The influence of significant others on the goal orientations of youngsters in physical education.
Carr, Sam ; Weigand, Daniel A.
The purpose of this study was to explore whether differences in
perceptions of the motivational climiate emphasized by teachers, peers
and sporting heroes correspond to differences in personal goal profiles
in children in PE; and to explore the relative influence that these
individuals have on children 's task and ego orientation.
Two-hundred and sixty-s ix secondary school PE pupils from the United
Kingdom completed surveys on personal goal orientations for PE and
perceptions of the motivational climate emphasized by PE teachers, peers
and sporting heroes. Results indicated that children with a high task
oriented goal profile componenet tended to inernalize higher perceptions
of a learning climate from these individuals than children with a low
task oriented profile component. Children with a high ego oriented
profile component tended to internalize higher perceptions of a
comparison climate from teachers, peers and heroes than those with a
lower ego oriented component. Teacher promotion of a learning climat e
was most strongly related to task orientation and peer promotion of a
comparision climate was mot strongly related to ego orientation. Results
are discussed in terms of the need to employ a goal profile approach in
the examination of antecedents of goal orientations, and the influence
that significant others have for enhancing the quality of
youngsters' PE experiences.
It has been recognised (Duda, 1996) that the domain of school based
physical education (PE) has great potential to reinforce a physically
active lifestyle for children. For many children, one of the first
opportunities they have to participate in organised sport is through PE.
Often it is the enjoyment that children get from their PE experiences
that compels them to join a local sports club and extend their sporting
interests (White, Kavussanu, & Guest, 1998). Sallis et al. (1992)
have additionally contended that school based PE has the most potential
for impacting public health because an existing infrastructure is
devoted to school based PE. However, despite this, research has
demonstrated that as they mature, a large percentage of youths lose
interest in PE and participation levels decrease (Van Wersch, Trew,
& Turner, 1992).
Duda (1996) has contended that in order to combat such disturbing
trends, and to maximise the opportunity for youngsters to engage in a
physically active lifestyle, researchers must develop an insight into
what is important about PE in the minds of youths. That is, an
understanding of why youths engage and invest in PE is necessary.
Accordingly, researchers (e.g., Ames, 1984; Duda, 1996; Nicholls, 1989)
have advocated the importance of the goal orientations that youngsters
hold in PE contexts. Such advances stem from an achievement goal theory
approach to motivation.
Achievement Goal Theory
A viable frame of reference for studying motivation in sport and PE
is achievement goal theory (Ames, 1984, 1992; Duda, 1992, 1993; Dweck,
1986; Elliot & Dweck, 1988; Maehr & Nicholls, 1980; Nicholls,
1984, 1989). Fundamentally, achievement goal theory posits that
individuals engage in achievement contexts for the primary purpose of
demonstrating competence. Such competence is construed, however, in
different ways, in accordance with the goal involvement that individuals
adopt (Nicholls, 1984, 1989) in specific situations. Specifically, in
task-involved states individuals view competence as synonymous with improvement, effort, learning, and task mastery. In contrast,
individuals in a state of ego-involvement equate competence with the
demonstration of superior ability and outperforming others (Nicholls,
1984, 1989).
Achievement goal theorists (e.g., Duda, 1992, 1996; Dweck &
Leggett, 1988; Nicholls, 1989, 1992) have contended that state goal
involvement is a function of both dispositional differences and the
characteristics of the situation. Dispositional differences are seen as
the a priori probability of internalising a particular goal orientation
and situational factors are seen as potentially altering these
probabilities (Dweck & Leggett, 1988). For example, Dweck and
Leggett have (1988) contended that in a situation affording a choice
between predominantly task- and ego- involved goals, individuals bring
with them a dispositional tendency (a goal Orientation) to favour one or
the other. If the situation offers no cues or specific goal emphasis it
is likely that the dispositional goal orientation will prevail and will
determine the individual's current state of goal involvement.
Goal Orientations
With reference to dispositional goal orientations, task-oriented
individuals have a general tendency to seek to demonstrate competence
using criteria such as personal progression, task mastery, the
application of effort, and the learning of skills (Nicholls, 1984,
1989). Ego-oriented individuals have a tendency to seek to demonstrate
competence by proving their superior ability to others (Nicholls, 1984,
1989).
Research (see Duda, 1996 for a review) in the PE domain has
indicated that individual differences in task and ego orientation have
important implications for sustained motivation and the quality of
experiences (e.g., Duda, 1992, 1993; Roberts, 1992). Consistent patterns
of affective and cognitive concomitants have been identified for
predominantly task, versus ego, oriented individuals. For example, task
oriented students have been shown to believe that success in PE was due
to effort, co-operation, and intrinsic interest whereas ego oriented
students believed success was due to deception and superior ability
(Solmon & Lee, 1996; Walling & Duda, 1995). Task oriented
students have also been found to perceive increasing self-esteem,
developing good citizenship, and fostering mastery as the purposes of
PE. In contrast, ego oriented students have viewed enhancing self-esteem
and social status as the purposes of PE (Papaioannou & McDonald,
1993; Walling & Duda, 1995). In addition, task oriented students
have also been shown to exhibit higher levels of intrinsic motivation
for PE than ego oriented students (Goudas & Biddle, 1994; Goudas et
al., 1995).
The antecedents of dispositional goal orientations in sport and PE
have received less attention than the concomitants. Achievement goal
theorists (e.g., Duda, 1996; Nicholls, 1989) have contended that
dispositional goal orientations stem partly from the social environment.
Accordingly, Duda (1987) has outlined the need for researchers to
address the impact that various significant social agents might have on
youngsters' goal orientations.
