The Importance of Analyzing Position-Specific Self-Efficacy.
Weigand, Daniel A. ; Stockham, Kimberley J.
The purposes of this study were to assess perceptions of
position-specific and cross-skill self-efficacy in a team sport and to
assess the effect of competition level on skill-specific self-efficacy.
Data were collected from 110 British amateur players (M age = 26.93, SD
= 5.8) competing in either a 1st, 2nd, or 3rd Division team of the
Godfrey Davis Women's Field Hockey League. One hour prior to a
league game players completed measures including 8 items assessing the
magnitude and strength (Bandura, 1986) of skill-specific self-efficacy.
Based on the results of a pilot study, the 8 skills were considered to
be task-specific to forward (3 items), defensive (3 items), and midfield (2 items) positions. Results of 3 X 3 (Position by Division) between
subjects MANOVA/ANOVAs and Scheffe tests indicated that forwards scored
significantly (p [less than] .05) higher on the forward-specific
self-efficacy scores than either midfielders or defense. Furthermore,
1st Division athletes scored significantly (p [less than] .05) higher on
the forward-specific self-efficacy scores than either 2nd or 3rd
Division athletes. There were no significant differences found between
athletes on the measure of cross-skill self-efficacy. Findings are
discussed with regard to the need to examine skill-specific, rather than
generalized, self-efficacy in the relationship between self-efficacy and
performance in sport.
According to Roberts (1992), "The theory of self-efficacy has
been the most extensively used theory for investigating motivational
issues in sport and exercise" (p. 11). Self-efficacy theory is a
social-cognitive approach to behavior that takes into account
behavioral, physiological, and cognitive factors, as well as
environmental influences. According to McAuley (1992), the theory
provides "[ldots]a common mechanism through which people
demonstrate control over their own motivation and behavior" (p.
109) because the theory focuses on the role of self-referent thought in
relation to psychosocial functioning. Bandura (1977, 1997) suggested
that self-efficacy affects choice of activities, effort expenditure,
persistence, and achievement. Therefore, because self-efficacy can be
broadly defined as the beliefs that individuals have in their capability
to engage successfully in a course of action sufficient to satisfy the
situational demands (McAuley, 1992), self-efficacy can be seen to be an
important determinant of physical activity and sport behavior. Athletes
who possess higher amounts of self-efficacy are more likely to choose
tasks they can accomplish, work harder, persist longer when they
experience difficulties, and achieve at a higher level than those with
lower levels of self-efficacy (Schunk, 1995).
Research has shown that self-efficacy is a determinant of
educational, social, clinical, and health-related behaviors (see
O'Leary, 1985; and Schunk, 1989). Furthermore, according to recent
reviews (Feltz, 1992; McAuley, 1992), self-efficacy theory also shows
considerable promise for explaining motivation and behavior in sport and
exercise contexts. It seems that repeated successes raise an
individual's efficacy appraisals and also increases motivation,
while repeated failures can lower these appraisals, which also may
decrease motivation (Bandura, 1977). Individual success in team sports,
such as field hockey, is often skill (or position) specific. For
example, defensive players may define success in terms of their ability
to keep the other team from scoring or being able to consistently hit
the ball a long distance. Conversely, forwards in field hockey need to
be accurate at shots on goal and have the ability to dribble the ball
successfully down the field. Thus, is appears that different skills are
requir ed for different positions that are largely dependent upon the
particular abilities individuals possess. Team-sport athletes are
therefore likely to develop certain skills (those more required for
certain desired positions) at the loss of other, less relevant skills.
Consequently, their self-efficacy for certain skills required by
specific positions may be higher than for other positions.
Although the findings from sport-related efficacy research are
compelling, the measurement of self-efficacy has been inconsistent
(Feltz & Chase, in press). According to Bandura (1986), the
measurement of self-efficacy cognitions should be carried out in a
microanalytical fashion by assessing an efficacy task specifically along
three dimensions: level, strength, and generality. The level of
self-efficacy concerns the expected performance attainment of
individuals (e.g., whether athletes think they can perform a task or
not), while strength is the certainty with which they expect to
successfully attain the task (e.g., on a 0-100 point scale). Generality,
however, refers to the number of domains in which individuals feel that
they are efficacious. Thus, someone who has a high perception of
self-efficacy in running may have an equally high degree of
self-efficacy in biking.
