Older vs. Younger Adult Male Marathon Runners: Participative Motives and Training Habits.
Ogles, Benjamin M. ; Masters, Kevin S.
Motivations for and training habits used to prepare for running a
marathon were compared between older adults, age [geq]50 (N = 104), and
younger adults, age 20 - 28 (N = 110). Motivations were assessed using
the Motivations of Marathoners Scales (MOMS) that includes nine
internally consistent scales measuring motives for running a marathon.
Older runners reported being more motivated by a general health
orientation, weight concern, life meaning, and affiliation with other
runners. Younger runners were more motivated by personal goal
achievement. Older and younger runners train approximately equal miles
and hours per week. However older runners train more months prior to the
race, and had completed more previous marathons than younger runners.
Younger runners who had completed previous marathons had significantly
lower best times than older runners.
Prior to the 1970s, long distance running was an activity primarily
reserved for elite athletes who trained for serious competition. With
the advent of "Jogging", however, long distance running became
a bona tide leisure activity. Today, rank and file runners can be seen
shuffling along the roads, in the parks, and in the numerous road races
(Yair, 1990). With this change in emphasis from elite to leisure status,
leisure researchers have become increasingly interested in investigating
various aspects of recreational running (e.g., Clough, Sheperd, &
Maughan, 1989; Yair, 1990). One sub-group of long distance runners,
marathoners, is particularly interesting to study because of the broad
impact training has on their lives. Marathon runners voluntarily expose
themselves to physical distress while following rigorous time-consuming
schedules and careful diets that are well beyond the levels necessary
for general health and fitness. They may schedule vacations around
potential races and prioritize the daily run at the top of their to-do
lists. Some develop a running identity that includes association with
runners' clubs and frequent racing. Some researchers have even gone
so far as to use the terms obligatory running or running addiction to
describe the dedicated activity of the long distance runner (Blumenthal,
O'Toole, & Chang, 1984; Dishman, 1985). Regardless of the terms
used, however, marathon runners certainly represent an unusual group of
motivated individuals involved in a unique leisure activity.
A number of investigators have explored the participative motives
of long distance runners. Carmack and Martens (1979) identified seven
categories runners gave for running, including: physical health,
psychological health, self-image, affiliation, achievement, rewards,
social influence, and availability. Johnsgard (1985a, 1985b), using an
open-ended questionnaire, reported similar clustering of subjects'
motivations for running. Summers and colleagues (Summers, Machin, &
Sargent, 1983; Summers, Sargent, Levey, & Murray, 1982), found goal
achievement, a test of personal worth, physical health, and the
influence of others to be the most frequently reported reasons for
running a marathon while increasing the level of fitness was reported
most frequently as the reason for initially beginning to run as a form
of exercise. Curtis and McTeer (1981) in their study of 587 marathoners
summarized that most runners began running as a way of improving their
physical or emotional health (e.g. lose weight, increase cardio respiratory fitness, relieve stress, maintain fitness for other sports),
but then move on to marathon running as part of a need for additional
challenges or personal achievement. Finally, Clough, Sheperd, and
Maughan (1989) investigated the self-report participative motives of
over 500 marathon and half-marathon runners. They constructed a
comprehensive list of items based on the motivational categories
developed at a round table conference of leisure researchers (Crandall,
1980). A factor analysis of runners responses resulted in six basic
categories of participative motives: challenge, health/fitness,
well-being, addiction, status, and social.
While these studies provide initial formulations concerning the
motives of long distance runners, Pargman's suggestion that the
motivations of long distance runners are only superficially understood
and that no comprehensive theory of running motivation is generally
accepted may well be equally applicable now as when first published in
1980. Certainly, many questions regarding the participative motives of
runners remain unanswered and some methodologies lack rigor. Few studies
have attempted to begin investigating differences in participative
motives among various sub-groups of runners. For example, age as a
possible confounding variable has frequently been ignored and most
studies include runners who train and compete in races of varying
distances. Although Johnsgard (1985a, 1985b) asked older runners to
retrospectively report their motives for running at a younger age, no
study has attempted to investigate differences between varying age
groups of runners. Similarly, the evolution of motivations has never
been studied using a longitudinal methodology. In addition, the data
concerning participative motives has typically been collected through
surveys using open-ended questions. The answers are then divided
according to content category with totals and percentages reported for
each. Recently, a standardized self-report instrument that measures
motives for running has been developed (Masters, Ogles, & Jolton,
1993) and includes nine internally consistent scales: health
orientation, weight concern, personal goal achievement, competition,
life meaning, psychological coping, self-esteem, affiliation, and
recognition. Increased measurement precision, examination of running
sub-groups, and consideration of the multi-dimensional nature of
motivations for running will certainly improve upon previous
methodologies for studying the motivations of runners.
