The relationship of physical self-perceptions and goal orientations to intrinsic motivation for exercise.
Boyd, Michael P. ; Weinmann, Carol ; Yin, Zenong 等
The purpose of this study was to examine the multivariate relationship between physical self-perceptions/goal orientations and
intrinsic motivation for exercise. Female undergraduates (N= 261)
enrolled in physical activity classes completed several self-report
measures assessing physical self-perceptions, task and ego goal
orientations for exercise, and intrinsic motivation for exercise. Factor
analysis initially confirmed stable fact or structures for all the
measured variables. Subsequent canonical correlation analysis revealed a
significant and meaningful relationship between a pair of canonical variates incorporating physical self-perceptions and task/ego
orientations as predictor variables and indices of intrinsic motivation
for exercise as dependent measures. Results revealed that a high
positive loading on self-perceptions of physical condition, moderately
positive loadings on perceptions of sports competence, physical
strength, and a task orientation for exercise, corresponded to a set of
intrinsic mo tivation criterion variables composed of
interest/enjoyment, perceived competence, effort/importance, and
tension/pressure for exercise. Ego orientation for exercise, however,
failed to significantly contribute to the multivariate relationship.
Discussion highlights the notion, that among the present sample.
self-perceptions of competence in the physical domain, and a task
orientation for exercise, are robust correlates of intrinsic motivation
for exercise.
The importance of cardiovascular fitness to both physical and
psychological health is well-documented (Bouchard, Shepard, &
Stephens, 1993). Physical benefits of aerobic training include decreases
in blood pressure, cholesterol levels, triglycerides, as well as resting
heart rate (Hoeger & Hoeger, 1998). Psychological benefits for those
who engage in regular aerobic exercise include reduction of both anxiety
and depression (Craft & Landers, 1998; Petruzzello, Landers,
Hatfield, Kubitz, & Salazar, 1991). Unfortunately, it is estimated
that clearly fifty percent of those individuals who initiate an exercise
regime discontinue participation within six months (Dishman, 1988). As a
result, exercise psychologists have directed considerable research
attention to psychological constructs potentially related to both the
initiation of, and adherence to an exercise regime.
Several theories have been advanced in an attempt to explain
intrinsic motivation for exercise. These include exercise self-efficacy
(Dzewaltkowski, Noble, & Shaw, 1990; McAuley & Courneya, 1992;
McAuley, Courneya, & Lettunich, 1991; Rudolph & McAuley, 1996),
intentions to exercise (Godin, Colantonio, Davis, Shepard, & Simard,
1986; Godin & Shepard, 1986; Hausenblaus, Carron, & Mack, 1997),
and exercise self-schemata (Eastabrooks & Courneya, 1997;
Kendzierski, 1988, 1990; Yin & Boyd, 2000). However, a theoretical
approach emanating from the self-esteem literature (Harter, 1985,1986;
Marsh & Shavelson, 1985; Shavelson, Hubner, & Stanton, 1976), is
the study of physical self-perceptions. Based on Harter's (1978,
1985) competence motivation theory, this model maintains that
individuals are motivated to engage in mastery behavior that satisfies
an intrinsic need for challenge and perceptions of control. The model
has been adopted in sport and exercise psychology and underscores the
importance of the salience of c ompetence perceptions to the development
of intrinsic motivation for physical activity, behavioral choice,
intensity, and persistence (Harter, 1985). Perceptions of competence, in
the form of physical self-perceptions, serve as a measure of self-esteem
in the physical domain and have been shown to reliably predict exercise
behavior (Fox & Corbin, 1989; Marsh, 1996; Marsh & Redmayne,
1994; Sonstroem & Morgan, 1989).
A social-cognitive model for studying motivation in sport and
exercise is goal perspective theory (Ames, 1992; Ames & Archer,
1988; Dweck, 1986; Dweck & Elliott, 1993; Maehr & Braskamp,
1986; Nicholls, 1984, 1989, 1992). Nicholls (1984,1989) asserts that
individuals engage in achievement behavior in order to demonstrate
competence and avoid the demonstration of incompetence. According to Nicholls, goal orientations are linked to the manner in which
individuals construe competence and define success in a given
achievement domain. Two goal perspectives, task and ego orientation, are
related to the amount of effort expended on a given task and are also
associated to subsequent referents of intrinsic motivation such as task
choice, performance, and persistence (Nicholls, 1984, 1989). A task
orientation involves a self-referenced conception of ability where
mastery of skills, or improvement, induce perceptions of competence. An
ego orientation, rather, represents a normatively referenced conception
of ability wher e perceptions of competence are based upon the
demonstration of superior ability or performing as well as others with
less effort. This framework has been used extensively in the study of
intrinsic motivation in sport and physical activity (Duda &
Whitehead, 1998). In conjunction, goal orientation theory and physical
self-perceptions (Fox & Corbin, 1989) provide an intuitively
appealing conceptual approach to the study of intrinsic motivation for
exercise.