In contrast, Bandura's conception of generality also suggests
that a high level of self-efficacy in one domain does not necessarily
result in a high level of self-efficacy in another domain (McAuley
1992). That is, there is a potential for specificity of self-efficacy
perceptions. For example, an individual with high expectations in tennis
may not have similar expectations in badminton. Although both sports use
a racquet to hit an object over a net, the biomechanics and motor skills
require sufficiently diverse abilities that self-efficacy perceptions
may significantly differ. In a related manner, team-sport athletes may
have developed skill-specific expertise required for certain positions,
at the expense of skill development necessary to be successful at other
positions. For instance, point guards in basketball may develop
expertise in dribbling and passing the ball, whereas centers may not be
able to dribble or pass as well as guards, but they should be able to
rebound better than guards.
In summary, based on Bandura's (1986) suggestions, it appears
that self-efficacy in sport or exercise should be measured by
evaluating: (a) the number of tasks that individuals can expect to
perform leading up to a target behavior, (b) the confidence with which
they expect to successfully attain each aspect of the target behavior,
and (c) the number of domains or skills in which they consider
themselves efficacious. Thus, the first purpose of this study was to
assess perceptions of cross-skill and skill-specific (i.e.,
position-specific) self-efficacy in British Women's Field Hockey.
In field hockey, forwards need to be able to demonstrate highly accurate
shots at the goal and an ability to dribble the ball successfully.
Conversely, defensive players are required to successfully defend short
corner shots and be able to push the ball a long distance. The role of
midfielders is a combination of the roles of forwards and the defense;
they need to be able to attack as well as defend. Considering the
different ro les that players have while on the pitch, it can be assumed
that field hockey players may have different perceptions of their
abilities to perform task-specific skills according to the positions
they play. Therefore, in support of the specificity aspect of
Bandura's generality principle, it was hypothesized that
athletes' self-efficacy would be significantly higher for the
skills relevant to their position than for the skills relevant to other
positions. Moreover, it also was expected that there would be no
significant differences between athletes on a measure of cross-skill
self-efficacy.
An additional purpose of this study was to assess the effect of
competition level on skill-specific self-efficacy. According to Bandura
(1977), performance accomplishments provide the most dependable and
influential source of self-efficacy. Thus, it is possible that athletes
competing at higher levels of ability might have higher performance
expectations than those competing at lower levels of ability.
Furthermore, athletes at higher levels of competition might have
developed their position-specific skills more so than those who compete
at lower levels. Therefore, the final hypothesis was that athletes
competing at higher levels of competition would have higher
position-specific self-efficacy than those competing at lower levels of
competition.
Method
Participants and Procedures
Data were collected from 110 female athletes (Mage = 26.93, SD =
5.8) who trained and played amateur British field hockey at least twice
a week throughout the season. Participants played for either a 1st
Division (n = 36), 2nd Division (n = 38), or 3rd Division (n = 36) team.
Athletes also were classified as either forwards (n = 37), midfielders
(n = 36), or defense (n = 37). After obtaining informed consent from
each participant, questionnaires were administered approximately one
hour before a game of the Godfrey Davis Women's League. The
questionnaires required approximately 15 minutes to complete.
Respondents were encouraged to ask the researcher about any questions
that arose during administration of the questionnaire. The participants
also were reminded that their participation was voluntary and that all
responses would be anonymous.
