Cognitive-affective and behavioral correlates of self-schemata in sport.
Boyd, Michael ; Yin, Zenong
Cognition is recognized as playing an integral role in motivation
and the regulation of sport behavior (Roberts, 1992). A well established
theoretical approach, emanating from the social cognition literature
(see Fiske & Taylor, 1984), yet relatively unexamined in the sport
sciences, is the role self-schemata plays in the onset and maintenance
of behavior. Self-schema theory, initially advanced by Markus (1977),
emphasizes the relevance of one's self-image and how this may not
only influence cognitive processing but behavioral variation in a
particular domain as well.
Schema theory is an information processing approach that assumes
people are active agents in constructing reality. A schema is said to be
a generic knowledge structure, formulated through experience, and
containing information organized about a given concept (Fiske &
Taylor, 1984). Schema may contain attributes concerning a given concept,
as well as a network of interrelationships among these attributes that
relegate how such conceptual knowledge is to be utilized (Hastie, 1981;
Rumelhart & Ortony, 1977; Taylor & Crocker, 1981). Schemata not
only guide the perception and selection of incoming information, but
also influence the organization of such information in memory, and
direct how this information is to be eventually utilized (Fiske &
Taylor, 1984). Once a schema is instantiated, spreading activation provides an intricate knowledge base from which to draw upon (Anderson,
1982). Schemata may encompass conceptual networks including objects,
persons, actions, and social situations (Rumelhart, 1980), but may also
involve self-schemata (Fiske & Taylor, 1984).
Self-schemata are cognitive generalizations about the self, deemed
important, in which an individual perceives oneself to harbor an extreme
point of view (Markus, 1977). Self-schemata are formulated by observing
one's own behavior repeatedly over time and eventually develop into
a framework which filters the input and output of behavioral information
related to the self (Markus, 1977). Furthermore, Markus (1977)
acknowledges that this self-related information is organized into a
cognitive network used for making judgments, decisions, and predictions
about the self, as well as for generating inferences which may go far
beyond information readily available. Self-schemata have been shown to
speed processing time of self-referent information, to enhance the
quality of self-related judgments, and to be more accessible in memory
than other information (Kuiper & Rogers, 1979; Markus, 1977; Rogers,
Kuiper, & Kirker, 1977).
The salience of self-schemata has been well documented in several
domain-specific areas as diverse as independence-dependence (Markus,
1977), body weight (Markus, Hamill, & Sentis, 1987), gender roles
(Markus, Crane, Bernstein, & Siladi, 1982), religious beliefs
(Caccioppo, Petty, & Sidera, 1982), and Type A & B behavioral
patterns (Strube, Berry, Lott, Fogelman, Steinhart, Morgan, &
Davison, 1986). Recently, the salience of exercise self-schemata has
been documented (Kendzierski, 1994), although presently no studies exist
investigating self-schemata in the sport domain.
Kendzierski (1988) found that exercise self-schematics reported to
have exercised more frequently and engaged in more exercise activities,
were more committed to exercise, and had more tricks and strategies to
facilitate exercise behavior than either aschematics (those without an
exercise self-schema) or non-exercise schematics (those with a
self-schema for not exercising). Exercise schematics have also been
found to endorse more exercise words as being self-descriptive, recall
more instances of exercise behavior, predict that they would exercise
more in the future, and are more likely to initiate an exercise regime
than either aschematics or non-exercise schematics (Kendzierski, 1990).
Although the paradigm has been applied to exercise behavior, presently
theorists have failed to document self-schemata in sport.
In the area of sport expertise, however, considerable research
attention has been directed to the examination of the cognitive and
motor responses which underscore expert sport performance (see
Abernathy, Burgess-Limerick, & Parks, 1994; McPherson, 1994; Thomas,
1994). Sport experts, compared to novice performers, are characterized as possessing more sport-specific semantic knowledge, which is extremely
organized, containing more defining features, and having more links
interrelating these semantic knowledge structures (French & Thomas,
1987; Thomas, French, & Humphries, 1986). McPherson (1994) has
described such expert sport knowledge, "as schemata...problem
representations...[and] node-link networks" (p. 228).
Expertise involves the utilization of two types of knowledge,
declarative and procedural (Anderson, 1982; Chi, Glaser, & Farr,
1988). Declarative knowledge refers to the "what to do" or
response selection component, whereas procedural knowledge refers to the
"doing it" or response execution. The former would include,
for example, knowledge about rules, player positions, and scoring.
Procedural knowledge rather involves decision-making and the selection
of an appropriate action given certain conditions. Declarative knowledge
operates in the form of a propositional network consisting of nodes and
links where each node represents a given concept connected by links
(Anderson, 1982; Chi & Rees, 1983). Studies in sport expertise
indicate that experts possess more declarative knowledge in sport than
do novices (French & Thomas, 1987; McPherson & French, 1991;
McPherson & Thomas, 1989). Moreover, the development of declarative
knowledge leads to greater procedural knowledge in the form of better
decision-making skills in game play (French & Thomas, 1987;
McPherson & French, 1991).
Several methods of measuring declarative knowledge in sport have
been developed. These include sport knowledge tests, free-recall tasks,
speed-accuracy decision-making skills, as well as verbal protocol of
planned action in simulated or active game competition (McPherson,
1994). Research on expertise has shown sport knowledge tests to be a
reliable and valid indicator of declarative knowledge in the sport
domain. Sport knowledge tests have been shown to discriminate expert and
novice performers, concerning sport-specific knowledge and superior
decision-making ability, among youth tennis and basketball players as
well as college age tennis players (French & Thomas, 1987; McPherson
and French, 1991; McPherson & Thomas, 1989). Knowledge tests have
also been developed in order to assess high and low baseball knowledge
(Chiesi, Spilich, & Voss, 1979; Nevett, French, Spurgeon, Rink,
& Graham, 1994). In light of the similarities between sport experts
and schematics, including the possession of a well developed
domain-specific propositional network of conceptual knowledge, as well
as superior decision-making ability, it would be predicted that sport
schematics would possess declarative knowledge in sport similar in
quality to that of experts.