The study of physical self-perceptions has its origins in the
self-esteem literature (Hailer, 1985, 1986). Unlike proponents of the
unidimensional approach to the study of self-esteem and competence
advanced in the past (Marsh, Byrne, & Shavelson, 1988; Marsh &
Shavelson, 1985), contemporary theorists have advocated a
multidimensional/hierarchical approach. Global self-esteem serves at the
apex of the conceptual network in this model, and is further divided
into situation-specific, subordinate domains of self-esteem (eg.
academic, social, emotional, physical domains). Moreover, each of these
domains is further divided into subdomains of even greater specificity.
Exercise psychologists have adopted this multidimensional model in their
study of self-concept in the physical domain (Fox & Corbin, 1989;
Marsh, Richards, Johnson, Roche, & Tremayne, 1994; Sonstroem, &
Morgan, 1989).
Fox & Corbin (1989) validated the multi-level heirarchical
organization of self-perceptions in the physical domain using the
Physical Self-Perception Profile (PSPP). Self-perceptions of competence
at the domain level are assessed utilizing a physical self-worth scale.
More importantly, at the subdomain level, is the measurement of
self-perceptions of physical condition, sports competence, physical
strength, and attractive body. Correlation procedures have confirmed the
heirarchical nature of the model, and factor analysis has demonstrated
construct validity of the four-factor subdomain structure. Fox and
Corbin (1989), for instance, reported that discriminant function
analysis correctly classified males and females seventy percent of the
time into activity/non-activity groups. Furthermore, canonical
correlation indicated that loadings on the various subdomains were
associated to the type of physical activity participation.
Self-perceptions of sports competence, for example, corresponded to ball
sports activit y, whereas perceptions of physical strength and physical
condition corresponded to weight training and aerobic exercise,
respectively. Utilizing the PSPP, Sonstroem, Speliotis, and Faua (1992)
found that adult exercisers were categorized correctly 84% of the time.
The most robust predictor of aerobic exercise among both men and women
has been shown to be self perceptions of physical condition (Hayes,
Crocker, & Kowalski, 1999; Sonstroem, et al. 1992; Sonstroem, Harlow
& Joseph, 1994). Marsh and his colleagues (Marsh, 1996; Marsh &
Redmayne, 1994; Marsh, Richards, Johnson, Roche, & Tremayne, 1994;
Marsh & Sonstroem, 1995) have also provided evidence for the
multidimensional structure of self-esteem in the physical domain and
validity of the PSPP. Evidence clearly suggests that the strength of
physical self-perceptions is reliably associated to exercise behavior
and therefore theoretically should also be related to intrinsic
motivation to exercise.
Goal orientations are also proposed to exert an effect upon the
motivation to exercise. Task and ego goal orientations have been
reported to be associated to both psychological and behavioral variation
in sport and physical activity achievement contexts (Duda, 1992; Duda
& Whitehead, 1998; Nicholls, 1992; Roberts, 1992). A task
orientation in sport has been shown to be related to
mastery/cooperation, satisfaction, enjoyment, and sources of perceived
physical competence including learning and improvement as well as goal
attainment (Boyd & Yin, 1996; Duda, 1989; Duda, et al., 1995;
Treasure & Roberts, 1994; White, Duda, & Keller, 1998; Williams,
1994). A task orientation has also been found to be positively related
to the belief that effort rather than ability leads to success in sport
and physical activity (Duda, Fox, Biddle, & Armstrong, 1992; Duda
& Nicholls, 1992; Treasure & Roberts, 1994). Regarding exercise
behavior, two studies have demonstrated a task orientation to be
associated to participation in mod erate to vigorous physical exercise
among children (Dempsey, Kimiecik, & Horn, 1993; Kimiecik, Horn,
& Shurin, 1996). Research suggests that participating in challenging
tasks, exerting high levels of effort, and experiencing self-improvement
are inherent goals for those who adopt a task orientation in the
physical domain,
An ego orientation in sport and physical activity has exhibited a
strikingly different pattern of results. In sport, an ego orientation
has been found to be associated to sources of perceived physical
competence including a preference for social comparison information,
interest in the performance of others, and performance outcome,
(Williams, 1994). Ego orientation in the physical domain is also related
to cognitive anxiety prior to sport performance (Hall & Kerr, 1997;
White & Zellner, 1996), cognitive interference during performance
(Hatzigeorgiadis & Biddle, 1999), and self-handicapping in sport
(Ryska, Yin, & Boyd, 1999). Unlike those with a task orientation,
ego-oriented individuals maintain the belief that ability, ratherthan
effort, leads to success in the physical domain (Duda, 1989; Duda et
al., 1992; Duda & Nicholls, 1992; Treasure & Roberts, 1994). Not
surprisingly, an ego orientation has been reported to be unrelated to
both intrinsic motivation in sport and moderate to vigorous physical
activity (Dempsey et al., 1993; Duda, Chi, Newton, Walling, &
Catley, 1995). For those who are ego-oriented, participation in physical
activity is apparently perceived as a means toward an end, outperforming
others on tasks of normative difficulty. In summary, intrinsic
motivation for physical activity has been shown to be associated to goal
orientation, however, it has also been outlined in relation to cognitive
evaluation theory.