This study begins the process of further investigating within-group
differences among marathoners by comparing the self-report participative
motives and training habits of older and younger male marathon runners.
Female marathon runners were not considered in this particular study,
because there were only a few women race participants over age 50. It
was hypothesized that older and younger marathon runners would report
differing motives for training and running a marathon, and that these
differences may lead to theoretical formulations regarding the evolution
of running motives over the life-span. More specifically, it was
hypothesized that older runners would report being more motivated by
health and weight concerns while younger runners would be more motivated
by competition and personal goal achievement.
Older and younger runners were also hypothesized to have different
training habits. The expectation was that older runners would train
equal miles per week, but at a slower pace, thus resulting in more
training hours per week. It was also expected that older runners would
have completed more previous marathons.
Finally, it was hypothesized that difference in motivation either
alone or interesting with age would be related to training and
performance variables. In particular, runners who reported social
motives (affiliation & recognition) as reasons for running were
expected to be less likely to train alone when controlling for age.
Similarly, age and social motives were hypothesized to interact in their
influence on time spent training alone. Runners who reported being more
motivated by achievement motives (competition & personal goal
achievement) were expected to report training more miles per week,
participating in more marathons, and having faster previous finishing
times when controlling for their age. Again, age was hypothesized to
interact with achievement motives with regards to these variables.
Finally, runners who reported general health as a primary motive for
running were expected to train fewer miles per week when controlling for
age.
Method
Subjects
Runners pre-registering for one of four midwestern marathons were
asked to complete a packet of questionnaires and return them by mail.
The initial response rates ranged from 38 to 45%. From the final subject
pool of 1075 runners, all older male runners (age [geq] 50) were
selected (N = 104). In addition, all male runners between the ages of 20
and 28 were selected as a comparison group (N = 110). The sample of 214
runners were predominantly white (94%), married (60%), men who earned an
average income of $36,000 per year. The self-reported average height was
70 inches and the average weight was 157 pounds.
Instruments
The packet of questionnaires included the Motivations of
Marathoners Scales (MOMS; Masters, Ogles, & Jolton, 1993) and a form
to collect demographic and training information as well as several other
instruments not relevant to this study.
Demographic and Training Questionnaire. Participants answered
questions regarding several variables of interest, including: age;
gender; race; yearly income; marital status; miles, hours and days of
training per week; years running; percent of time training alone; number
of previously completed marathons; best and average finish times for
previous marathons; and the number of runners typically associated with
during a week.
Motivations of Marathoners Scales (MOMS). The MOMS is a 56-item
instrument that has nine scales. The contents of the items and scales
were developed based on previous studies investigating the motives of
long distance runners (Carmack & Martens, 1979; Curtis & McTeer,
1981; Johnsgard, 1985, 1989; Masters & Lambert, 1989; Summers, et
al., 1983; Summers, et al., 1982). Through a review of previous studies,
four broad categories of motives for running were identified:
psychological, physical, social, and achievement. Within these broad
categories nine specific motives for running a marathon were also
gleaned from previous research. Psychological motives are comprised of
maintaining or enhancing self-esteem, providing a sense of life meaning
or aesthetics, and problem solving or coping with negative emotions.
Physical motives for marathon running include general health benefits,
and weight concern. Social motives include affiliation with other
runners, and recognition or approval from family or friends. Finally,
achievement motives for marathon running include competition with other
runners, and personal goal achievement. The nine scales of the MOMS are
adequately reliable both in terms of internal consistency (range .80 to
.92) and test-retest estimates (intraclass correlations range from .71
to .90). in addition, evidence for the validity of the instrument has
been presented in several studies (Masters & Ogles, 1995; Masters
& Ogles, 1998; Masters, Ogles, & Jolton, 1993; Ogles, Masters,
& Richardson, 1995).
Procedure
Runners were recruited to participate in the study during pre-race
registration at four midwestern marathons. As the marathon entrants
registered for the race, they were asked to take home and complete a
packet of questionnaires and return them via the mail. A total of 2528
packets were distributed with 1075 (43%) usable tests returned. From
this pool of participants our two groups were selected.
Results
Three major analyses were conducted: a comparison of motivations
for the two age groups, a comparison of training and performance
statistics for the two age groups, and an analysis of the relationship
between motives and training behaviors.
