-seven varsity athletes from a mid-sized NCAA Division I
university in the United States participated in the study. Participants
were members of the university's varsity cross-country (10 females,
14 males), track and field (20 females, 13 males), swimming (16 females,
7 males), and diving (5 females, 2 males) teams. Athletes ranged in age
from 17 to 25 years (Mage = 19.65, SD = 1.62).
Measures
Athletes completed self-report inventories that assessed
demographic information, perfectionism, global self-esteem, and state
self-esteem. The demographic inventory asked athletes to provide
information about their age, gender, and the varsity team that they
currently competed with.
Perfectionism. Perfectionism was assessed with the Frost-MPS. The
instrument contains 35 items that measure the perfectionism dimensions
of Personal Standards (seven items), Concern Over Mistakes (nine items),
Doubts About Actions (four items), Parental Criticism (four items),
Parental Expectations (five items), and Organization (six items).
Respondents indicate their level of agreement to each item on a 5-point
scale (1 = Strongly Disagree; 5 = Strongly Agree). Higher composite
subscale scores represent higher levels of perfectionism on each
respective dimension. All of the subscales possess acceptable levels of
internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha ranged from .77 to .93;
Frost et al., 1990). Recent studies in sport by Hall and associates
(1998) and Gould and colleagues (1996) have also reported acceptable
levels of internal consistency for the Frost-MPS subscales. However,
Hall et al. did not use the organization subscale and had to delete one
item from the doubts about actions subscale before achieving a n alpha
of .69. Validation studies of the Frost-MPS have shown that its
subscales are related in a meaningful way to other perfectionism
measures including Hewitt and Flett's (1991) MPS (see Frost et al.,
1993) and Burns's (1980) perfectionism scale (see Frost et al.,
1990).
Global self-esteem. The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES;
Rosenberg, 1965) was used to assess levels of global self-esteem (i.e.,
trait self-esteem). Respondents rate their level of agreement across ten
items using a 5-point scale (I = Strongly Disagree; 5 = Strongly Agree).
Reverse scoring of appropriate items and summation of the resulting
scores produces a unidimensional assessment of self-esteem with higher
scores representing higher levels of global self-esteem. Research has
indicated that the RSES provides a valid and reliable assessment of
global self-esteem (for reviews see Blascovich & Tomaka, 1991;
Wylie, 1989). The RSES is the most frequently used measure of trait
self-esteem in social psychological research (Baldwin & Sinclair,
1996) and has been used to measure global self-esteem in athletes by a
number of sport psychology researchers (e.g., Berry & Howe, 2000;
Mahoney, 1989; Prapavessis & Grove, 1998; Teetor Waite, Gansneder,
& Rotella, 1990).
State self-esteem. A modified version of the State Self-Esteem
Scale (SSES; Heatherton & Polivy, 1991) was used to assess
athletes' state levels of self-esteem in the context of competitive
sport. The original SSES asks respondents to indicate how they feel or
think at the present time using a 5-point scale (1 Not at All; 5 =
Extremely). After reversing the scores on appropriate items, scores are
summed such that higher composite subscale scores reflect higher levels
of state self-esteem. The original 20-item instrument is composed of
three subscales that measure performance self-esteem, social
self-esteem, and appearance self-esteem. The performance subscale (seven
items) measures how proud individuals are of their performance. The
social subscale (seven items) assesses how embarrassed, self-conscious,
or foolish people feel about their public image. The appearance subscale
(six items) measures how concerned or sensitive respondents feel about
their physical appearance.
In the present study, the items of the SSES were reworded to obtain
a situation-specific measure of state self-esteem in sport. For example,
the item "I feel confident about my abilities" from the
original SSES was reworded as "I feel confident about my athletic
abilities." The instructions of the modified SSES (henceforth
labeled, Sport-SSES) informed respondents that the purpose of the
inventory was "to identify how people view themselves in regards to
being athletes and being athletic." Given this instructional set,
it was felt that some of the original SSES items should be deleted
entirely from the instrument because their inclusion might jeopardize the face validity of the modified scale. For example, the researchers
were concerned that including sport-specific versions of some items from
the original SSES (e.g., "I am dissatisfied with my weight"
and "I feel unattractive") could have caused respondents to
question the stated purpose of the questionnaire. Consequently, the six
items of the appearance sub scale and four items from the social
subscale of the original SSES were not included in the Sport-SSES. The
final 10-item Sport-SSES contained the seven items from the performance
subscale and three items from the social subscale of the original SSES.
Table 1 contains a complete list of the items in the modified
instrument. Given the extensive deletion and modification of items from
Heatherton and Polivy's (1991) original scale, the Sport-SSES must
be viewed as an exploratory instrument with no previously established
validity and reliability evidence.