The Influence of Significant Others
One factor that has begun to generate some interest from
social-cognitive researchers is the structure of the motivational
climate emphasised by various significant others. From this perspective,
based upon the work of Ames and her colleagues (Ames, 1992; Ames &
Archer, 1988), the motivational climate emphasised by a given social
agent is determined by that agent's own personal goals, emphasised
evaluation and reward processes, and task, authority and grouping
structures of the environment. Individuals are able to detect this
achievement climate from interaction with social agents in achievement
contexts.
In classroom contexts (e.g., Ames, 1992) a mastery-oriented climate
is hypothesised to prevail when teachers focus on self-improvement and
task mastery by rewarding effort and personal progression. Such a
climate induces a task focus and students typically adopt adaptive
achievement strategies such as investing high amounts of effort,
choosing challenging tasks, and demonstrating persistence even when
facing difficulty. In contrast, if students perceive a focus on
normative criteria, social comparison, and competition, a
performance-oriented climate prevails. A performance-oriented climate
normally occurs as a function of teachers selectively praising more able
students and emphasising competition above effort and personal
progression. Performance climates typically induce maladaptive achievement strategies such as low levels of effort, avoidance of
challenging tasks, and an increased likelihood that children will give
up in challenging situations (Ames, 1992).
Research has begun to incorporate such perspectives on the
motivational climate in the context of PE. In particular, perceptions of
climates consistently emphasised by teachers and peers are beginning to
be regarded as a salient influence on the development of
youngsters' personal goal orientations.
For example, the influence of the motivational climate emphasised
by PE teachers has been examined (Goudas & Biddle, 1994;
Papaiouannou, 1994, 1995). Borrowing from the work of Ames (e.g., Ames
& Archer, 1988) in an academic classroom context, such research has
indicated that perceptions of a performance-oriented PE class climate is
associated with higher ego orientation in youngsters. On the other hand,
perceptions of a class climate that is predominantly mastery-oriented
are associated with higher task orientation in youngsters. For example,
Goudas et al. (1995) manipulated the climate in a PE athletics class. As
hypothesised, students exposed to the mastery-oriented climate developed
a higher task orientation and levels of intrinsic motivation than
students exposed to the performance-oriented climate.
The influence of the motivational climate created by peers in PE
has been allotted less attention. However, research (e.g., Carr,
Weigand, & Hussey, 1999) is beginning to suggest that peers are
influential in the development of dispositional goal orientations. For
example, Carr et al. (1999) examined parental, peer, and teacher
influence on various cognitive and affective correlates of children and
adolescents' PE experiences. Results indicated that
adolescents' task orientation for PE was predicted by perceptions
of a lower worry-conducive climate from peers. In addition, perceptions
of a higher pursuit of comparison and lower promotion of learning
emphasised by peers predicted adolescents' ego orientation.
Despite the increasing indication that the social environment plays
an important role in the development of goal orientations, researchers
have, to date, neglected to look beyond those social agents with whom
children experience direct personal interaction. One such socio-cultural
agent, whom researchers (e.g., Russell, 1979; Smith, 1974) have
advocated has vast potential to impact upon children's
intellectual, moral, emotional, and psychosocial development, are
sporting heroes. Such heroes constantly perform in a domain highly
valued by the majority of children and are viewed by many as
representing a frame of reference for sporting reality.
Sport Heroes Influence
Smith (1974) has defined sport heroes as individuals (and possibly
groups of individuals) who provide actors with a frame of reference that
serves to facilitate judgements about various issues. Role modelling has
been shown to be a major influence upon aggression and attitudinal
outcome variables (e.g., Bandura, 1973; Russell, 1979; Smith, 1974) and
has also been theorised (Russell, 1979) to have potential for value and
belief mediation. Indeed, the importance of observational learning via
socialisation processes has long been recognised (e.g., Bandura, 1969).
In accordance with a central tenet of Bandura's (1973) role
modelling hypothesis, role models most highly attended to, and therefore
most likely to influence behaviour, are those in possession of high
status in prestige, power, and competence hierarchies. Smith (1974) has
additionally contended that even when observers dislike the attributes
of role models, providing the model appears highly successful in
obtaining social and material rewards, imitation and influence are
probable.
One area of competence that is particularly highly valued by the
majority of children is athletic ability (Coie, Dodge, & Kupersmidt,
1991; Evans & Roberts, 1987). Hence, in accordance with
Bandura's assumptions, the elite and professional athletes of
contemporary society are likely to be highly attended to, and well
regarded by children. Youngsters especially are therefore likely to be
impressionable and open to influence by society's sporting role
models, that influence possibly extending to affect the quality of their
intellectual, moral, emotional, and psychosocial development (Russell,
1979).
One recent perspective (i.e., Carr, Weigand, & Jones, 2000),
has applied research (e.g., Perse, 1990; Perse & Rubin, 1989; Rubin
& McHugh, 1987; Rubin, Perse, & Powell, 1985) examining
children's interaction with media heroes to a sporting context.
From this perspective it is suggested, that through processes of
parasocial interaction and wishful identification (Perse & Rubin,
1989; Rubin & McHugh, 1987), youngsters might be able to deduce an
opinion of the achievement orientations that their sport heroes hold. In
contexts outside of sport, media heroes have certainly been demonstrated
to influence the values and beliefs adopted by youngsters (e.g., Perse,
1990; Rubin & Perse, 1987; Rubin et al., 1985).
Carr et al. (2000) extended research on the motivational climate
and suggested that goal orientations might also extend from sporting
heroes to young admirers. These researchers assessed 150 children and
adolescents' perceptions of their sporting heroes' achievement
orientations, along with their personal goal orientations for sport
(i.e., soccer/netball). Results indicated that youngsters'
perceptions of a higher mastery-orientation emphasised by heroes
predicted higher personal task orientation for sport. In addition,
perceptions of higher ego orientation and lower mastery-orientation
emphasised by heroes predicted higher personal ego orientation for
sport. However, a link between youngsters' goal orientations for PE
and their perceptions of their heroes' goal orientations has yet to
be examined.