Cognitive evaluation theory posits that intrinsic motivation is the
result of satisfying an inherent need for perceptions of competence and
self-determination (Deci, 1975; Deci & Ryan, 1985). According to the
theory, intrinsic motivation is fostered when feelings of competence are
increased or perceptions of self-determination and internal control are
salient. Physical activity perceived to be interesting, challenging,
providing feelings of pleasure and satisfaction, or performed for its
own sake rather than external causation, induces intrinsic motivation
(Vallerand & Fortier, 1998). When achievement behavior,
characterized as volitional behavior in which people strive for success,
persist in light of failure, and experience pride of accomplishment
(Gill, 1986), such as in sport and exercise, is causally perceived to be
under one's internal control, intrinsic motivation is enhanced.
Extrinsic motivation, however, results from achievement behavior
performed for some tangible reward or to avoid negative conseq uences
rather than for the inherent pleasure it provides (Deci, 1975; Vallerand
& Fortier, 1998). Deci & Ryan (1985) contend that extrinsically
motivated individuals engage in given activity as a means toward an end,
such as outperforming others, subsequently perceiving the locus of
causality of their behavior to be linked to external causation.
Cognitive evaluation theory suggests that intrinsically motivated
exercise enthusiasts are those who possess greater perceptions of
physical competence or engage in exercise behavior for the feelings of
self-determination, perceptions of control, and satisfaction exercise
provides. Conversely, those who possess lower perceptions of competence,
exercise exclusively for external rewards, or perceive their exercise
behavior to be externally controlled, should express little intrinsic
motivation for exercise. The Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI;
McAuley, Duncan, & Tammen, 1989) has been utilized extensively to
measure intrinsic motivation for physical activity.
In accordance with competence motivation theory (Hailer, 1985), a
primary purpose of this study was to examine whether physical
self-perceptions are associated to intrinsic motivation for exercise. As
only limited research has linked goal orientations to exercise behavior,
a secondary purpose of the study was to discover whether a task
orientation and efforts toward mastery and improvement correspond to
intrinsic motivation for exercise. Moreover, ego orientation and a
preoccupation with social comparison of fitness were expected to
demonstrate little predictive value with regard to intrinsic motivation
for exercise. It was hypothesized, therefore, that physical
self-perceptions and a task orientation for exercise would be associated
to intrinsic motivation for exercise.
Method
Participants and Procedure
Undergraduate female students (N=261), age 18-43 years (M=21.6,
SD=3.9), enrolled in twelve aerobic dance classes at a university in
Southern California served as subjects in the study. Upon approval of
the study by a university institutional review board, instructors were
solicited by the principal investigator of the study for participation
of their class. Data was collected during the first 30 minutes of a
regular class session in the final two weeks of the semester.
Participants were told that their involvement in the study was voluntary
and anonymous in nature, they could withdraw at any time without
consequence, and then subsequently completed both informed consent and
several questionnaires. Each instrument was thoroughly explained,
instructions read orally by the investigator, and clarifications
addressed as well as encouraged before participants were requested to
complete each measure in an honest manner.
Measures
Physical Self-perceptions. The Physical Self-Perception Profile
(PSPP; Fox, 1990) was utilized in order to measure physical
self-concept. The PSPP was designed to quantify subdomains of
self-esteem in the physical domain in an effort to predict physical
activity levels. The instrument contains 30 items distributed among four
6-item subdomain subscales assessing self-perceptions of sports
competence, physical condition, attractive body, and physical strength,
and one scale (6 items) quantifying general physical self-worth at the
domain level. Respondents are requested to choose between two
contrasting descriptors, indicate which of the two alternatives best
describes them, and finally choose whether the descriptor is, "sort
of true for me", or, "really true for me". Items for each
subscale are scored from 1 (low) to 4 (high) and summed in order to
arrive at a total score for each subdomain. Construct and predictive
validity as well as internal reliability of the PSPP have been well
documented (Fox & Corbin, 19 89; Sonstroem et al., 1992; Sonstroem
et al., 1994; Marsh & Sonstroem, 1995).
Goal Orientations. In order to assess goal orientations for
exercise, the Perception of Success Questionnaire (POSQ; Roberts &
Balague, 1989, 1991) was adapted for an exercise context. Items were
slightly modified by altering the word "sport" in the stem to
"exercise". The POSQ is a 12-item instrument consisting of two
6-item subscales designed to quantify task and ego orientation in sport.