Measures
Self-Efficacy. Based on the recommendations of Locke and Latham
(1990), an instrument was designed to assess cross-skill and
skill-specific self-efficacy in field hockey. A pilot study was
conducted to evaluate skills required for effective performance at
forward, midfield, and defense positions. Interviews with 10 female 1st
Division hockey players were conducted by the second author (a
collegiate field hockey player) approximately one hour before a league
game. Based on responses from the participants in the pilot study, self-
efficacy was assessed by having players record the level to which they
thought they were capable of performing eight items considered to be
major components of the game and task- specific to forward (3 items),
defensive (3 items), and midfield (2 items) positions. Skills that were
considered to be most relevant to forwards included open-field scoring,
dribbling, and penalty shots. The items related to defensive positions
were defending short corners, tackling, and hitting the ball fo r
distance. Pushing the ball and lifting an aerial ball successfully were
the skills identified as important for midfielders. Participants were
first required to indicate (in Column A) whether or not they felt
capable (Yes or No) of executing each skill at four ascending levels of
difficulty (e.g., I can hit the ball 16, 25, 50, or 75 yards), then were
required (in Column B) to rate their certainty of performing each level
from 0% (Extremely Uncertain) to 100% (Absolute Certain). They also were
instructed that a "No" response in Column A equaled 0% in
Column B. Cross-skill self-efficacy was scored by calculating a mean
score including all eight items. Higher scores represented greater
self-efficacy. This cross-skill scale generated an acceptable internal
consistency: Cronbach's (1951) alpha = .78. Skill-specific
self-efficacy scores were the values generated by the position-specific
items separately. Only two of these three scales generated acceptable
internal consistency: forward-specific alpha = .70 and def ense-specific
alpha = .67 (Weiss et al. [1985] identified alpha coefficients of .60 as
acceptable for the internal consistency of a sport scale, therefore the
defense-specific scale was considered acceptable). The midfield-specific
scale was not considered to be internally consistent (alpha = .24) and
was therefore excluded from additional analyses.
Results
Descriptive statistics for the cross-skill and position-specific
self-efficacy scores are presented in Table 1. Results of a 3 X 3
(Position by Division) between subjects ANOVA on the cross-skill
self-efficacy scores revealed no significant main effects for position,
F (2, 101) = 0.44, p [greater than] .05, nor division, F (2, 101) =
0.47, p [greater than] .05, nor was there an interaction effect, F (4,
101) = 0.46, p [greater than] .05. In contrast, results of a 3 X 3
(Position by Division) between subjects MANOVA on the forward- and
defense-specific self-efficacy scores revealed significant main effects
for position, F (4, 200) = 7.69, p [less than] .001, and division, F (4,
200) = 2.83, p [less than] .05, but not an interaction, F (8, 200) =
0.78, p [greater than] .05. Results of subsequent 3 X 3 (Position by
Division) univariate ANOVAs revealed that the significant main effects
were generated on the forward-specific self-efficacy scores,
[F.sup.Position] (2, 101) = 8.09, p [less than] .001; [F.sup.Division]
(2, 101) = 4.21, p [less than] .05. Specifically, post hoc Scheffe tests
indicated that forwards scored significantly (p [less than] .05) higher
(M = 62.55) on the forward-specific self-efficacy scores than either
midfielders (M = 48.30) or defense (M = 45.27); 1st Division athletes
scored significantly (p [less than] .05) higher (M= 60.01) on the
forward-specific self-efficacy scores than either 2nd (M = 48.06) or 3rd
(M = 48.46) Division athletes. In summary, these results indicate that,
in comparison to a cross-skill measure of self-efficacy, significant
differences between positions and Divisions emerged only when
position-specific self-efficacy was assessed.
Discussion
The purposes of this study were to assess perceptions of
position-specific and cross-skill self-efficacy in a team sport and to
assess the effect of competition level on skill-specific self-efficacy.
Results of analyses of variance and post hoc tests provide partial
support for the hypothesis that position-specific efficacy expectations
were significantly higher for the skills relevant to specific positions
than for the skills relevant to other positions. Specifically,
self-efficacy perceptions for forwards were significantly higher than
either midfielders or those on defense for the skills of open-field
scoring, dribbling, and taking penalty shots, skills that most field
hockey experts would agree are more important for forwards than for
others. Furthermore, in support of the second hypothesis, there were no
significant differences found between athletes on a measure of
cross-skill self-efficacy.
Results of analyses of variance and post hoc tests also provide
partial support for the final hypothesis. Specifically, 1st Division
players had significantly higher self-efficacy perceptions than either
2nd or 3rd Division athletes for those skills deemed to be most
appropriate for forwards.
The results of this investigation seem to support Bandura's
(1986) belief that the measurement of self-efficacy, at least in a team
sport, should be carried out in a microanalytical fashion by assessing
task-specificity. That is, rather than measuring cross-skill efficacy
expectations, including various sport-specific skills, one should take
into account the potential ability differences between individuals at
different positions and assess skill-specific self-efficacy.
Furthermore, athletes competing at different levels of competition may
have diverse performance expectations that relate more to specific, than
general, task-related abilities. These findings have implications for
applied sport psychologists.