Teachers, Peers, and Sport Heroes' Influence on PE Goal
Orientations
White, Kavussanu, and Guest (1998) have stressed the need for
researchers to begin to examine how a combination of significant others
might relate to youngsters' personal goal orientations for PE. From
this viewpoint it is contended that existing research has centered on
singularly addressing the influence of various social agents without
examining some of the possible combinations of significant others. The
purpose of this study was threefold. Firstly, this study attempted to
broaden existing research on PE goal orientations and the influence of
significant others by exploring individual differences in
children's PE goal orientations and perceptions of the motivational
climate emphasised by PE teachers, peers, and sport heroes. Secondly,
this study explored the relative influence of the teacher, peer, and
sport hero motivational climate on youngsters' PE goal
orientations. Thirdly, this study evaluated a context (PE) which has
undergone major changes since the implementation of the National
Curriculum, but h as received little research attention with regards to
goal orientations and motivational climate.
Drawing upon previous contentions (e.g., Ames, 1984; Fox et al.,
1994; Harwood, 2000; White, 1998) goal orientations were examined using
a goal profile approach. From this perspective it is suggested that goal
orientations are independent constructs (Harwood, 2000) and hence the
effect of one goal orientation cannot be predicted without consideration
of the other. It was hypothesised that children with a high task/low ego
goal profile for PE would perceive the climate emphasised by significant
others to be more task involving. In contrast, children with a high
ego/low task profile were hypothesised to internalise perceptions of a
more ego involving climate from significant others. No predictions were
made concerning high task/high ego and low task/low ego profile groups
due to the exploratory nature of this study and to the fact that the
literature has rarely considered these groups. In addition, no
hypotheses were made regarding the relative influence of the socialisers
as the literature has not sufficiently addressed this area of research.
Method
Participants
Participants were 266 pupils attending schools in the east of
England. Participants had an age range of 11-15 years. There were 145
males (M = 12.4, SD = .05) and 121 females (M = 13.0, SD = .89). A
breakdown of the sample revealed an almost exclusively Caucasian group
(97%).
Measures
Peer and Teacher Influence. In order to assess the influence of
peers and teachers, the PE Class Climate Scale (PECCS; Biddle, Cury,
Goudas, Sarazin, Famose, & Durand, 1995) was employed in its
original format. The PECCS is designed to assess the perception children
have of the motivational climate in PE classes and consists of subscales
designed to measure children's perceptions of the climate initiated
by the teacher and pupils.
To measure teacher influence, the two subscales (7 items) assessing
children's perceptions of the teacher initiated motivational
climate were employed. The first subscale measured teacher promotion of
a learning oriented climate and consisted of four items. The second
subscale measured teacher promotion of a comparison oriented climate and
consisted of three items. Children responded to the stem "In my PE
lessons..." and answered each item of the learning oriented (e.g.,
"The PE teacher is pleased when each pupil learns something
new") and comparison oriented (e.g., "The PE teacher only
bothers with those who are good at sport") subscales on a 5-point
Likert-type scale ranging from I (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Strongly
Agree).
To assess peer influence, the subscales (8 items) of the PECCS
assessing children's perceptions of the pupil initiated
motivational climate were employed. The first subscale measured peer
promotion of a learning oriented climate and consisted of five items
(e.g., "My friends learn new things and they feel pleased").
The second subscale measured peer promotion of a comparison oriented
climate and consisted of three items (e.g., "My friends try to do
better than each other"). The terminology within items was adapted
to reflect measurement of peer influence (my friends), as opposed to
pupil influence (the pupils). Items were answered in the above described
format (e.g., children responded to the stem "In my PE
lessons..." and answered each item on a similar 5-point Likert-type
scale) and a mean score was calculated for each subscale. Biddle et al.
(1995) have demonstrated the PECCS to display adequate factorial structure, good internal consistency, and acceptable test-retest
reliability.
Sport Heroes Influence. In order to assess this construct, a
measure constructed by Carr et at. (2000) to assess youngsters'
perceptions of the achievement orientation that their sporting heroes
aspire to was adapted. This measure was based on the assumption that
children and adolescents can deduce the achievement orientation that
their heroes aspire to based on the kind of indirect, parasocial
interaction that children have been shown to engage in with their
sporting heroes (see Carr et al., 2000). Therefore, items reflect
perceptions of a specific achievement orientation aspired to by the hero
that could be deducted by children from the kind of interaction that
children have with their sporting heroes (i.e., watching them play on
TV, seeing interviews, hearing commentary, reading newspapers and
magazines, etc.). An example of such an item is "My sporting hero
seems to be most pleased when he/she beats everyone else."
The initial measure (see Carr et al., 2000) consisted of three
subscales reflecting children's perceptions of: (a) mastery
orientation in their heroes (12 items), (b) ego orientation in their
heroes (7 items), and (c) heroes being primarily concerned with winning
(5 items). However, for the purpose of this study one major change was
made to this measure.
It was considered that the subscale assessing heroes' concern
with winning was too synonymous with the subscale assessing heroes'
ego orientation. Additionally the face validity of the items in this
subscale was deemed questionable. Hence, this subscale was omitted from
this study. The two subscales assessing heroes' mastery and ego
orientation (i.e., 19 items) were retained in their original format.
Youngsters therefore responded to 19 items for this measure. Twelve
items assessed perceptions of a mastery orientation in heroes (e.g.,
"It seems like his/her ultimate goal is to do his/her best")
and 7 items assessed perceptions of an ego orientation in heroes (e.g.,
"It seems like he/she would feel bad if he/she doesn't do as
well as others"). All items were assessed on a 5-point Likert-type
scale ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree).