Procedure
The coaches of the eight varsity athletic teams were contacted and
asked for permission to recruit their team members as possible
participants. Once permission was secured from the coaches, one member
of the research team met with and presented the study to the members of
each team during their respective competitive seasons. Athletes were
informed that the purpose of the study was to "examine the thought
processes that occur in athletes in regards to being athletic and
towards athletic performance." The researcher explained that
participation consisted of completing a questionnaire packet. Individual
team members were then allowed to decide if they wished to participate
in the study. All athletes who were approached voluntarily agreed to
participate in the study and provided written informed consent prior to
completing the questionnaire packet. Instruments were completed in
classroom settings at least 24 hours prior to competition. Coaches were
not present during test administration. The presentation order of
instruments was counterbalanced to control for any order effects.
Results
Psychometric Analyses
Sport-SSES. To examine the latent structure of the Sport-SSES, an
exploratory principal axes factor analysis was conducted upon the data
provided by the athletes. Two eigenvalues (([lambda])> 1.0 were
obtained from the extraction (([[lambda].sub.1] = 4.17,
([lambda].sub.2], = 1.22). The two factors accounted for 64% of the
variance in the data. Factors were rotated orthogonally with a varimax
rotation and transformed obliquely using direct oblimin (delta = 0).
Thurstone's (1947) principle of simple structure was used to
judge the adequacy of the orthogonal and oblique solutions. In the
present analysis, the simple structure across the set of items was
superior for the oblique solution. Examination of the two factors (see
Table I) indicated that the factor structure of the Sport-SSES varied
considerably from the original SSES. Of the six items that loaded on the
first factor (F 1), three were originally part of the SSES performance
subscale (i.e., items 3,4, and 10), and three were originally part of
the SSES social subscale (i.e., items 2, 6, and 7). In other words, the
six items relate to athletes' satisfaction with current performance
levels and also to concerns about negative social evaluation. Given that
negative social evaluation concerns are sometimes considered a part of
performance failure concerns (see Dunn, 1999, for related discussion), F
I was labeled, Satisfaction With Current Sport Performance (SCSP).
However, it should also be noted that self-accepta nce (which is
generally viewed as a major dimension of self-esteem; Sonstroem, 1997)
is founded upon the principle of satisfaction with oneself (Wylie,
1979). Therefore, it is possible that F1 may also be tapping an aspect
of state self-esteem relating to self-acceptance of sport performance.
Items 1, 5, 8, and 9 loaded on the second factor (F2). All of these
items were originally in the performance subscale of Heatherton and
Polivy's (1991) instrument. A sense of confidence and competence
regarding one's athletic abilities, especially in comparison to
others, is a common feature inherent within these four items. The second
factor was therefore deemed to reflect an athlete's general level
of Perceived Athletic Competence (PAC).
The inter-factor correlation was quite large (r = .61). When
interpreting the meaning of this correlation, it is important to note
that five of the six items in F 1 (i.e., items 2, 3,4, 6, and 7) were
reversed scored prior to conducting the EFA. In other words, a higher
composite subscale score on Fl reflects a higher level of satisfaction
with current performance. Thus, it makes theoretical sense that
satisfaction with one's current sport performance (F1) would be
positively correlated with perceived athletic competence (F2).
Internal consistency analyses. The internal consistency
coefficients (alphas) for all scales and subscales were (.70 (see Table
2), indicating acceptable levels of internal reliability. However, it
should be noted that coefficient alpha for the parental expectations
subscale of the Frost-MPS did not exceed .70 when all five items were
originally included. To reach an acceptable level of internal
consistency, one item from the subscale (Item 26: "My parents have
always had higher expectations for my future than I have") had to
be deleted. This item was subsequently omitted from all further data
analyses.
Tests of Gender Effects
Two multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA) were conducted to
determine if gender differences existed across the subscales of the
Frost-MPS, RSES, and Sport-SSES. In the first MANOVA, gender was entered
as the independent variable, with RSES, PAC, and SCSP scores as
dependent variables. The overall multivariate test was non-significant,
Wilks' [LAMBDA] = .927, F (3, 83) 2.169, p = .10, indicating that
there were no significant differences between the responses of male and
female athletes on these variables. In the second MANOVA, gender was
again entered as the independent variable, and the six Frost-MPS
subscales entered as dependent variables. A non-significant multivariate
test statistic was again obtained, Wilks' [LAMBDA] = .937, F (6,
80) = .892, p = .51, indicating no gender differences on the
perfectionism subscales. Given these results, male and female data were
collapsed into a single data set. Table 2 contains the mean subscale
item scores (and standard deviations) of all instruments, as well as t
he correlations among all measures for the combined sample.