Mental skills training techniques should address each of
Bandura's (1977, 1989, cited in Feltz, 1992) four primary sources
of self-efficacy information (performance accomplishments, vicarious experiences, verbal persuasion, and physiological states) with a focus
on developing skill-specific self-efficacy. Some of these areas have
been developed by Schunk (1995) in a comprehensive review of
self-efficacy, motivation, and performance, although not with specific
reference to skill-specific self-efficacy. First, Schunk emphasized the
importance of assessing how procedures affect self-efficacy and
motivation. That is, in addition to assessing how interventions affect
performance outcomes, educators and coaches should be providing
instruction that "includes periods of self-directed mastery or
independent practice where learners practice skills on their own"
(p. 132). This should not only facilitate skill acquisition, but also
enhance self-efficacy. The current findings suggest that in team sports
these self-direct ed mastery experiences should be skill-specific.
Athletes need to develop certain abilities to successfully perform
certain roles on a team. Each role is therefore likely to generate
specific efficacy expectations. Performance accomplishments are often
skill-specific in a team sport (although they also could be team
oriented, e.g., team effort, team communication, team work, cf Zaccaro,
Blair, Peterson, & Zazanis, 1995). If sport psychologists are going
to facilitate enhanced self-efficacy for athletes on a team, then they
should recommend that coaches provide opportunities for individuals to
develop skills relevant to specific roles and abilities.
Schunk (1995) also indicated that the research on self-efficacy in
education and sport suggests that peers should be employed as models.
Vicarious experiences are important sources of information and the
research seems to suggest that, although adults (e.g., teachers and
coaches) make good models for teaching skills, peers may make better
models. Self-efficacy may be better enhanced by models who are more
similar to the observers than those more expert on the skills being
developed. Thus, sport psychologists working with teams may want to
recommend that coaches identify individuals who are sufficiently
competent in the skills being developed to use as models for those who
are not as well developed. The present results further suggest that in
team sports these models should be demonstrating skills that are
specific to certain roles. By viewing similar others demonstrating
competent role-specific skill execution, the observers should better
concentrate on what is being said and shown and consequently improve t
heir performance and enhance their self-efficacy.
Another of Schunk's (1995) recommendations was that teachers
and coaches should provide specific and credible feedback to learners.
According to Schunk, research in sport and education has shown that
specific "feedback that denotes how performance has improved is
likely to raise self-efficacy and motivation" (p. 132). It is not
enough for coaches to tell their athletes that they have "done
well" unless the athletes know what specifically they have done
well. Moreover, coaches will not facilitate self-efficacy by telling
athletes that they are improving if the athletes believe they are
struggling. Sport psychologist should therefore suggest to coaches that
verbal persuasion should be realistic and targeted toward specific
skills that are developing or need development. If athletes are aware of
their strengths and weaknesses, they will have more realistic
expectations about their abilities and understand where they should be
putting forth more or less effort in practice.
The last of Schunk's (1995) suggestions is that coaches and
teachers should use goals to foster commitment. According to Bandura
(1989, cited in Feltz, 1992), one source of efficacy information is
emotional arousal and research (Locke & Latham, 1990) has shown that
goals motivate people to exert effort necessary to meet task demands and
select appropriate strategies for task completion, which should reduce
anxiety and enhance self-efficacy. Athletes who set and attain goals may
experience an increase in self-efficacy which may facilitate setting
more difficult goals. Achieving these more difficult goals will again
enhance self-efficacy. Thus, as athletes observe goal progress, they are
becoming more skillful, and their self-efficacy should continue to
increase as well. The current findings also suggest that in team sports
goals should be skill-specific. That is, athletes should be setting
goals that focus on the skills necessary for effective performance
according to the position in which they want to develo p skills. One
caveat needs discussing. Although the research seems to suggest that
goals may be effective in sport, the literature also contains studies
with conflicting or inconsistent results on the effects of goal setting
in sport (see Locke, 1991, 1994; Weinberg & Weigand, 1993, 1996).
Thus, additional research is needed to better understand the influence
that goals have on self-efficacy and performance in sport.
In conclusion, although the results of this study should be taken
as preliminary, the findings seem to suggest that self-efficacy should
be considered relative to role-specific skills, at least in team sports,
rather than as a measure of cross-skill self-efficacy across the various
skills necessary for success in a particular sport. Sport psychologists
should be aware of this when designing mental skills training packages
and recommend that coaches enhance performance and self-efficacy by
focusing on the skills relevant to positions and roles.