Differences in Motives
In order to evaluate the differences in self-report motivations for
training for and running a marathon, a one-way (older vs. younger)
multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was calculated using the MOMS
scales as the dependent variables. The overall MANOVA was significant,
F(9,200) = 8.28,p [less than].00l, indicating a significant difference
between the two age groups. Univariate analyses revealed that older
runners were more strongly motivated to run as part of a broad health
orientation, F(1,208) = l8.63, p[less than].001, weight concern,
F(1,208) = 6.25, p[less than].01, life meaning, F(1,208)= 6.19,p[less
than].01, and affiliation with other runners, F(1,208) = 6.50,p [less
than].01. Younger runners were more motivated by personal goal
achievement, F(l,08)= l5.05,p[less than].001 (Table 1). Older and
younger runners did not differ in their level of competitiveness, p
[greater than] .05.
Differences in Training Variables
Differences between the age groups on training variables were
evaluated using independent t-tests (Table 2). Older and younger runners
train approximately equal miles and hours per week and do not differ in
the amount of time they train alone. Older runners, however, did train
more months prior to the race, t (211) = -3.O6,p [less than].01, and
completed significantly more marathons than the comparison group, t
(208) = -4.50,p [less than].001. Finally, younger runners who had
completed previous marathons had significantly lower best times than
older runners, t (208) = -4.50, p [less than].001.
Predicting Training Differences
In order to examine the interaction of age and motivations on
training variables, four stepwise regressions were conducted. First, the
influence of age group, competitiveness, and personal goal achievement
were examined as to their relationship with the number of attempted
marathons. It was hypothesized that runners who were more motivated by
competition and personal goal achievement would also participate in more
marathons. Because significant differences existed in the number of
marathons completed between older and younger runners, a dichotomous variable representing the age group was entered into the equation first
followed by a stepwise analysis of the remaining variables. This was
done in order to assess the contribution of motivational variables
towards the number of completed marathons after accounting for age group
differences. As can be seen in Table 3, runners self-report endorsement
of competitiveness and an age group by competitiveness interaction term
were significantly related to the number of marathons completed after
accounting for differences attributed to age group membership. Older
runners who reported competition as a more important reason for running
were more likely to have participated in more marathons. However,
younger runners who endorsed competition as a reason for running were
likely to have participated in fewer marathons. Personal goal
achievement and the age group by personal goal achievement interaction
term did not enter the equation.
Second, the influence of age group, competitiveness, and personal
goal achievement were examined in relationship to personal best marathon
finish times. Again, because significant differences existed in the best
finish times between older and younger runners, a dichotomous variable
representing the age group was entered into the equation first followed
by a stepwise analysis of the remaining variables. It was hypothesized
that runners who reported being more motivated by competitiveness and
personal goal achievement would have lower personal best marathon finish
times regardless of age. In fact, runners' scores on the
competitiveness scale did correlate with best finish times (Table 3).
Marathoners who more heavily endorsed competitiveness as a motive for
running had lower personal best finish times regardless of the age group
to which they belonged.
Third, the influence of age group, competitiveness, personal goal
achievement, and health orientation on the number of training miles per
week was examined. It was hypothesized that runners more heavily
endorsing competitiveness or personal goal achievement as motives for
training and running a marathon would train more miles per week. On the
other hand, runners who were more motivated by a general health
orientation would be more representative of recreational runners and
would therefore run fewer training miles. It was also suspected there
may be an interaction between age group and competitive, personal goal
achievement, or general health orientation motives. Three interaction
terms age group X competitiveness, age group X health orientation, and
age group X personal goal achievement were therefore included in the
analysis.
In fact, competitiveness and general health orientation did enter
the equation and predicted training miles per week in the hypothesized
direction. Runners endorsing competitive motives trained more miles per
week while runners emphasizing general fitness as a motive for running
tended to train fewer miles per week. Although personal goal achievement
had a significant simple correlation with training miles per week, it
did not account for a sufficient amount of unique variance to enter the
equation after the competition and general health orientation scales
(Table 3). None of the interaction terms were significantly related to
training miles.
The final regression analysis examined the influence of age group,
affiliation, and recognition on the percent of time runners train alone.
Runners more heavily endorsing social motives for running were
hypothesized to train less time alone than their counterparts. It also
was suspected there may be an interaction between age group and
affiliative or approval motives and therefore two interaction terms were
included in the analysis: age group X affiliation, and age group X
recognition.