Multivariate Relationship Between Perfectionism and Self-Esteem
Canonical correlation (RC) analysis was used to examine the
multivariate relationship between perfectionism and self-esteem. The
three measures of self-esteem (i.e., RSES scores and the two Sport-SSES
subscales) comprised the criterion set, and the six Frost-MPS subscales
comprised the predictor set. Statisticians generally recommend that the
minimum subject-to-variable ratio to conduct canonical correlation
analysis should be 10:1 (Hair, Anderson, Tatham, & Black, 1998;
Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996). Given that the present study has 87
participants and nine variables in the analysis, the subject-to-variable
ratio (9.67:1) falls slightly short of this recommended criterion, and
so results should be interpreted with some degree of caution. The
overall multivariate test was significant, Wilks' [and]= .371, F
(18,221.10) = 5.156, p <.001. Of the three canonical functions
extracted, only the first function was statistically significant
([R.sub.c] = .74, p < .001). Hair et al. (1998) recommend that the
"practical si gnificance" (p. 451) of a [R.sub.c] should be
considered in addition to the level of statistical significance when
interpreting results. To this end, Tabachnick and Fidell (1996) suggest
that to achieve practical significance, the minimum acceptable [R.sub.c]
value should be [absolute value of .30]; the present [R.sub.c] value
(.74) greatly exceeds this criterion. Redundancy analysis revealed that
35% of the variance in the self-esteem variate was explained by the
perfectionism variate.
Statisticians generally recommend that variables with canonical
loadings < [absolute value of .30] on their respective canonical
variates should not be interpreted (e.g., Hair et al., 1998; Tabachnick
& Fidell, 1996). Six of the nine variables in the analysis exceeded
this criterion, and were therefore used for interpretative purposes. In
the self-esteem canonical variate, RSES and SCSP variables had strong
negative loadings (-.899 and -.890 respectively), and PAC had a moderate
negative loading (-.544). This pattern of canonical loadings reflects an
orientation towards low self-esteem (cf. Sonstroem et al., 1991;
Sonstroem & Morgan, 1989). On the perfectionism canonical variate,
concern over mistakes and doubts about actions had strong positive
loadings (.762 and .792 respectively) and parental criticism had a
moderate positive loading (.393). The canonical loadings for the
personal standards, organization, and parental expectations subscales of
the Frost-MPS did not reach the criterion level of(.30( (i.e., .038,
-.076, and . 074 respectively), and were therefore not interpreted.
Given that concern over mistakes, doubts about actions, and parental
criticism are generally associated with maladaptive perfectionism, the
moderate to high canonical loadings for these variables suggests that
the corresponding canonical variate represents a maladaptive profile of
perfectionism. Overall, the canonical correlation results reveal that a
maladaptive profile of perfectionism (as represented by the pattern of
loadings on the perfectionism variate) is strongly correlated with low
self-esteem among athletes in the present study.
Discussion
The purpose of the present study was to examine the multivariate
relationship between perfectionism and self-esteem among competitive
athletes using an analytic protocol that allowed the consideration of
all six dimensions of the Frost-MPS simultaneously when distinguishing
between adaptive and maladaptive profiles of perfectionism. Canonical
correlation results supported the hypothesis that a maladaptive profile
of perfectionism would be associated with low levels of self-esteem. In
contrast, there was no evidence to support the hypothesis that an
adaptive profile of perfectionism would be associated with higher levels
of self-esteem. Although Parker (1997) found differences in self-esteem
levels between adaptive and maladaptive perfectionists, the present
results are in line with other research which has found no relationship
between adaptive dimensions of perfectionism and the self-esteem
construct (e.g., Flett et al., 1991; Rice et al., 1998). Indeed, Rice et
al. have even questioned whether relationships between adaptive
dimensions of perfectionism and self-esteem exist at all. Clearly, more
research is required to examine the relationship between adaptive
perfectionism and self-esteem in sport.
In the current study self-esteem was measured with trait (i.e., the
RSES) and state instruments (i.e., the Sport-SSES). The RSES measured
global self-esteem which can be conceptualized as an average feeling
that people have about themselves across a variety of social contexts
(cf. Heatherton & Polivy, 1991). Given that the Sport-SSES was
considered to be a newly developed instrument with no previously
established validity or reliability evidence, it is important to note
that the two Sport-SSES subscales (i.e., 'perceived athletic
competence' and 'satisfaction with current sport
performance') had moderate to strong correlations with the RSES (r
= .56 and r = .63 respectively: see Table 2). The magnitude and
direction of these correlations are very similar to those obtained by
Heatherton and Polivy with 102 university undergraduate students who
completed the RSES and the original SSES. Thus, although the two
Sport-SSES subscales that emerged from the exploratory factor analysis
in this study (see Table 1) had conceptual differences with the original
SSES subscales, the observed inter-scale correlations between the RSES
and Sport-SSES provide support for the position that the Sport-SSES is
measuring components of state self-esteem in sport.