Daniel A. Weigand, Ph.D., is a Principal Lecturer of Sport &
Exercise Psychology in the School of Physical Education, Sport, &
Leisure at De Montfort University, Bedford. Kimberley J. Stockham is a
graduate of De Montfort University, Bedford.
This investigation is based on an undergraduate honours degree study at De Montfort University, Bedford, completed by Kimberley J.
Stockham under the supervision of Daniel A. Weigand, Ph.D.
References
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New
York: Freeman.
Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal
structure of tests. Psychometrika, 16,297-334.
Feltz, D. L. (1992). Understanding motivation in sport: A
self-efficacy perspective. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in Sport
and Exercise (pp. 93-105). Champaign Il: Human Kinetics.
Feltz, D. L., & Chase, M. A. (in press). The measurement of
self-efficacy and confidence in sport. In J. Duda (Ed.), Advancements in
sport and exercise psychology measurement. Morgantown, WV: Fitness
Information Technology.
Locke, E. A. (1991). Problems with goal-setting research in
sports-and their solution. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology,
13, 311-316.
Locke, E. A. (1994). Comments on Weinberg and Weigand. Journal of
Sport & Exercise Psychology, 16,212-215.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A Theory of goal-setting
and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
McAuley, E. (1992). Understanding exercise behavior: A
self-efficacy perspective. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in sport
and exercise (pp. 107-127). Champaign Il: Human Kinetics.
O'leary, A. (1985). Self-efficacy and health. Behavior and
Research Therapy, 23,437-451.
Roberts, G. C. (1992). Motivation in sport and exercise: Conceptual
constraints and convergence. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in sport
and exercise (pp. 3-29). Champaign Il: Human Kinetics.
Schunk, D. H. (1989). Self-efficacy and achievement behaviors.
Educational Psychology Review, 1, 173-208.
Schunk, D. H. (1995). Self-efficacy, motivation, and performance.
Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 7, 112-137.
Weinberg, R., & Weigand, D. (1993). Goal setting in sport and
exercise: A reaction to Locke. Journal of Sport & Exercise
Psychology, 15, 88-96.
Weinberg, R. S., & Weigand, D. A. (1996). Let the discussions
continue: A reaction to Locke's comments on Weinberg and Weigand.
Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 18,89-93.
Weiss, M.R., Bredemeier, B.J., & Shewchuk, R. (1985). An
intrinsic/extrinsic sport motivation scale for the youth sport setting:
A confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Sport Psychology, 7,75-91.
Zaccaro, S. J., Blair, V., Peterson, C., & Zazanis, M. (1995).
Collective efficacy. In J. E. Maddux (Ed.), Self-efficacy adaptation and
adjustment: Theory, research and application (pp. 308-330). New York:
Plenum.
Means and Standard Deviations Across Positions and Divisions on
Cross-Skill and Position-Specific Self-Efficacy
Forwards [a] Midfielders [b]
Self-efficacy M SD M SD
Cross-skill 64.56 14.24 56.87 15.17
Forward-specific 62.55 [12] 19.09 48.30 [1] 21.53
Defense-specific 74.58 16.46 74.81 17.24
1st Division [e] 2nd Division [f]
Cross-skill 64.06 12.99 58.11 12.87
Forward-specific 60.01 [12] 19.39 48.06 [1] 19.04
Defense-specific 76.70 12.28 75.88 15.72
Defense [c]
Self-efficacy M SD F [d]
Cross-skill 59.79 14.00 0.44
Forward-specific 45.27 [2] 22.02 8.09 [***]
Defense-specific 81.14 13.04 1.92
3rd Division [g]
Cross-skill 59.20 17.56 0.47
Forward-specific 48.46 [2] 25.69 4.21 [*]
Defense-specific 77.88 19.21 0.13
Note. Midfield-specific self-efficacy scores are excluded due
to the scale's lack of internal consistency. Variables are
percentages of certainty of executing the skills. Similar
subscripts indicate groups that are significantly (p [less
than] .05) different.
(a.)n = 37.
(b.)n = 36.
(c.)n/ = 37.
(d.)(*.)p [less than] .05,
(**.)p [less than] .01,
(***.)p [less than] .001.
(e.)n = 36.
(f.)n = 38.
(g.)n = 36.