Personal Goal Orientations. Personal goal orientations were
assessed using the 13-item Task and Ego Orientation in Sport
Questionnnaire (TEOSQ; Duda, 1993). Specifically, the TEOSQ requires
participants to think of when they felt most successful in
"sport" (for the current study, this was changed to
"PE") and then respond to seven items reflecting task
involvement (e.g., "I feel most successful in PE when I learn
something new") and six items reflecting ego involvement (e.g.,
"I feel most successful in PE when I can do better than my
friends"). Participants responded on a S-point Likert-type scale
ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree). The TEOSQ is
scored by calculating a mean score for each of the two subscales. Higher
scores on each subscale represent a greater internalization of that goal
orientation. Additionally, Boyd and Callaghan (1994) have found the
TEOSQ to demonstrate adequate psychometric properties in studies of
children and much evidence exists to demonstrate favourable reliab ility
and validity (Duda & Whitehead, 1998).
Procedures
After obtaining informed consent and written permission from
schools and teachers, questionnaires were administered to participants.
Administration was completed in small groups following regular PE
lessons and without the presence of the PE teacher. Respondents typically took between 15-20 minutes to complete the measures.
Additionally, participants were reminded that their participation was
voluntary and that all responses would remain anonymous.
Results
Statistical Analyses
An exploratory factor analysis was conducted on the items designed
to measure youngsters' perceptions of their sport heroes'
achievement orientation. In order to determine whether children with
different goal profiles for PE internalised different perceptions of
teacher and peer motivational climates and different perceptions of
their sport hero's achievement orientations a one-way MANOVA was
conducted. The teacher, peer, and hero variables served as the dependent
variables and goal profiles served as the independent variable. Multiple
regression analyses were conducted to determine whether task and
ego-orientation are better predicted by teacher, peer, or sport hero
variables.
Exploratory Factor Analysis
An Exploratory Factor Analysis (with Quartimax rotation) examined
the structure of the 19 items designed to measure youngsters'
perceptions of their sporting hero's achievement orientation. A
factor weight of 0.40 was required for an item to be considered to have
loaded on a particular factor. Two factors with eigenvalues greater than
one emerged, accounting for 40% of the total variance (see Table 1).
Twelve items, that appeared to reflect children's perceptions of a
mastery orientation in their hero, loaded on Factor I (Mastery
Orientation). Factor 2 (Ego Orientation) comprised seven items capturing
a perception of the hero being primarily ego orientated (i.e., concerned
with winning and the demonstration of ability).
Table 1
Factor Analysis of the Items to Measure Children's Perceptions of
Achievement Orientation in Sporting Heroes (N = 266)
Items Mastery Orientation
It seems like improving after .52
trying hard is very important to
him/her.
I think he/she seems to always try .49
to improve his/her skills.
I think that he/she seems to think .43
that mistakes are just part of
learning.
It seems like his/her ultimate .59
goal is to do his/her best.
It seems like trying his/her best .57
would be more important than
winning or losing.
It seems like learning new things .61
would be most important to
him/her.
It seems like progressing his/her .64
skills after trying hard would be
most important to him/her.
It seems like improving his/her .56
skills would be most important
to him/her.
It seems like doing his/her best .56
is most important to him/her.
It seems like trying hard is most .67
important to him/her.
It seems like trying to achieve .51
his/her personal goals would be
most important to him/her.
It seems like performing to the best of his/her .47
ability would be most important to him/her.
It seems like he/she dislikes losing because
It seems like he/she would feel bad if he/she
doesn't do as well as others.
I think that he/she seems to dislike doing
things he/she is not good at in front of others.
It seems like doing well without trying
is most important to him/her.
It seems like winning without trying hard
is most important to him/her.
It seems like scoring more points/goals than
others is most important to him/her.
It seems like beating opponents is very
important to him/her.
Items Ego Orientation
It seems like improving after
trying hard is very important to
him/her.
I think he/she seems to always try
to improve his/her skills.
I think that he/she seems to think
that mistakes are just part of
learning.
It seems like his/her ultimate
goal is to do his/her best.
It seems like trying his/her best
would be more important than
winning or losing.
It seems like learning new things
would be most important to
him/her.
It seems like progressing his/her
skills after trying hard would be
most important to him/her.
It seems like improving his/her
skills would be most important
to him/her.
It seems like doing his/her best
is most important to him/her.
It seems like trying hard is most
important to him/her.
It seems like trying to achieve
his/her personal goals would be
most important to him/her.
It seems like performing to the best of his/her
ability would be most important to him/her.
It seems like he/she dislikes losing because .56
It seems like he/she would feel bad if he/she .45
doesn't do as well as others.
I think that he/she seems to dislike doing .58
things he/she is not good at in front of others.
It seems like doing well without trying .61
is most important to him/her.
It seems like winning without trying hard .66
is most important to him/her.
It seems like scoring more points/goals than .63
others is most important to him/her.
It seems like beating opponents is very .56
important to him/her.
Note: Only loadings > .40 are included.
Internal Consistency
The internal consistency of all subscales implemented within this
study was examined using Cronbach's (1951) alpha coefficient. Table
2 displays the alpha coefficients for all variables. The subscale
assessing perceptions of teacher promotion of a comparison oriented
climate attained satisfactory internal consistency (i.e.,> 0.7,
Nunnally, 1978) with the deletion of one item ("In my PE lessons
the PE teacher is particularly pleased with those who win"). All
other subscales demonstrated adequate internally consistency.