Both affiliation and recognition entered the equation and predicted
percent of time training alone in the hypothesized direction. Although
recognition and percent of time training alone had an insignificant
simple correlation, recognition was significantly related to amount of
time training alone after accounting for affiliative motives. Runners
endorsing affiliative motives were more likely to report training with
other runners. Similarly, runners who reported running as a way of
gaining approval or recognition were more likely to run with other
runners during training. The interaction terms did not enter the
equation.
Discussion
Older runners do in fact endorse different reasons for training and
running a marathon when compared to younger runners. As a group, older
runners more heavily endorsed developing and maintaining a level of
fitness and health, including weight benefits, as reasons for running.
On the other hand, younger runners more heavily endorsed personal goal
achievement. The Personal Goal Achievement scale is based on items, such
as: running to beat my best time, trying to push myself beyond my
limits, to improve my running speed, etc. As would be assumed, older
runners are not as influenced by the notion of personal best, but
participate more for general health benefits while younger runners
endorse the achievement of personal running goals as the most pressing
reason for running a marathon. While they differed in terms of personal
goal achievement, older and younger runners did not differ in terms of
their endorsement of competition as a motive for running a marathon. In
fact, neither group heavily endorsed competitio n as an important reason
for training and running a marathon,
Older runners reported life meaning and affiliation with other
runners as more influential motives for training and running a marathon.
The life meaning scale includes items that center around the idea of
running to add a sense of meaning, completeness, peace, and purpose to
life or having time to be alone with the world. The affiliation scale
measures the degree to which runners report participating as a way of
meeting and enjoying other runners' association. We expected that
career runners would develop and report more secondary reasons (second
to physical fitness) for running since they would be more likely to
discover additional positive benefits while participating in an extended
training program.
Although the sample was cross-sectional, these differences may be
indicative of the evolution of running motives over the life-span.
Perhaps younger runners begin running as part of an effort to achieve
personal running goals. With added training time they meet other
runners, spend many hours on isolated roads with little traffic, and
notice the positive psychological and physical benefits of long distance
running. As a result, their attributions of motives evolve such that
they report additional reasons for running with greater emphasis. In
addition, with age they may lose some quickness, endurance, or
competitive drive and thus report less achievement oriented motives and
more life meaning related motives. On the other hand, perhaps our sample
of older runners have always maintained the same set of motives since
beginning to run and the more competitive, goal-oriented peer runners
have discontinued running. In this case, certain motives may lead to
enhanced chances of continued participation in this leisure activity
over a longer period of time. Certainly, the most parsimonious explanation is a cohort effect. Nevertheless, more research regarding
the changing motives of the distance runner is needed.
Differences also emerged on several training variables. Older
runners have been participating in marathons for a longer period of time
and as a result have put in more months of training. Yet, younger
runners have faster personal best marathon finishing times. This is
remarkable considering that older and younger runners train at
approximately the same running speed - equal hours and miles per week.
Perhaps younger runners perform better in competition than older runners
even though they have equivalent amounts of training. Since, younger
runners more heavily endorse personal goal achievement as a reason for
running, they may be more driven to perform well in the race while older
runners train for health reasons and are content to complete the race.
Alternatively, one might suspect that older runners who have
participated in an average of 15 marathons each would have a personal
best time early in their careers that would be comparable to the younger
sample. Maybe the sample of older runners began running later in life.
Or, perhaps, the most hard-driving, competitive runners dropped-out
through attrition or injury and were therefore not a part of the older
sample. Similarly, if a subgroup of runners is systematically eliminated
from one cohort through injury or attrition, differences in motivations
for these runners may be very useful for developing training strategies.
Further examination of participants who run as a result of
competitiveness versus those who view the activity as a recreational or
leisure activity is warranted. Another conclusion might be that the
sample of younger runners included a higher proportion of seriously
competitive runners. At the same time, the standard deviations for both
groups are quite large and indicate the wide variati on that exists
within groups. Clearly, longitudinal research may lead to more
conclusive findings.
The final purpose for this study was to investigate the
relationship between self-report motivations and training variables
while considering both the main effects of age and the possible
interaction of motives with age. Runners who reported being motivated
more by competition with other runners and less by a general health
orientation have different training habits and performance results.
Runners more heavily endorsing competitive reasons for running tended to
train more miles per week, participate in more marathons, and have
faster personal best finishing times. This relationship between
competitiveness and training habits is maintained when controlling for
age.
One interesting exception to the general relationship between
competitiveness and training variables involves the interaction of age
and competition with regards to the total number of completed marathons.