Each item was preceded by the phrase, "I feel most successful in
exercise when..." answered on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from
strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). A total score for both task
and ego orientation is derived by adding six items contained in each
subscale. Examples of items embedded within the task and ego subscales
respectively are: "I work hard", and, "I am the
best". The POSQ has been modified previously in the physical domain
(Roberts, Treasure & Hall, 1994) and has demonstrated adequate
psychometric properties including factorial validity and internal
consistency (Roberts et al., 1994; Robert s & Ommundsen, 1996;
Roberts & Treasure, 1995; Treasure & Roberts, 1994).
Intrinsic Motivation. Intrinsic motivation for exercise was
assessed using the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) developed by
Ryan (1982). The IMI has been adapted in the past to quantify intrinsic
motivation in the physical activity domain (McAuley et al., 1989) and is
composed of four subscales assessing interest/enjoyment, perceived
competence, effort/importance, and tension/pressure. Scoring for these
items ranges from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). Total
scores for each subscale are established by summing the respective items
of each scale. Higher scores are indicative of greater levels of
intrinsic motivation whereas lower scores reflect less intrinsic
motivation. Items were slightly modified in order to reflect
participation in exercise. Examples of items on the instrument include,
"I enjoy exercise very much" (interest/enjoyment), "1 am
satisfied with my exercise performance" (perceived competence),
"I put a lot of effort into exercise" (effort/importance),
and, "I feel tense while ex ercising" (tension/pressure).
Construct validity and internal reliability of the IMI have been
successfully demonstrated within a physical activity context (McAuley et
al., 1989; McAuley, Wraith, & Duncan, 1991).
Results
Psychometric Properties of the Scales
The component structure of the various subscales contained in the
PSPP, POSQ, and IMI instruments was assessed using principal components
factor analysis. Varimax rotation and Kaiser normalization procedures
provided meaningful interpretation of the underlying simple structure
and the number of factors to be retained. A factor loading of .50 was
established as a minimum criterion in order to be included in the model.
For the PSPP, five factors emerged, identified as physical
self-worth, sports competence, physical condition, physical strength,
and attractive body with eigenvalues of 1.78, 4.34, 3.59, 3.46, and 6.08
respectively accounting for 64% of the variance among the items. In
contrast to the proposed hierarchical model (Fox & Corbin, 1989),
however, the physical self-worth scale and the attractive body subscale
demonstrated considerable factor overlap and were subsequently deleted from further analysis. This factor overlap between the attractive body
and physical self-worth scales has been reported elsewhere (Sonstroem et
al., 1992). The remaining three subscales of physical self-perceptions
accounted for 38% of the variation in the questionnaire items. Cronbach
(1951) alphas for these subscales were .90 for sports competence (6
items), .86 for physical condition (6 items), and .85 for physical
strength (6 items).
The POSQ also demonstrated a clear factor structure for
interpretation. Two factors emerged with eigenvalues greater than 1.00
and were identified as task (6 items) and ego orientation (6 items) for
exercise, accounting for 34% and 21% of the variance in
participants' responses, respectively. The interfactor correlation
was negligible (r = .04), consistent with past research (Treasure &
Roberts, 1994; Roberts & Treasure 1995) confirming orthogonality of
the two factors. Alpha coefficients were .71 and .90 for the task and
ego orientation for exercise subscales, respectively.
The IMI also exhibited a stable factor structure as all items
loaded on their respective factors meeting the minimum criterion to be
retained for interpretation. Eigenvalues were greater than 1.00 for the
interest/enjoyment (5 items), perceived competence (4 items),
effort/importance (5 items), and tension/pressure (4 items) intrinsic
motivation subscales, respectively, accounting for 60.1% of the variance
in the items. Internal reliability coefficients were .82
(interest/enjoyment), .77 (perceived competence), .76 (effort/
importance), and .77 (tension/ pressure) for the intrinsic motivation
for exercise subscales.
Descriptive Statistics
Means and standard deviations of the physical self-perception
(PSPP), task and ego orientations for exercise (POSQ), and intrinsic
motivation for exercise (IMI) subscales are displayed in Table 1.
Significant zero order correlations (2-tailed) were observed between
task orientation for exercise and both interest/enjoyment (r .29), and
effort/importance (r = .36). Self-perceptions of physical condition were
associated to interest/enjoyment (r = .49), perceived competence (r
.59), effort/importance (r = .49), and tension/pressure (r = -.27).
Intrinsic Motivation for Exercise
In order to examine the multivariate relationship between the
linear combination of the two sets of variables composed of physical
self-perceptions/goal orientations for exercise, and indices of
intrinsic motivation for exercise, canonical correlation was performed.
The predictor variables were composed of self-perceptions of sports
competence, physical condition, and physical strength, as well as task
and ego orientations for exercise. The criterion variables consisted of
the four measures of intrinsic motivation for exercise, specifically,
interest/enjoyment, perceived competence, effort/importance, and
tension/pressure.
The overall multivariate relationship was significant (Wilk's
lambda = .74, F(4, 252) = 21.73,p <.0001). Follow-up canonical
correlation analysis indicated three significant functions emerged.