Table 2
Internal Consistency of Subscales
Variable Alpha Coefficient
Task orientation .83
Ego orientation .85
Peer promotion of learning .72
Peer promotion of comparison .74
Teacher promotion of learning .80
Teacher promotion of comparison .73
Sporting hero mastery orientation .80
Sporting hero ego orientation .73
Goal Profile Groups
Previous research (e.g., Fox et al., 1994; White, 1998) has
advocated a goal profile approach to the examination of goal
orientations. From this perspective it is suggested that to separately
examine the effects of task and ego orientation is illogical since both
goal orientations must be taken into account to adequately address their
consequences. A goal profile approach was therefore adopted in this
study. Researchers (e.g., Fox et al., 1994; White, 1998) have previously
incorporated mean splits of task and ego orientation scores to divide
samples into four goal profile groups because there are no published,
population-specific norms for the TEOSQ. However, Treasure (2000) has
advocated checking mean and median similarity before resorting to mean
splits. Mean and median values for task and ego orientation did not
appear to differ substantially (M task orientation = 3.95, median =
4.00; M ego orientation = 3.34, median = 3.50). Mean splits of task and
ego orientation scores were therefore conducted to divid e the sample
into four goal profile groups of high task/low ego, high task/high ego,
low task/high ego, and low task/low ago.
Orthogonality of the TEOSQ
A simple bivariate correlation analysis was conducted to determine
the orthogonality of the TEOSQ. Results indicated a weak,
non-significant correlation between task and ego orientation (r = .19).
This confirmed the contention that the two goal orientations are
independent constructs. Hence, one may have a dominant goal orientation
or be high, or low, in both orientations.
Goal Profile Differences in Perceptions of Motivational Climate
from Significant Others
A one-way MANOVA was conducted to test for goal profile differences
in perceptions of the motivational climate emphasised by teachers and
peers, and perceptions of the achievement orientations emphasised by
sports heroes. Keppel (1991) has suggested that for MANOVA to hold true
(i.e., minimise risk of Type I error) then multivariate normality and
homogeneity of variance-covariance matrices should be evident. An
examination of the Fmax (see Keppel, 1991) ratios for within-group
variances in this study revealed that there was no problem with
heterogeneity within the data set. However, the data did display a
certain degree of skewness. In this situation, to reduce the risk of
committing a Type I error, Keppel (1991) suggests reducing the
significance level. Hence, the significance level for this analysis was
reduced from .05 to .025. The MANOVA revealed a significant main effect
for goal profiles (Wilks's lambda = .53, F( 18, 724) = 7.26,p
.000).
A Bonferroni-type correction was required when examining the
univariate F values, in accordance with the number of dependent
variables. The new significance level was therefore set at .006
(.025/6). Univariate analyses revealed significant differences among
profile groups. The means across all dependent variables for the four
profile groups, together with F values and significance of F are
displayed in Table 3. Specifically, ANOVA and Scheffe post hoc tests
indicated the following:
For teachers: (a) youngsters with a low task/low ego orientation
had lower perceptions of teacher promotion of a learning climate than
high task/high ego, high task/low ego, and low task/high ego oriented
youngsters, and (b) youngsters with a high task/low ego orientation had
lower perceptions of teacher promotion of a comparison climate than low
task/high ego oriented youngsters.
For peers: (a) youngsters with either high task/high ego or high
task/low ego goal orientations had higher perceptions of peer promotion
of learning climates than youngsters with low task/high ego and low
task/low ego goal orientations, and (b) youngsters with high task/high
ego orientation and low task/high ego orientation had higher perceptions
of promotion of comparison in peers than youngsters with high task/low
ego orientation and low task/low ego orientation.
For sporting heroes: (a) youngsters with high task/high ego and
high task/low ego goal orientations had higher perceptions of mastery
orientation in their heroes than youngsters with low task/low ego goal
orientations, and (b) youngsters with high task/low ego orientation had
lower perceptions of an ego orientation in heroes than youngsters with
high task/high ego orientation and low task/high ego orientation.
Table 3
Mean Differences Between Coal Profiles on Perceptions of Teacher, Peer,
and Hero Climate
High task/ High task/ Low task/
high ego (1) low ego (2) high ego (3)
Variable M SD M SD M
Peer learning 4.15 .55 4.06 .69 3.64
Peer 3.93 .78 2.97 1.22 3.67
Comparison
Teacher 4.34 .64 4.35 .60 3.97
learning
Teacher 3.02 1.27 2.47 1.28 3.29
Comparison
Hero mastery 4.04 .51 4.13 .40 3.85
Hero ego 3.31 .75 2.73 .64 3.28
Low task/ Low task/
high ego (3) low ego (4)
Variable SD M SD F value (5) eff size
Peer learning .67 3.38 .66 23.27 (*) .21
Peer .74 3.07 .92 18.20 (*) .18
Comparison
Teacher .68 3.47 .99 21.68 (*) .20
learning
Teacher .87 2.93 1.20 4.63 .05
Comparison
Hero mastery .57 3.53 .60 17.23 (*) .18
Hero ego .71 2.98 .63 10.25 (*) .11
Note: Means represent subject's scores on a 5-point Likert-type scale
for each variable, ranging from 1 (low) through 5 (high).
(1)n = 94
(2)n = 6
(3)n = 43
(4)n = 68
(5)(*)p < .004
Multiple Regression Analyses
In order to determine which of the social variables (i.e., teacher,
peer, and hero variables) best predicted youngsters' task and ego
orientation for PE regression analyses were conducted. We used the test
method of SPSS statistical program which calculates the amount of unique
variance in criterion variables accounted for by the predictor variables regardless of their order of entry into the equation.
Table 4 displays the regression results for task orientation.
Perceptions of a learning climate emphasised by teachers emerged as the
most important predictor of task orientation accounting for 32% of the
variance. In addition, perceptions of a learning climate from peers
accounted for 8% of the variance in task orientation and perceptions of
a mastery orientation in heroes accounted for a further 3%.
Table 5 displays the regression results for ego orientation.