Younger runners who had participated in fewer marathons were more likely
to report competition as a motive for running. Older runners who
reported competition as a motive for running were more likely to have
completed more marathons. Perhaps this relationship is indicative of
runners' perceived chance of success. Older runners are more likely
to place because the number of competitors diminishes in the higher age
groups. At the same time, younger runners who are participating in a
marathon may be former high school or college track athletes who are now
increasing their running distance.
Runners who report running as a way of meeting other runners and
gaining recognition from friends and family tend to spend less time
training alone. This finding generalizes previous research concerning
affiliative motives in other content areas where people with a high need
for affiliation have been found to learn social roles more quickly,
identify faces more quickly, perform better with affiliative incentives,
be involved in more social activities, etc. (McClelland, 1985). The
importance of affiliative motives for participation in team sports has
been duly noted (Butt, 1987), yet affiliation as an important
determinant of a typically isolating activity such as running is often
neglected. Even though nearly every study of runners' self-report
participative motives finds social motives to be important, this is the
first study to indicate that runners from different age groups report
varying levels of affiliative motives as a reason for their continued
involvement in the activity. Not only does this finding have
consequences for exercise compliance and fitness, but research regarding
the participative motives of people involved in other apparently
isolating leisure activities should consider the potential implications
of affiliation.
While some clear differences in older and younger runners'
participative motives and training habits have been identified, the
application of these findings to a developmental theory of motivations
for running must remain tentative. Self-report methods for measuring
human motives and cognitions have been questioned (Nisbett & Wilson,
1977) and certainly additional and ongoing validation of the MOMS is
required. Obvious limitations are also involved when making inferences
regarding the development of motives over time while using a
cross-sectional design. Similarly, other variables (e.g., personality
type) not included in the current study could account for differences in
motivations both between and within groups (i.e., Hinkle, Lyons, &
Burke, 1989). In addition, our sample included a wide variety of runners
in terms of level of competitiveness. Some differences in self-report
motives may exist between elite and recreational runners that confuse the data reported here. In the "long run", however, additional
research may benefit the marathon community as well as all those
interested in leisure activities by delineating the motives for training
and running a marathon and their evolution over time.
References
American College of Sports Medicine. (1990). The recommended
quantity and quality of exercise for developing and maintaining
cardiorespiratory and muscular fitness in healthy adults. Medicine and
Science in Sports and Exercise, 22, 265-274.
Blumenthal, J. A., O'Toole, L. C., & Chang, J. L. (1984).
Is running an analogue of anorexia nervosa? Journal of the American
Medical Association, 252,520-523.
Butt, D. S. (1987). Psychology of sport. New York: Van Nostrand
Reinhold.
Carmack, M.A., & Martens, R. (1979). Measuring commitment to
running: A survey of runners' attitudes and mental states. Journal
of Sport Psychology, I, 25-42.
Clough, P., Sheperd, J., & Maughan, R. (1989). Motives for
participation in recreational running. Journal of Leisure Research, 21,
297-309.
Crandall, R. J. (1980). Motivations for leisure. Journal of Leisure
Research, 12, 45-54.
Curtis, J. & McTeer, W. (1981, January). The motivation for
running. Canadian Runner, 18-19.
Dishman, R. K. (1988). Exercise adherence. Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Dishman, R. K. (1985). Medical psychology in exercise and sport.
Medical Clinics of North America, 69, 123-143.
Hinkle, J. S., Lyons, B., &Burke, K. L. (1989). Manifestation of Type A behavior pattern among aerobic runners. Journal of Sport
Behavior, 12, 131-138.
Johnsgard, K. (1985a). The motivation of the long distance runner:
I. Journal of Sports Medicine, 25, 135-139.
Johnsgard, K. (1985b). The motivation of the long distance runner:
II. Journal of Sports Medicine, 25, 140-143.
Masters, K. S., & Ogles, B. M. (1995). An investigation of the
different motivations of marathon runners with varying degrees of
experience. Journal of Sport Behavior; 18, 69-79.
Masters, K. S., & Ogles, B. M. (1998). Cognitive strategies
relate to injury, motivation, and performance among marathon runners:
Results from two studies. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 10,
281-296.
Masters, K.S., Ogles, B.M., & Jolton, J.A. (1993). The
development of an instrument to measure motivation for marathon running:
The motivations of marathoners scales (MOMS). Research Quarterly for
Exercise and Sport, 64, 134-143.