However, only the first function exceeded the .30 criterion (10% of the
variance) suggested as the minimal level for meaningful interpretation
(Tabachnick & Fidell, 1989). The analysis revealed that the
relationship between the linear combinations of the two sets of
variables could best be explained by one function with a Rc value of .68
([R.sup.2]= .46). A minimal canonical loading of .30 is considered to
contribute significantly to the multivariate relationship (Pedhazur,
1982). As Table 2 demonstrates, a high positive loading on physical
condition, and moderately positive loadings on task orientation for
exercise, sports competence, and physical strength, were positively
associated to the criterion set of variables composed of interest/
enjoyment, perceived competence, and effort/importance, and were
negatively relat ed to tension/pressure for exercise. Ego orientation
for exercise, however, failed to significantly contribute to the
multivariate relationship. The redundancy index revealed that 16% of the
variance in the four indices of intrinsic motivation for exercise could
be explained by the linear combination of the predictor variables. A
redundancy value of 10% is recommended as a significant and meaningful
cutoff for interpretation (Pedhazur, 1982).
Discussion
Results of the present study demonstrated that physical
self-perceptions and a task orientation for exercise were associated to
intrinsic motivation for exercise. Canonical correlation analysis
revealed that, among undergraduate female exercisers, self-perceptions
of sports competence, physical condition, and physical strength, and a
task orientation for exercise, were positively related to a set of
criterion variables indicative of intrinsic motivation. A high, positive
loading on perceptions of physical condition, and moderately positive
loadings on perceptions of sports competence and physical strength
corresponded to a set of intrinsic motivation criterion measures
including interest/enjoyment, perceived competence, and
effort/importance during exercise. These same physical self-perceptions
were negatively related, however, to perceptions of tension/pressure
during exercise.
This behavioral pattern in the exercise domain is consistent with
tenets of both competence motivation (Harter, 1985) and cognitive
evaluation (Deci & Ryan, 1985) theories. Proponents of competence
motivation theory maintain that perceptions of physical competence lead
to higher degrees of intrinsic motivation for physical activity.
Application of the theory to exercise behavior has demonstrated that
perceived physical competence, in the form of physical self-perceptions,
is predictive of exercise behavior (Fox & Corbin, 1989). Cognitive
evaluation theory highlights the significance of feelings of competence
and self-determination, also theorized to lead to desirable levels of
intrinsic motivation. Physical activity perceived to be interesting,
challenging, and providing feelings of pleasure and satisfaction is
postulated to enhance intrinsic motivation (Vallerand & Fortier,
1998). In support of theoretical rationale, in the present study,
feelings of competence in the form of physical self-perceptions were f
ound to be intimately associated to intrinsic motivation for exercise.
The multidimensional approach to the study of physical self-concept
highlights the influence of the subdomains of physical condition, sports
competence, body attractiveness, and physical strength, to the
prediction of exercise behavior (Fox & Corbin, 1989; Hayes, et al.,
1999; Sonstroem et al., 1992; Sonstroem et al., 1994). Three of these
four physical self-perception measures were linked to intrinsic
motivation to participate in exercise among the present sample. However,
not unlike earlier studies (Sonstroem, et al., 1992; Sonstroem et al.,
1994), the body attractiveness subscale failed to demonstrate a stable
factor structure and was subsequently deleted from further analysis. The
most robust predictor of intrinsic motivation for exercise was
self-perceptions of physical condition which historically has been shown
to be most influential of the four subscales in predicting self-reported
exercise behavior (Fox & Corbin, 1989; Sonstroem et al., 1992;
Sonstroem et al., 1994).
Canonical correlation also revealed that a task orientation for
exercise was moderately and positively related to intrinsic motivation
for exercise. Only a limited number of studies have noted a meaningful
relationship between a task orientation and degree of
moderate-to-vigorous physically activity, exclusively among children
(Dempsey et al., 1993; Kimiecik et al., 1996). As the present results
suggest, a task orientation may not only induce greater enjoyment for
exercise but also leads to lower levels of tension/pressure. Focusing
upon mastery, self-improvement, and effort, rather than evaluating
one's unique level of fitness in relation to others, who may
already be in an exercise maintenance stage, appears to be a functional
strategy for maintaining intrinsic motivation and may also play a
significant role in adherence to an exercise regime. Individuals who are
either initiating involvement in an exercise program or are not
currently in a desirable state of cardiovascular condition, as well as
more experi enced exercise enthusiasts, would do well to take a
task-oriented approach to exercise while pursuing steady fitness
improvement over time.