The current findings are relevant to the context of sport, however,
the bivariate correlations obtained in this study also support
predictions made by Sonstroem and Morgan's (1989) Exercise and
Self-Esteem Model about the influence of perceived physical competence
and self-acceptance on global self-esteem. Specifically, Sontsroem and
Morgan's model predicts that individuals who have low perceived
physical competence and who lack self-acceptance will have low global
self-esteem. The strong positive correlations between RSES and PAC (r =
.56), and between RSES and SCSP (r = .63), clearly demonstrate that
athletes with low perceived physical competence (i.e., PAC) and low
self-acceptance (i.e., a proposed component of SCSP) are inclined to
have lower global self-esteem than athletes with higher perceived
physical competence and higher self-acceptance. Although Sonstroem and
Morgan's model has been tested extensively in the exercise
psychology literature (e.g., Baldwin & Courneya, 1997; Sonstroem et
al., 1991; S onstroem, Harlow, & Josephs, 1994), researchers have
not examined the model's utility in the competitive sport domain
(with the notable exception of a study by Sonstroem, Harlow, &
Salisbury, 1993). Thus, the current study provides some evidence for the
model's validity in sport.
The bivariate relationships between maladaptive perfectionism
subscales and global self-esteem were in the expected direction, with
parental criticism, concern over mistakes, and doubts about actions all
having significant negative correlations with the RSES. Indeed, the
direction and statistical significance of all correlations between the
Frost-MPS subscales and the RSES (see Table 2) are very similar to those
obtained by Slaney et al. (2001) with a sample of 174 undergraduate
students. As was the case in this study, Slaney and colleagues found
significant negative correlations between the RSES and the parental
criticism, concern over mistakes, and doubts about actions subscales of
the Frost-MPS, and non-significant correlations between the RSES and the
three remaining Frost-MPS subscales (i.e., personal standards, parental
expectations, and organization). The multivariate relationship between
perfectionism and self-esteem in the current study was examined using
canonical correlation. The pattern of canoni cal loadings across both
canonical variates, and the size of the canonical correlation ([R.sub.c]
= .74) clearly showed that low levels of self-esteem are strongly
associated with maladaptive perfectionism. That is, low self-esteem was
identified by the pattern of moderate to strong negative loadings for
RSES, PAC, and SCSP on the self-esteem variate. Maladaprive
perfectionism was reflected by the moderate to strong positive loadings
of the parental criticism, concern over mistakes, and doubts about
actions subscales on the perfectionism variate. Overall, the bivariate
and canonical correlation results demonstrate that maladaptive
perfectionism is associated with low self-esteem among the present
sample of athletes. Overall, these findings are consistent with previous
research that has examined the relationship between perfectionism and
self-esteem in non-sport settings (i.e., Accordino et al., 2000;
Campbell & Di Paula, 2002; Cheng et al., 1999; Flett et al., 1991;
Parker, 1997; Preusser et al., 1994; Rice e t al., 1998; Slaney et al.,
2001; Vohs et al., 1999).
The concern over mistakes, doubts about actions, and parental
criticism dimensions of the Frost-MPS have traditionally been identified
as maladaptive dimensions of perfectionism by researchers in sport
settings (e.g., Frost & Henderson, 1991; Gould, Udry, et al., 1996;
Hall et al., 1998). Indeed, Hall et al. observed a similar pattern of
loadings (to that obtained in the present study) for concern over
mistakes, doubts about actions, and parental criticism on a canonical
variate that reflected a profile of maladaptive perfectionism in a
sample of competitive high school cross-country runners. However, in
contrast to the present findings, an additional feature of the
maladaptive perfectionism profile identified by Hall et al. was a strong
positive canonical loading for the personal standards dimension of
perfectionism. Similarly, in the cluster of "unhealthy
perfectionists" in Parker's (1997) study of academically
talented school children, high concern over mistakes, doubts about
actions, and parental critici sm scores were also accompanied by high
personal standards scores. Clearly, the combination of high concern over
mistakes, doubts about actions, and parental criticism has a maladaptive
function. However, potential reasons why personal standards did not load
on maladaptive perfectionism in the present investigation need to be
considered.