Perceptions of a comparison climate from peers emerged as the most
important predictors of ego orientation accounting for 30% of the
variance. In addition, perceptions of an ego orientation in heroes
accounted for 7% of the variance in ego orientation and perceptions of a
mastery orientation in heroes accounted for a further 2%.
Table 4
Stepwise Regression Analyses for Task Orientation
Variable Beta R-Square Change p
1 Teacher Comparison .57 .32 .000
2 Peer Learning .35 .09 .000
3 Hero Mastery .18 .02 .000
4 Hero Ego .07 .00 ns
5 Peer Comparison -.02 .00 ns
6 Teacher Comparison -.02 .00 ns
Note: Total R-Square = 0.44,F (6,258) = 33.67, p<0.001
Table 5
Stepwise Regression Analyses for Ego Orientation
Variable Beta R-Square Change P
1 Peer Comparison .54 .30 .000
2 Hero Ego .29 .07 .000
3 Hero Mastery .09 .01 ns
4 Teacher Learning .06 .00 ns
5 Peer Learning .04 .00 ns
6 Teacher Comparison .03 .00 ns
Note: Total R-Square = 0.38, F(6,258) 26.73, p<0.001
Discussion
In the context of achievement goal theory (Nicholls, 1989) the
first purpose of this study was to explore how differences in
youngsters' goal profiles for PE corresponded to differences in
perceptions of the motivational climate emphasised by teachers, peers,
and sporting heroes. It was hypothesised that children with a high
task/low ego profile for PE would perceive the climate emphasised by
significant others to be more task involving. In contrast, children with
a high ego/low task profile were hypothesised to internalise perceptions
of a more ego involving climate from significant others.
Results supported these hypotheses. Specifically, children with
high task/low ego and high task/high ego profiles had higher perceptions
of task involving climates emphasised by their teachers and peers than
the other two profile groups. Additionally, high task/low ego and high
task/high ego children also had higher perceptions of a task involved
climate created by teachers, and higher perceptions of an ego
orientation in sporting heroes than the low task/ low ego profile group.
Also in support of hypotheses, results indicated children with low
task/high ego and high task/high ego profiles had higher perceptions of
ego involving climates in teachers and peers than children with high
task/low ego and low task/low ego profiles. Additionally, low task/high
ego and high task/high ego children had higher perceptions of an ego
orientation in sporting heroes than children with a high task/low ego
profile.
Previous researchers (e.g., Duda, 1997; Fox et al., 1994; Harwood,
2000; White, 1998) have supported the use of a goal profile approach to
research grounded in an achievement goal theory framework. From this
perspective, for example, Harwood (2000) has contended that to
separately predict the psychological effects of task and ego orientation
alone tells one little about practical reality. Harwood (2000) likens
this process to predicting the results of a doubles match by looking at
only one of the pairing. Hence, it is argued that to separately analyse the cognitive-behavioural connotations of either task or ego
orientations as a single construct might be misleading as one does not
take into consideration the other half of an orthogonal unit.
However, results of this study do not provide strong support
advocating the use of a goal profile approach to the investigation of
links between motivational climates emphasised by significant others and
personal goal orientations. For example, these results invariably demonstrated that a high task orientation, regardless of whether
accompanied by a high, or low, ego orientation, related to higher
perceptions of task involving climates emphasised by significant others.
That is, levels of ego orientation appeared to have no moderating effect
on the relationship between task orientation and perceptions of the
motivational climate from significant others. Similarly, a high ego
orientation, whether accompanied by a high, or low, task orientation,
related to higher perceptions of an ego involving climate emphasised by
significant others (with the exception of teachers). Hence, levels of
task orientation appeared to have no moderating effect on the
relationship between ego orientation and motivational climates.
It is therefore contended that when considering the connotations of
goal orientations a goal profile approach is certainly merited (see
Duda, 1997; Fox et al., 1994; Harwood, 2000; White, 1998). However, when
investigating conceptual antecedents of goal orientations, such as the
motivational climate, it would appear less essential to incorporate goal
profiles. For example, to manipulate Harwood's (2000) above
analogy, when predicting the result of a doubles match it is
inappropriate to examine only one of the pairing. Yet when exploring the
abilities of the two players in a doubles team, one might equally expect
the antecedents of each player's ability to originate from
different sources. Hence, in examining the specific origins of goal
orientation components, it would appear feasible to separately examine
the predictors of task and ego orientation. Despite this, the results of
this study provided a strong indication of the important role that a
variety of significant others play in the development of youngst
ers' goal orientations.
On this note, it was the second purpose of this study to explore
the relative influence that teachers, peers, and sport heroes have on
youngsters' task and ego orientation for PE. No hypotheses were
made for this aspect of the study as there is scarce literature
available in this area of research.
Firstly, results indicated that the peer group made the strongest
contribution to the prediction of youngsters' ego orientation.
Specifically, youngsters were more likely to develop a higher ego
orientation if they perceived that peers' emphasised a higher
comparison oriented climate in PE. The peer groups' promotion of a
learning climate also made a strong positive contribution to the
prediction of task orientation. However, teacher promotion of a learning
climate in PE lessons made the strongest contribution to predicting
youngsters' task orientation.
The potentially maladaptive motivational consequences that a high
level of ego orientation can have for youngsters (see Duda, 1996 for a
review) when unaccompanied by a high level of task orientation has been
outlined (Duda, 1997). Duda (1997) has suggested that development of a
high task orientation is an essential accompaniment to youngsters'
levels of ego orientation. She contends that a task orientation is
beneficial as individuals may be motivated "over the long
haul" as "they have a strong task orientation to fall back on
when their sense of normative ability is in jeopardy" (p.309).
Consequently, results of this study would perhaps suggest that
teachers, as the strongest predictor of youngsters' task
orientation for PE, are an important social agent on which to target
interventions designed to induce a higher dispositional level of task
orientation in youngsters. Interventions aimed at youngsters might
therefore be most effective if transmitted via PE teachers.