An ego orientation was found to be unrelated to intrinsic
motivation for exercise. This finding, however, was not unexpected and
is consistent with research findings in the sport domain (Duda, et al.,
1995). For those who are ego-oriented, perceptions of competence are
normatively referenced, dependent upon the ability to outperform others
(Duda, 1992; Duda & Whitehead, 1998). An ego orientation and
concomitant preoccupation to perform better than others is a maladaptive dispositional strategy for motivating oneself to participate long-term
in an exercise program, reported to be associated to anxiety in the
physical domain (Hall & Kerr, 1997). Moreover, an ego orientation in
conjunction with low perceived physical ability has been theorized to
contribute to dropout in the physical domain (Duda, 1987). Commercial
advertisement of fitness corporations often promote pictorials of highly
physically fit individuals perhaps generating unrealistic expectations
to potential clients which may ultimately discourage the public from
engaging in ongoing exercise. Beginning as well as more experienced
exercisers, therefore, would be well advised to take a task-oriented
approach to exercise and working toward steady personal fitness
improvement over the long term.
Factor analysis of the subscales contained within both the POSQ
(goal orientation; Roberts & Balague, 1989) and the IMI (intrinsic
motivation; McAuley et al., 1989), modified for exercise, exhibited a
stable and internally reliable factor structure. Both scales have been
modified earlier in the sport and exercise psychology literature and
have held up well psychometrically (McAuley et al., 1989; Treasure &
Roberts, 1994). The two scales appear to be both valid and reliable
measures of exercise goal orientations and intrinsic motivation,
respectively. Further research should attempt to replicate the present
findings in order to extend the utility of these instruments in the
exercise domain among adult as well as youth populations.
In summary, higher physical self-perceptions and a task orientation
for exercise were found to be associated to intrinsic motivation for
exercise. In order to maintain optimal motivational levels for exercise,
practitioners would do well to emphasize a task orientation and personal
improvement over time in their teaching strategies, rather than
normatively referenced perceptions of fitness competence. A task
orientation and efforts toward improvement may subsequently eventually
lead to higher physical self-perceptions, which as the results suggest,
are also related to higher levels of intrinsic motivation to exercise.
Exercise provides substantial physical and psychological benefits for
those who choose to participate and therefore warrants ongoing research.
References
Ames, C. (1992). Achievement goals, motivational climate, and
motivational processes. In G.C. Robe Roberts (Ed.), Motivation ion in
sport and exercise. (pp. 161-176). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the
classroom: Students' learning strategies and motivation processes.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 260-267.
Bouchard, C., Shepard, R.J., & Stephens, T. (1993). Physical
activity, fitness, and health: Consensus statement. Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Boyd, M.P., & Yin, Z. (1996). Cognitive-affective sources of
sport enjoyment in adolescent sport participants. Adolescence, 31(122),
283-295.
Craft, L.L., & Landers, D.M. (1998). The effects of exercise on
clinical depression and depression resulting from mental illness: A
meta-analysis. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 20,339-357.
Cronbach, L.J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure
of tests. Psychometrika, 16,427-334.
Deci, E.L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum Press.
Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and
self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Dempsey, J.M., Kimiecik, J.C., & Horn, T.S. (1993). Parental
influence on children's moderate to vigorous physical activity
participation: An expectancy-value approach. Pediatric Exercise Science,
5, 151-167.
Dishman, R.K. (1988). Exercise adherence research: Future
directions. American Journal of Health Promotion, 3,52-56.
Duda, J.L. (1987). Toward a developmental theory of children's
motivation in sport. Journal of Sport Psychology 9, 130-145.
Duda, J.L. (1989). The relationship between task and ego
orientation and the perceived purposes of sport among male and female
high school athletes. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11,
318-335.
Duda, J.L. (1992). Motivation in sport settings: A goal perspective
approach. In G.C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in sport and exercise. (pp.
57-91). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Duda, J.L., Chi, L., Newton, M.L., Walling, M.D., & Catley, D.
(1995). Task and ego orientation and intrinsic motivation in sport.
International Journal of Sport Psychology, 26, 40-63.
Duda, J.L., Fox, K.R., Biddle, S.J.H., & Armstrong, N. (1992).
Children's achievement goals and beliefs about success in sport.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 62, 313-323.
Duda, J.L., & Nicholls, J.G. (1992). Dimensions of achievement
motivation in schoolwork and sport. Journal of Educational Psychology,
84, 1-10.
Duda, J.L., & Whitehead, J. (1998). Measurement of goal
perspectives in the physical domain. In J.L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in
sport and exercise psychology measurement. (pp. 21-48). Morgantown, WV:
Fitness Information Technology.
Dweck, C.S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning.
American Psychologist, 41, 1040-1048.
Dweck, C.S., & Elliott, E. (1993). Achievement motivation. In
M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Volume 4:
Socialization, personality, and social development. (pp. 643-691). New
York: Wiley.
Dzewaltkowski, D.A., Noble, J.M., & Shaw, J.M. (1989). Physical
activity participation: Social cognitive theory versus the theories of
reasoned action and planned behavior. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 12, 388-405.
Eastabrook, P., & Courneya, K.S. (1997). Relationships among
self-schema, intention, and exercise behavior. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 19, 156-158.