Examination of the small non-significant bivariate correlations
between personal standards and the three self-esteem variables (Table 2)
shows that personal standards perfectionism was not related to trait
self-esteem or state self-esteem in this study. Sonstroem (1997) states
that people "tend to rely on any perceived success, skill, or
positive attribute as a basis for establishing, enhancing, or
maintaining self-esteem" (p. 4). Therefore, it seems reasonable to
speculate that it is athletes' perceptions of success (or failure)
relative to the personal standards that they set for themselves which
influence self-esteem, as opposed to the setting of high or low personal
standards per se that influences self-esteem. In other words, regardless
of whether athletes have a tendency to set high or low personal
standards, self-esteem may only be affected when athletes receive
information (either internally, or from people in the social
environment) that influences their perceptions of success or failure on
the task. Slaney, Rice, and Ashby (2002) have also proposed that it may
not be "high personal standards per se that contribute to poor
emotional adjustment [including low self-esteem]; rather, it may be the
responses that people have to their perceptions that they constantly
fail to meet their own standards that lead to emotional
difficulties" (p. 81). Thus, athletes with high levels of concern
over mistakes, doubts about actions, and parental criticism are likely
to perceive that the task was not completed well enough (regardless of
whether high or low personal standards pertaining to that task were set)
and to view themselves as failures (Blatt, 1995; Hamachek, 1978). As a
result of these negative self-evaluations, losses in self-esteem are
likely to occur (Bums, 1980).
In contrast to the ambiguous nature of personal standards, research
in youth sport has shown that the parental expectations dimension is
generally associated with maladaptive perfectionism. For example, Gould,
Udry, et al. (1996) found that youth tennis players (M age = 16.4 years)
suffering from burnout had significantly higher levels of parental
expectations than players who were not suffering from burnout.
Similarly, Hall et al. (1998) found that parental expectations had a
strong positive loading on a canonical variate that reflected
maladaptive perfectionism in their sample of high school cross-country
runners (M age = 14 years). In contrast, previous perfectionism research
in sport that has been conducted with adults has found little, if any,
functional relationship between parental expectations perfectionism and
obligatory running behaviors (M age = 44.07 years: Coen & Ogles,
1993) and reactions to mistakes in competition (intercollegiate
athletes: Frost & Henderson, 1991). Given that parental expectat
ions did not load on the maladaptive perfectionism variate and had no
significant bivariate correlations with any of the self-esteem measures
in this study (see Table 2), it is possible that age plays a role in
determining the functional role of parental expectations perfectionism
in sport. We might speculate that younger athletes are more likely to be
living at home and to be dependent upon their parents for many things in
their lives (including feedback and expectancies about performance). In
contrast, it is possible that adult athletes (including the
intercollegiate athletes in this study) are less likely to be as
dependent upon (or influenced by) their parents in terms of social
support and informational feedback about performance. Future research is
required to examine the extent to which age is a factor that determines
the adaptive or maladaptive nature of parental expectations in sport.
It should also be recognized that the two major interpersonal
dimensions of perfectionism that were measured in this study (i.e.,
parental criticism and parental expectations) focus the
respondent's attention entirely upon the standards, expectations,
and reactions of the parent. However, Dunn et al. (in press) have
recently proposed that in the competitive sport environment the coach
may be a highly salient source of interpersonal or socially prescribed
perfectionism (Hewitt & Flett, 1991). To this end, using data
provided by 174 high performance male Canadian Football players (M age
18.24 years), Dunn and colleagues empirically derived a dimension of
perfectionism (labeled Perceived Coach Pressure) which they presented as
a situationally specific component of interpersonal perfectionism in
sport. The perceived coach pressure dimension reflected the degree to
which athletes perceived that their coaches held high standards and were
overly critical of their (i.e., the players') performance efforts
in competit ion and training. Dunn and his associates found that a high
level of perceived coach pressure was associated with a maladaptive
profile of perfectionism among the sample. Previous research has shown
that it is not uncommon for athletes to perceive that coaches demand
perfection or set extremely high standards of performance in the
competitive sport environment (e.g., Krane, Greenleaf, & Snow,
1997). Future research that includes perceived coach pressure as an
interpersonal dimension of perfectionism may help further our
understanding of the relationship between multidimensional perfectionism
and self-esteem in sport.
In conclusion, results of the present study highlight the
importance of measuring perfectionism as a multidimensional construct.