On this note, Epstein (1989) has coined the TARGET acronym which,
she contends, can be sufficiently manipulated to influence
children's perceptions of the motivational climate within
classrooms. The TARGET acronym represents the six structural features of
task, authority, reward, grouping, evaluation, and timing structures of
the achievement context. Epstein (1989) has suggested that each of the
TARGET constructs can be effectively manipulated by teachers to convey
to children a strong mastery oriented (task oriented) class climate
(readers are referred to Treasure & Roberts, 1995, for a thorough
review of this process). Treasure (1993) has provided support for the
successful manipulation of the PE motivational climate using the
constructs of the TARGET acronym. Specifically, if children perceived
the constructs of the TARGET acronym to be mastery oriented, they
internalised higher task orientation for PE. Hence, it is possible to
disseminate to PE teachers information on how to structure pedagogical pattern s so as to maximise the quality of youngsters' PE
motivation. This study suggests that such interventions aimed at PE
teachers have the greatest potential to influence the development of
youngsters' PE task orientation.
Additionally, results suggested that youngsters' sport heroes
emerged as predictors of both their task and ego orientation for PE.
Specifically, if youngsters' perceived a higher mastery orientation
in their hero they internalised a higher task orientation and if they
perceived a higher ego orientation in their hero they internalised a
higher ego orientation. Intervention efforts might therefore also be
directed at youngsters' sport heroes Carr et al. (2000) have
suggested that attempting to effect an alteration in general media
portrayal of sport heroes would seem to provide a headache to even the
keenest of researchers. These researchers have suggested that it may be
more beneficial to remove the media transmission of heroes to youths by
enabling children to come into direct contact with sporting heroes in
physical activity contexts. This way, heroes can be directly responsible
for transmitting a motivational climate to youths that is unaffected by
media portrayal and bias.
Accordingly, the English Sports Council has recently launched the
"Sporting Ambassadors" initiative (English Sports Council,
1998). This initiative is aimed at youngsters aged 4-18 years and
provides opportunities for successful sportsmen and women to visit
schools and youth sport organisations in order to interact with children
in physical activity contexts. Among the objectives of this initiative
is for sporting heroes to help to develop young peoples' physical
activity motivation, to serve as role models, and to help youngsters
maintain a keen interest and enthusiasm in physical activity and other
school studies. Such objectives will likely be achieved by sporting
heroes working alongside physical educators and practitioners,
interacting with children in physical activity contexts, coaching
youngsters, lecturing youngsters, and a variety of other methods
(English Sports Council, 1998).
In the context of this investigation, such an initiative would
appear to have great potential for sporting heroes to positively
influence the achievement motivation of youngsters in sport and physical
activity. Through careful structuring of the climate that they convey to
youngsters, sporting heroes involved in the "Sporting
Ambassadors" initiative have the potential to emphasise a higher
task involved climate and to positively influence the quality of
youngsters' achievement motivation on a direct level.
To conclude, we found that significant others played a substantial
role in mediating children's goal orientations for PE. However,
results did not appear to advocate the implementation of a goal profile
approach to examining the antecedents of goal orientations. In addition,
results indicated that certain social agents have a larger relative
influence than others in explaining children's goal orientations
and this may have important implications for structuring intervention
efforts.
References
Ames, C. (1984). Conceptions of motivation within competitive and
non competitive goal structure. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.), Self-related
cognitions in anxiety and motivation (pp-229-246). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Ames, C. (1992). Motivational climate and achievement related
patterns In: G. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in Sport and exercise,
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 1992, pp. 161-176.
Ames, C. & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the
classroom: Student's learning strategies and motivation processes.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 260-267.
Bandura, A. (1969). Social learning theory of identificatory
processes: In: D.A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of Socialization Theory and
Research, pp. 213-262, Chicago: Rand Mcnally.
Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis.
Englewood Cliffs, N.J.Prentice-Hall.
Biddle, S., Cury, F.,Goudas, M., Sarrazin, P., Famose, J., P.,
& Durand, M. (1995). Development of scales to measure perceived
physical education class climate: a cross national project. British
Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 341-358.
Boyd, M., & Callaghan, J. (1994). Task and ego perspectives in
organised youth sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 22,
411-424.
Carr, S., Weigand, D.A., & Hussey, W. (1999). The relative
influence of parents, teachers, and peers on children's and
adolescent's achievement and intrinsic motivation and perceived
competence in PE. Journal of Sport Pedagogy, 5:1,28-51.
Carr, S., Weigand, D.A., & Jones, J. (2000). The relative
influence of parents, peers, and sporting heroes on goal orientations of
children and adolescents in sport. Journal of Sport Pedagogy, 6:2,34-54.
Coie, J.D., Dodge, K.A., & Kupersmidt, J. (1991). Peer group
behaviour and social status. In S.R. Asher and J.D. Coie (Eds.), Peer
Rejection in Childhood (pp. 17-59). New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Cronbach, L.J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and internal structure of
tests. Psychometrika, 16,297-3.33.
Duda, J.L., (1987). Toward a developmental theory of achievement in
sport. Journal of Sport Psychology, 9, 130-145.
Duda, J.L. (1992). Motivation in sport settings: A goal perspective
approach. In G.C. Roberts (Ed), Motivation in Sport and Exercise.
Champaign, IL, Human Kinetics, pp 57-91.
Duda, J. (1993). Goals: A social cognitive approach to the study of
motivation in sport. In: R.N. Singer,-.M. Murphey, & L.K. Tennant
(Eds.). Handbook of Research in Sport Psychology (pp. 421-436).
Duda, J.L. (1996). Maximising motivation in sport and physical
education among children and adolescents: The case for greater task
involvement. Quest, 48, 290-302.