McClelland, D. C. (1985). Human motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott,
Foresman and Company.
Nisbett, R. E., & 'Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than
we can know: Vrbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review,
27, 533. 543.
Ogles, B. M., Masters, K. S., & Richardson, S. A. (1995).
Obligatory running and gender: An analysis of participative motives and
training habits. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 26, 233-248.
Pargman, D. (1980). The way of the runner: An examination of
motives for running (pp. 90- 98). In R. M. Suinn (Ed.), Psychology in
sports: Methods and applications. Minneapolis: Burgess.
Sallis, J. F., & Hovell, M. F. (1990). Determinants of exercise
behavior. Exercise and Sport Sciences Review, 18, 307-330.
Summers, J. J., Machin, V. J., & Sargent, G. I. (1983).
Psychosocial factors related to marathon running. Journal of Sport
Psychology 5, 314-331.
Summers, J. J., Sargent, G. I., Levey, A. J., & Murray, K. D.
(1982). Middle-aged, non-elite marathon runners: A profile. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54, 963-969.
Yair, G. (1990). The commitments to long distance running and
levels of activity: Personal or structural? Journal of Leisure Research,
22, 213-227.
Means and Standard Deviations on the
Masters-Ogles Marathon Scale (MOMS)
for Older and Younger Runners
Older Runners Younger Runners
MOMS Scale M SD M SD
Life Meaning [*] 3.59 1.62 3.05 1.56
Self-Esteem 4.82 1.33 4.55 1.45
Psychological Coping 3.23 l.55 3.13 1.52
Weight Concern [*] 3.80 1.71 3.22 1.70
Health Orientation [**] 5.22 1.38 4.32 1.63
Recognition 3.32 1.53 3.32 1.62
Affiliation [*] 3.33 1.39 2.83 1.42
Competition 3.03 1.66 3.37 l.6l
Personal Goal Achievement 4.53 1.38 5.23 1.25
(1.)M per item
(*.)p [less than].01
(**.)p [less than].001
Means and Standard Deviations on Training
Variables for Older and Younger Runners
Older Runners Younger Runners
Training Variable M SD M SD
Miles training per week 46.50 14.17 49.19 19.87
Hours training per week 8.91 7.87 8.79 4.99
Months of training [*] 69.66 77.08 42.19 50.85
% time training alone 75.22 28.68 80.32 23.07
# marathons finished [**] 15.08 29.17 2.34 3.82
Best finishing time [**] 225.36 42.01 196.01 33.38
(*.)p[less than].0l
(**.)p[less than].001
Step-Wise Regression Analyses Predicting Number of Marathons
Completed, Best Finishing Time, Miles Training per Week, and
Percent of Time Training Alone
Variable [B.sup.a] r [R.sup.2]
Predicting Number of Marathons Completed
Age Group -.33 .44 .19
Competition 2.08 .26 .29
Age Group X Competition 1.70 .51 .36
Personal Goal Achievement .08
Age Group X Personal Goal .48
Predicting Best Finishing Time
Age Group 12.51 .33 .11
Competition -8.41 -.36 .21
Personal Goal Achievement -.34
Age Group X Competition .30
Age Group X Personal Goal .27
Predicting Miles Training per Week
Competition 3.77 .34 .12
Health Orientation -2.00 -.15 .15
Personal Goal Achievement .27
Age Group X Competition -.10
Age Group X Health Orient -.01
Age Group X Personal Goal -.06
Predicting Percent of Time Training Alone
Affiliation -6.61 -.23 .06
Recognition 3.77 .03 .10
Age Group -.10
Age Group X Affiliation -.03
Age Group X Recognition -.02
Variable [F.sup.b]
Predicting Number of Marathons Completed
Age Group 35.83 [***]
Competition 30.58 [***]
Age Group X Competition 27.48 [***]
Personal Goal Achievement
Age Group X Personal Goal
Predicting Best Finishing Time
Age Group 14.60 [***]
Competition 15.11 [***]
Personal Goal Achievement
Age Group X Competition
Age Group X Personal Goal
Predicting Miles Training per Week
Competition 27.38 [***]
Health Orientation 18.22 [***]
Personal Goal Achievement
Age Group X Competition
Age Group X Health Orient
Age Group X Personal Goal
Predicting Percent of Time Training Alone
Affiliation 12.25 [***]
Recognition 10.45 [***]
Age Group
Age Group X Affiliation
Age Group X Recognition
(a.)B = weights for final equation with all entered variables
(b.)F = F for full equation after each step.
(***.)p[less than].001