Fox, K.R. (1990). The Physical Self-Perception Profile Manual.
DeKalb, IL: Northern Illinois University, Office of Health Promotion.
Fox, K.R., & Corbin, C.B. (1989). The physical self-perception
profile: Development and preliminary validation. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 11, 408-430.
Gill, D.L. (1986). Competitiveness among females and males in
physical activity class. Sex Roles, 15, 239-247.
Godin, G., Colantonio, A., Davis, G.M., Shepard, R.J., &
Simard, C. (1986). Prediction of leisure time exercise behavior among a
group of lower limb disabled adults.Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42,
272-279.
Godin, G., & Shepard, R.J. (1986). Psychosocial factors
influencing intentions to exercise of young students from grades 7 to 9.
Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 57 (I), 41-52.
Hall, H. K., & Kerr, A.W. (1997). Motivational antecedents of
precompetitive anxiety in youth sport. The Sport Psychologist, 11,
24-42.
Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered: Toward a
developmental model. Human Development, 21,34-64.
Harter, S. (1985). Competence as a dimension of self-evaluation:
Toward a comprehensive model of self-worth. In R. Leahy (Ed.), The
development of the self. (pp.55-121). New York: Academic Press.
Harter, S. (1986). Processes underlying the construction,
maintenance and enhancement of self-concept in children. In J. Suls
& A. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological perspective on the self. (Vol.
3., pp. 136-182). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
Hatzigeorgiadis, A., & Biddle, S. (1999). The effects of goal
orientation and perceived competence and cognitive interference during
tennis and snooker performance. Journal of Sport Behavior, 22(4),
479-501.
Hausenblaus, H.A., Carron, A.V., & Mack, D.E. (1997).
Application of the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior to
exercise behavior: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 19, 36-51.
Hayes, S.D., Crocker, P.R.E., & Kowlaski, K.C. (1999). Gender
differences in physical self-perceptions, global self-esteem and
physical activity: Evaluation of the physical self-perception profile
model. Journal of Sport Behavior, 22(1), 1-14.
Hoeger, W.K., & Hoeger, S.A. (1998). Lifetime physical fitness
& wellness: A personalized program. Englewood, CO: Morton Publishing
Co.
Kendzierski, D. (1988). Self-schemata and exercise. Basic and
Applied Social Psychology, 9(1),45-59.
Kendzierski, D. (1990). Exercise self-schemata: Cognitive and
behavioral correlates. Health Psychology, 9(1), 69-82.
Kimiecik, J., Horn, T.S., & Shurin, C.S. (1996). Relationships
among children's beliefs, perceptions of their parents'
beliefs and their moderate-to-vigorous physical activity. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 67,324-336.
Maehr, M.L., & Braskamp, L.A. (1986). The motivation factor: A
theory of personal investment. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books
Marsh, H.W. (1996). Construct validity of the Physical
Self-Description Questionnaire: Responses: Relations to external
criteria. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18, 11-131.
Marsh, H.W., Byrne, B.M., & Shavelson, R.J. (1988). A
multifaceted academic self-concept: Its heirarchical structure and its
relation to academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80,
366-380.
Marsh, H.W., & Redmayne, R.S. (1994). A multidimensional
physical self-concept and its relation to multiple components of
physical fitness. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 16, 45-55.
Marsh, H.W., Richards, G., Johnson, S., Roche, L., & Tremayne,
P. (1994). Physical Self-Description Questionnaire: Psychometric
properties and a multitrait-multimethod analysis of relations to
existing instruments. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 16,
270-305.
Marsh, H.W., & Shavelson, R.J. (1985). Self-concept: Its
multifaceted, heirarchical structure. Educational Psychologist, 20,
107-125.
Marsh, H.W., & Sonstroem, R.J. (1995). Importance ratings and
specific components of physical self-concept: Relevance to predicting
global components of self-concept and exercise. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology 17, 84-104.
McAuley, E., & Courneya, K.S. (1992). Self-efficacy
relationships with affective and execution responses to exercise.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology 22, 312-326.
McAuley, E., Courneya, K.S., & Lettunich, J. (1991). Effects of
acute and long-term exercise on self-efficacy responses in sedentary,
middle-aged males and females. The Gerontologist, 31,534-542.
McAuley, E., Duncan, T., & Tammen, V. (1989). Psychometric
properties of the intrinsic motivation inventory in a competitive sport
setting: A confirmatory factor analysis. Research Quarterly for Exercise
and Sport, 60,48-58.
McAuley, E., Wraith, S., & Duncan, T.E. (1991). Self-efficacy,
perceptions of success, and intrinsic motivation for exercise. Journal
of Applied Social Psychology 21, 139-155.
Nicholls, J.G. (1984). Conceptions of ability and achievement
motivation. In R. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in
education: Student motivation. (Vol. 1, pp. 39-73). New York: Academic
Press.