Moreover, the study highlights benefits of considering all dimensions of
perfectionism simultaneously when attempting to identify profiles of
perfectionism that are either adaptive or maladaptive in nature. The
generalizability of the study's findings are limited by the
relatively small sample size and "psychometric infancy" of the
Sport-SSES, however, results demonstrate a clear link between
maladaptive perfectionist orientations and low self-esteem in sport. It
is also important to recognize that perfectionism in the present study
was assessed with the Frost-MPS which conceptualizes perfectionism as a
relatively stable global personality trait. However, it may be
beneficial to conceptualize and measure perfectionism as a
domain-specific construct (Flett & Hewitt, 2002). For example, in a
study of perfectionism among career mothers (N 67), Mitchelson and Bums
(1998) found significa ntly higher scores on all three subscales of the
Hewitt-MPS (Hewitt & Flett, 1991)another global measure of
perfectionism-when items were modified to reflect mothers'
perfectionist orientations "at home" in comparison to their
perfectionist orientations "at work." Thus, research with
sport-specific measures of perfectionism (including the recently
developed Sport-MPS; Dunn et al., in press) may provide a better
understanding of perfectionism in sport, and may subsequently aid with
the examination of perfectionism and its relationships with other
cognitive, affective, and behavioral variables in the sport domain.
Table 1
Pattern Coefficients From Principal Axes Analysis of the Sport-SSES
Pattern
Coefficients (a)
Item Full Item Descriptions F1 F2
2. I am worried about whether I
am regarded as a success or a
failure as an athlete. .58 .24
3. I feel frustrated or rattled
about my athletic
performance. .84 -.06
4. I feel that I am having
trouble performing
athletically. .79 .08
6. I feel displeased with my
athletic performance. .97 -.09
7. I am worried about what other
people think of my athletic
ability. .58 .20
10. I feel like I am not doing
well athletically. .53 -.02
1. I feel confident about my
athletic abilities. .24 .62
5. I feel as athletic as others. -.10 .75
8. I feel confident that I am a
good athlete. .17 .63
9. I feel that I have less
athletic ability right now
than others. .02 .59
Note. F1 = Satisfaction with Current Sport Performance; F2 = Perceived
Athletic Competence. a Interfactor correlation ([r.sub.F1F2]) = .61
Table 2
Means, Standard Deviations, Internal Consistencies (), and Correlations
(r) Between Self-Esteem and Perfectionism Measures
Self-Esteem Perfectionism
RSES (a) PAC (b) SCSP (c) PS (d)
M=4.06 M=3.80 M=3.36 M=3.68
Subscales (SD=.58) (SD=.74) (SD=.89) (SD=.67)
RSES [alpha]=.83
PAC .56 **** [alpha]=.78
SCSP .63 *** .60 **** [alpha]=.88
PS .09 -.00 -.14 [alpha]=.80
O .05 .08 .07 .13
COM -.43 **** -.34 **** -.59 **** .50 ****
DAA -.58 **** -.31 *** -.47 **** -.07
PE .01 -.16 -.14 .41 ***
PC -.30 **** -.30 *** -.26* .16
Perfectionism
O (e) COM (f) DAA (g) PE (h)
M=3.90 M=2.25 M=2.41 M=2.80
Subscales (SD=.75) (SD=.67) (SD=.71) (SD=.81)
RSES
PAC
SCSP
PS
O [alpha]=.90
COM -.03 [alpha]=.84
DAA -.08 .31 *** [alpha]=.70
PE .17 -23 *** .06 [alpha]=.70
PC -.06 -42 *** -38 **** -47 ****
Perfectionism
PC (i)
M=1.74
Subscales (SD=.72)
RSES
PAC
SCSP
PS
O
COM
DAA
PE
PC [alpha]=.80
Note. Mean subscale item scores and standard deviations are listed under
each subscale title. Internal consistency coefficients (alpha) are
contained in the main diagonal. Subscale correlations (r) are contained
in the lower triangular matrix.
(a)Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale.
(b)Perceived Athletic Competence.
(c)Satisfaction with Current Sport Performance.
(d)Personal Standards.
(e)Organization.
(f)Concern Over Mistakes.
(g)Doubts About Actions.
(h)Parental Expectations.
(i)Parental Criticism.
* p<.05.
** p<.01.
*** p<.005.
**** p<.001.
Footnotes
(1.) A detailed description and review of research that has
employed Hewitt and Flett's (1991) MPS is beyond the scope of this
paper. Interested readers are directed to Enns and Cox (2002) for an
overview
(2.) Although Dunn et al. (in press) did not use the Frost-MPS
(Frost et al., 1990), they did use a sport-specific measure of
perfectionism that was adapted from the Frost-MPS.
References
Accordino, D. B., Accordino, M. P., & Slaney, R. B. (2002). An
investigation of perfectionism, mental health, and achievement
motivation in adolescents. Psychology in the Schools, 37,535-545.
Baldwin, M. K., & Courneya, K. S. (1997). Exercise and
self-esteem in breast cancer survivors: An application of the exercise
and self-esteem model. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology,
19,347-358.