Duda, J.L. (1997). Perpetuating myths: A response to Hardy's
1996 Coleman Griffith Address. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 9,
307-313.
Duda, J.L., & Whitehead, J. (1998). Measurement of goal
perspectives in the physical domain. In J. Duda (Ed.), Advances in Sport
and Exercise Psychology Measures. Morgantown, WV: FIT Press.
Dweck, C.S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning.
American Psychologist, 4, 1048-1048.
Elliott, E.S., & Dweck, C.S. (1988). Goals. An approach to
motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 54, 5-12.
English Sports Council. (1998). Sporting Ambassadors: A brief guide
to the English Sports Council's new Sporting Ambassadors scheme.
JWL ltd. London.
Epstein, J. (1989). Family structures and student motivation: A
developmental perspective. In C. Ames & R. Ames (Eds.), Research on
motivation in education (Vol. 3, pp. 259-295). New York: Academic Press.
Evans, J., & Roberts G.C. (1987). Physical competence and the
development of children's peer relations. Quest, 39,23-35.
Fox, K., Goudas, M., Biddle, S., Duda, J., & Armstrong, N.
(1994). Children's task and ego goal profiles in sport. British
Journal of Educational Psychology, 64, 253-261.
Goudas, M. & Biddle, S. (1994). Perceived motivational climate
and intrinsic motivation in school physical education classes. European
Journal of Psychology of Education, 9, 241-250.
Goudas, M., Biddle, S., Fox, K., & Underwood, M. (1995). It
aint what you do its the way that you do it! Teaching style affects
children's motivation in track and field lessons. The Sport
Psychologist, 9, 254-264.
Harwood, C.G. (2000). Point, Counter-Point: Goal Profiling.
Association for the Advancement of Applied Sport Psychology Newsletter,
Vol. 15, No. 1, 7-12.
Keppel, G. (1991). Design and Analysis: A Researcher's
Handbook, third edition, Prentice-Hall, NJ.
Maehr, M.L., & Nicholls, J.G. (1980). Culture and achievement
motivation: A second look. In N. Warren (Ed.), Studies in cross-cultural
psychology: Vol. 3, (pp. 221-267). New York: Academic Press.
Nicholls, J.G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of
ability, subjective, experience, task choice, and performance.
Psychological review, 91, 328-346.
Nicholls, J.G. (1989). The Competitive Ethos and Democratic
Education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric Theory. New York, Mcgraw-Hill.
Papaioannou, A. (1995) Differential perceptual and motivational
patterns when different goals are adopted. Journal of Sport &
Exercise Psychology, 17, 18-34.
Papaioannou. A. (1994). Development of a questionnaire to measure
achievement orientations in physical education. Research Quarterly for
Exercise and Sport, 65, 11-20.
Perse, E.M. (1990). Media involvement and local news effects.
Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 34, 17-36.
Perse, E.M., & Rubin, R.B. (1989). Attribution in social and
parasocial relationships. Communication Research, 16, 59-77.
Roberts, G.C. (1992). Motivation in Sport and Exercise: Conceptual
Constraints and Convergence. In G.C. Roberts (Ed), Motivation in Sport
and Exercise. Champaign, IL, Human Kinetics, pp 3-30.
Rubin, R.B., & Mchugh, M.P. (1987). Development of parasocial
interaction relationships. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media,
31, 279-292.
Rubin, R.B. & Perse, E.M. (1987). Audience activity and soap
opera involvement: A uses and effects investigation. Human Communication
Research, 14, 246-268.
Rubin, R.B., Perse, E.M., & Powell, R.A. (1985). Loneliness,
parasocial interaction, and local television news viewing. Human
Communication Research, 12, 155-180.
Russell, G.W. (1979). Hero selection by Canadian ice hockey players: Skill or aggression? Canadian Journal of Applied Sport
Sciences, 4, 309-313.
Sallis, J.F., Simons-Morton, B., Stone, E., Corbin, C., Epstein,
L.H., Faucette, N., Ianotti, R., Killen, J., Klesges, R., Petray, C.,
Rowland, T., & Taylor, W. (1992). Determinants of physical activity
and intervention in youth. Medicine and Science in Sport and Exercise,
24,248-257.
Smith, M.D. (1974). Significant others' influence on the
assaultive behaviour of young hockey players. International Review of
Sport Sociology 3, 45-58.
Smith, G.J. (1976). An Examination of the Phenomenon of Sports Hero
Worship. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sciences, 1, 259-271.
Solmon, M.A., & Lee, A.M. (1996) Practice schedule effects on
the performance and learning of low- and high-skilled students: An
applied study. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 15, 136-150.
Treasure, D. (1993). A social-cognitive approach to understanding
children achievement behavior, cognitions, and affect in competitive
sport. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at
Urbana-Champaign.
Treasure, D. (2000). Point, Counter-Point: Goal Profiling.
Association for the Advancement of Applied Sport Psychology Newsletter,
Vol. 15, No. 1, 7-12.
Treasure, D., & Roberts, G.C. (1995). Applications of
achievement goal theory to physical education: Implications for
enhancing motivation. Quest, 47, 1-14.
Van Wersch, A., Trew, K., & Turner, I. (1992). Post-primary
school pupils' interest in physical education: Age and gender
differences. British Journal of Educational Psychology 62, 56-72.
Walling, M.D., & Duda, J.L. (1995). Goals and their
associations with beliefs about success in and perceptions of the
purposes of physical education. Journal of Teaching in Physical
Education, 14, 140-156.
White, S.A. (1998). Adolescent goal profiles, perceptions of the
parent-initiated motivational climate and competitive trait anxiety. The
Sport Psychologist, 12, 16-33.
White, S.A., Kavassanu, M., & Guest, 5. (1998). Goal
orientations and perceptions of the motivational climate created by
significant others. European Journal of Physical Education, 3,212-228.