Nicholls, J.G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic
education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Nicholls, J .G. (1992). The general and the specific in the
development and expression of achievement motivation. In G.C. Roberts
(Ed.), Motivation in sport and exercise. (pp.3 1-54), Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics.
Pedhazur, E.J. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral research.
New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Petruzello, S.J., Landers, D.M., Hatfield, B.D., Kubitz, K.A.,
& Salazar, W (1991 ).A meta-analysis on the anxiety reducing effects
of acute and chronic exercise. Sport Medicine, 11, 143-182.
Roberts, G.C. (1992). Motivation in sport and exercise: Conceptual
constraints and convergence. In G.C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in sport
and exercise. (pp. 3-30).Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Roberts, G.C., & Balague, G. (1989). The development of a
social-cognitive scale of motivation. Paper presented at the Seventh
World Congress in Sport Psychology, Singapore.
Roberts, G.C., & Balague, G. (1991). The development and
validation of the Perception of Success Questionnaire. Paper presented
at the FEPSAC Congress, Cologne, Germany.
Roberts, G.C., & Ommundsen, Y. (1996). Effect of goal
orientation on achievement beliefs, cognitions, and strategies in team
sport. Scandinavian Journal of Science in Medicine and Sport, 6,46-56.
Roberts, G.C., & Treasure, D.C. (1995). Achievement goals,
motivational climate and achievement strategies and behaviors in sport.
International Journal of Sport Psychology 26,64-80.
Rudolph, D.L., & McAuley, E. (1996). Self-efficacy and
perceptions of effort: A reciprocal relationship. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology 18,216-223.
Ryan, E.D. (1982). Control and information in the intrapersonal sphere: An extension of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 43, 450-461.
Ryska, T.A., Yin, Z., & Boyd, M. (1999). The role of
dispositional goal orientation and team climate on situational
self-handicapping among young athletes. Journal of Sport Behavior,
22(3), 410-415.
Shavelson, R.J., Hubner, J.J., & Stanton, G.C. (1976).
Self-concept: Validation of construct interpretations. Review of
Educational Research, 46, 407-441.
Sonstroem, R.J., Harlow, L.L., & Josephs, L.( 1994). Exercise
and self-esteem: Validity of model expansion and exercise associations.
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 16,29-42.
Sonstroem, R.J., & Morgan, W.P. (1989). Exercise and
self-esteem: Rationale and model. Medicine and Science in Sports and
Exercise, 21, 329-337.
Sonstroem, R.J., Speliotis, E.D., & Fava, J.L. (1992).
Perceived physical competence in adults: An examination of the physical
self-perception profile. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology
14,207-221.
Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (1989). Using multivariate
statistics (2nd ed.). New York: Harper & Rowe.
Treasure, D.C., & Roberts, G.C. (1994). Cognitive and affective
concomitants of task and ego goal orientations during the middle school
years. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 16, 15-28.
Vallerand, R.J., & Fortier, M.S. (1998). Measures of intrinsic
and extrinsic motivation in sport and physical activity: A review and
critique. In J.L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in sport and exercise psychology
measurement. (pp. 81-101) Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information
Technology.
White, S.A., Duda, J.L., & Keller, M.R. (1998). The
relationship between goal orientation and perceived purposes of sport
among youth sport participants. Journal of Sport Behavior, 21 (4),
474-483.
White, S.A., & Zellner, S.R. (1996). The relationship between
goal orientation, beliefs about the causes of sport success, and trait anxiety among high school, intercollegiate, and recreational sport
participants. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 58-72.
Williams, L. (1994). Goal orientations and athlete's
preferences for competence information sources. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 16, 416-430.
Yin, Z., & Boyd, M.P. (2000). Behavioral and cognitive
correlates of exercise self-schemata. Journal of Psychology,
134(3),296-282.
Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, and Reliability Cofficients of All Measures
M SD Alpha
Goal orientations
Ego 2.86 1.02 .90
Task 4.62 .37 .71
Physical self-perceptions
Sports competence 2.35 .72 .90
Physical condition 2.54 .69 .86
Physical strength 2.45 .60 .85
Intrinsic motivation
Interest/enjoyment 3.75 .74 .82
Perceived competence 3.78 .64 .77
Effort/importance 3.95 .70 .76
Tension/pressure 2.26 .77 .77
Table 2
Correlations, Canonical Correlation, percents of Variance, and
Redundancies Between Physical Self-Perceptions/Goal Orientations and
Intrinsic Motivation
Canonical Loadings
Physical Self-Perceptions/
Goal Orientations
Task Orientation .493
Ego Orientation .128
Sports Competence .595
Physical Condition .936
Physical Strength .477
Percent of variance .34
Redundancy .16
Intrinsic Motivation
Interest/Enjoyment .780
Perceived Competence .893
Effort/Importance .810
Tension/Pressure -.440
Percent of variance .56
Redundancy .26
Canonical correlation .68