Baldwin, M. W., & Sinclair, L. (1996). Self-esteem and
"if...then" contingencies of interpersonal acceptance. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology 71, 1130-1141.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A
social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Baumeister, R. F. (Ed.). (1993). Self-esteem: The puzzle of low
self-regard. New York: Plenum.
Berry, T. R., & Howe, B. L. (2000). Risk factors for disordered
eating in female university athletes. Journal of Sport Behavior, 23,
207-219.
Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1991). Measures of self-esteem.
In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures
of personality and social psychology attitudes (pp. 115-160). San Diego,
CA: Academic Press.
Blatt, S. J. (1995). The destructiveness of perfectionism:
Implications for the treatment of depression. American Psychologist, 50,
1003-1020.
Burns, D. D. (1980). The perfectionist's script for
self-defeat. Psychology Today, 14(6), 34-52.
Campbell, J. D., & Di Paula, A. (2002). Perfectionistic
self-beliefs: Their relation to personality and goal pursuit. In G. L.
Flett & P. L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory research, and
treatment (pp. 181-198). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Cheng, S. K., Chong, G. H., & Wong, C. W. (1999). Chinese Frost
Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale: A validation and prediction of
self-esteem and psychological distress. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
55, 1051-1061.
Coen, S. P., & Ogles, B. M. (1993). Psychological
characteristics of the obligatory runner: A critical examination of the
anorexia analogue hypothesis. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology
15, 338-354.
Coopersmith, S. (1967). The antecedents of self-esteem. San
Francisco: Freeman.
Davis, C. (1997). Normal and neurotic perfectionism in eating
disorders: An interactive model. International Journal of Eating
Disorders. 22, 421-426.
Dunn, J. G. H. (1999). A theoretical framework for structuring the
content of competitive worry in ice hockey. Journal of Sport &
Exercise Psychology 21, 259-279.
Dunn, J. G. H., Causgrove Dunn, J. L. & Syrotuik, D. (in
press). Relationship between multidimensional perfectionism and goal
orientations in sport. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology.
Enns, M. W., & Cox, B. J. (2002). The nature and assessment of
perfectionism: A critical analysis. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt
(Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory research, and treatment (pp. 63-88).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2002). Perfectionism and
maladjustment: An overview of theoretical, definitional, and treatment
issues. In G. L. Flett & P. L, Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory
research and practice (pp. 5-31). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Blankstein, K., & O'Brien, S.
(1991). Perfectionism and learned resourcefulness in depression and
self-esteem. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 61-68.
Fox, K. R. (1997). The physical self From motivation to well being.
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Freedman, J. (1978). Happy people: What happiness is, who has it,
and why New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.
Frost, R. O., Heimberg, R. G., Holt, C. S., Mattia, J. I.,
&Neubauer, A. L. (1993). A comparison of two measures of
perfectionism. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 119-126.
Frost, R. O., & Henderson, K. J. (1991). Perfectionism and
reactions to athletic competition. Journal of Sport & Exercise
Psychology 13, 323-335.
Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990).
The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research,
14,449-468.
Gould, D., Tuffey, S., Udry, E., & Loehr, J. (1996). Burnout in
competitivejunior tennis players: II. Qualitative analysis. The Sport
Psychologist, 10,341-366.
Gould, D., Udry, E., Tuffey, S., & Loehr, J. (1996). Burnout in
competitivejunior tennis players: I. A quantitative psychological
assessment. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 322-340.
Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C.
(1998). Multivariate data analysis (5th ed). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Hall, H. K., Kerr, A. W., & Matthews, J. (1998). Precompetitive
anxiety in sport: The contribution of achievement goals and
perfectionism. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology 20, 194-217.
Hamachek, D. E. (1978). Psychodynamics of normal and neurotic
perfectionism. Psychology, JS, 27-33.
Hardy, L., Jones, G., & Gould, D. (1996). Understanding
psychological preparation for sport: Theory and practice of elite
performers. New York: Wiley.
Heatherton, T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and
validation of a scale for measuring state self-esteem. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 60, 895-910.
Henschen, K. (2000). Maladaptive fatigue syndrome and emotions in
sport. In Y. L. Hanin (Ed.), Emotions insporc(pp. 23 1-242). Champaign,
IL: Human Kinetics.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self
and social contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with
psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60,
456-470.
Kerr, G. A., & Goss, J. D. (1997). Personal control in elite
gymnasts: The relationships between locus of control, self-esteem, and
trait anxiety. Journal of Sport Behavior, 20, 69-83.
Koivula, N., Hassm(n, P., & Fallby, J. (2002). Self-esteem and
perfectionism in elite athletes: Effects on competitive anxiety and
self-confidence. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 865-875.
Krane, V., Greenleaf, C. A., & Snow, J. (1997). Reaching for
gold and the price of glory: A motivational case study of an elite
gymnast. The Sport Psychologist, 11, 53-71.
Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K., & Downs, D. L.
(1995). Self-esteem as an interpersonal monitor: The sociometer
hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 518-530.
Mahoney, M. J. (1989). Psychological predictors of elite and
non-elite performance in Olympic weightlifting. International Journal of
Sport Psychology, 20, 1-12.
McFarlin, D. G., Baumeister, R. F., & Blascovich, J. (1984). On
knowing when to quit: Task failure, self-esteem, advice, and respondent
persistence. Journal of Personality, 52, 138-155.
Mitchelson, J. K., & Bums, L. R. (1998). Career mothers and
perfectionism: Stress at work and home. Personality and Individual
Differences, 25,477-485.
Parker, W. D. (1997). An empirical typology of perfectionism in
academically talented children. American Educational Research Journal,
34, 545-562.
Prapavessis, H., & Grove, J. R. (1998). Self-handicapping and
self-esteem. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology 10, 175-184.
Preusser, K. J., Rice, K. G., & Ashby, J. 5. (1994). The role
of self-esteem in mediating the perfectionism-depression connection.
Journal of College Student Development, 35, 88-93.
Rice, K. G., Ashby, J. S., Slaney, R. B. (1998). Self-esteem as a
mediator between perfectionism and depression: A structural equations
analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology 45,304-314.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Scanlan, T. K., Stein, G. L., & Ravizza, K. (1989). An in-depth
study of former elite figure skaters: II. Sources of enjoyment. Journal
of Sport & Exercise Psychology 11, 65-83.
Scanlan, T. K., Stein, G. L., & Ravizza, K. (1991). An in-depth
study of former elite figure skaters: III. Sources of stress. Journal of
Sport & Exercise Psychology, 13, 103-120.
Slaney, R. B., Rice, K. G., & Ashby, J. S. (2002). A
programmatic approach to measuring perfectionism: The almost perfect
scales. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory,
research, and treatment (pp. 63-88). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Slaney, R. B., Rice, K. G., Mobley, M., Trippi, J., & Ashby, J.
S. (2001). The revised Almost Perfect Scale. Measurement and Evaluation
in Counseling and Development, 34, 130-145.
Sonstroem, R. J. (1997). The physical self-system: A mediator of
exercise and self-esteem. In K. R. Fox (Ed.), The physical self: From
motivation to well-being (pp. 3-26). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Sonstroem, R. J., Harlow, L. L., Gemma, L. M., & Osborne, S.
(1991). Test of structural relationships within a proposed exercise and
self-esteem model. Journal of Personality Assessment. 56, 348-364.
Sonstreom, R. J., Harlow, L. L., & Joseph, L. (1994). Exercise
and self-esteem: Validity of model expansion and exercise associations.
Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 16, 29-42.
Sonstreom, R. J., Harlow, L. L., & Salisbury, K. S. (1993).
Path analysis of a self-esteem model across a competitive swim season.
Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 64, 335-342.
Sonstroem, R. J., & Morgan, W. P. (1989). Exercise and
self-esteem: Rationale and model. Medicine and Science in Sports and
Exercise, 21, 329-337.
Tabachnick, B. G.,& Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate
statistics (3rd ed.).New York: HarperCollins.
Teetor Waite, B., Gansneder, B., & Rotella, R. J. (1990). A
sport-specific measure of self-acceptance. Journal of Sport &
Exercise Psychology 12, 264-279.
Terry-Short, L. A., Owens, R. G., Slade, P. D., & Dewey, M. E.
(1995). Positive and negative perfectionism. Personality and Individual
Differences, 18, 663-668.
Thurstone, L. L. (1947). Multiple-factor analysis. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Vohs, K. D., Bardone, A. M., Joiner, Jr., T. E., Abramson, L. Y,
Heatherton, T. F. (1999). Perfectionism, perceived weight status, and
self-esteem interact to predict bulimic symptoms: A model of bulimic
symptom development. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 108,695-700.
Wylie, R. C. (1979). The self-concept: Vol. 2. Theory and research
on selected topics (rev. ed.). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Wylie, R. C. (1989). Measures of self-concept. Lincoln: University
of Nebraska Press.
Zinsser, N., Bunker, L., & Williams, J. M. (2001). Cognitive
techniques for building confidence and enhancing performance. In J. M.
Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology: Personal growth to peak
performance (4th ed., pp. 284-311). Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.
Address Correspondence To: Dr. John Dunn, Faculty of Physical
Education and Recreation, E-424 Van Vi jet Centre, University of
Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada T6G 2H9.