Kendzierski (1990) has made a call to examine the relationship
between self-schemata in the sport and exercise domain and other
cognitive constructs including self-efficacy, self-motivation, and
self-esteem. Self-efficacy is a cognitive mechanism which mediates the
strength of an individual's conviction that one can successfully
execute a particular task (Bandura, 1977). Feltz (1992) contends that
the most widely used theory for examining self-confidence in sport has
been self-efficacy, which determines the challenge and perseverance an
individual is willing to undertake in the presence of difficulty.
Self-efficacy has consistently been shown to be associated to
performance in sport (Feltz, 1982, 1988; Feltz & Mugno, 1983;
McAuley & Gill, 1983; Weinberg, Gould, & Jackson, 1979; Weiss,
Weise, & Klint, 1989). In the sport psychology literature,
self-efficacy has been used synonymously with self-confidence (Roberts,
1992). It would be expected that sport schematics would possess more
self-efficacy, in the form of self-confidence, than either aschematics
and non-schematics.
In the model, enjoyment should be included as a dependent variable
because of its contemporary significance to sport commitment (Scanlan
& Simons, 1992). Enjoyment, or the lack thereof, has been shown to
be related to both participation and dropout in sport (Gill, Gross,
& Huddleston, 1983; Gould, Feltz, Horn & Weiss, 1982). Sport
enjoyment has also been reported to be associated to perceived
competence in the physical domain as well as preference for challenge
(Chalip, Csikszentmihalyi, Kleiber, & Larson, 1984; Scanlan &
Lewthwaite, 1986; Wankel & Kreisel, 1985). Schematics would be
predicted to express greater levels of sport enjoyment than
non-schematics, as by definition the sport domain is highly important to
the image they have of themselves.
Finally, self-reported behavioral measures of participation in
sport will be assessed. Past research examining exercise self-schemata
has found that exercise schematics recalled more instances of exercise
and predicted that they would exercise more in the future than either
aschematics or non-schematics (Kendzierski, 1990). based on this
research, it is predicted that sport schematics would report higher
frequency of participation in their sport than non-schematics.
Given the notable research concerning self-schemata in the social
cognition literature and its influential effect on categorical accessibility, emotional valence, and the regulation of behavior,
presently there is a need to examine self-schemata in the sport domain.
The purpose of the present study was to document self-schemata in the
baseball domain and to examine the cognitive (declarative knowledge,
self-confidence), behavioral (frequency of participation), and affective (enjoyment) correlates of self-schemata in baseball. Sport schemata was
operationalized in accordance with the universally accepted model
(Markus, 1977), although due to the exploratory nature of the study no
specific hypotheses were generated.
Method
Subjects
Subjects were 165 undergraduate students (78 females and 87 males)
enrolled in first aid and safety courses at a South Texas university.
Mean age of the subjects was 24.84 years (SD = 4.97). Informed consent
was established as subjects were told that their participation was
voluntary and anonymous. Subjects were requested to answer study
questions in an honest manner.
Measures
Sport Self-Schemata. In order to assess whether participants were
schematic for baseball (schematics), schematic for not playing baseball
(non-schematics), or aschematic, the methodology (described in detail
below) devised by Markus (1977) was used. This methodology employs three
key phrases, initially designed to assess independence-dependence
self-schemata, and later modified to measure self-schemata in domains as
diverse as body weight (Markus et al., 1987), gender roles, (Markus et
al., 1982), Type A and B behavior (Strube et al., 1986), and exercise
(Kendzierski, 1988, 1990). Participants in the present study were
requested to respond to the three key phrases, modified from Markus
(1977) original items: "someone who plays baseball/softball
regularly," "someone who keeps playing
baseball/softball," and "someone who is physically active in
baseball/softball", embedded within a larger scale. The Markus
(1977) protocol used requires subjects to indicate on an 11-point
response scale, ranging from I (does not describe me), to 11 (describes
me), whether each of the three items is self-descriptive or not.
Using the Markus (1977) methodology, subjects were also requested
to indicate the importance of each descriptor phrase "to the image
you have of yourself, regardless of whether or not the trait describes
you." Markus (1977) contends that the development of
domain-specific self-schemata requires that not only must an individual
categorize oneself as active in a given domain, but that the category
must also be important to one's self-image. Utilizing the method of
categorization suggested by Markus (1977), schematics were categorized as those who rated at least two of the three self-schemata items as
highly self-descriptive (scores 8-11) and also rated at least two of the
three descriptors as extremely important to their self-image (scores
8-11). To be classified a non-schematic, one must have rated at least
two of the three self-schemata items as extremely non-descriptive
(scores 1-4) and also have rated at least two of the descriptors as
highly important to their self-image (scores 8-11). Finally, aschematics
were classified as those who rated at least two of the three
self-schemata items as moderately self-descriptive (scores 4-7) and also
rated at least two of the three descriptors as not highly important to
the image they have of self (scores 1-7). Those participants who failed
to fall into any of the three specified self-schemata categories were
left unclassified.
Baseball Knowledge. A subset of the Baseball Knowledge Test (Nevett
et al., 1994) designed to assess players' declarative knowledge in
baseball (and softball) was used to quantify sport-specific knowledge of
rules, player positions, strategies and decision-making in the game. The
16-item test provides a multiple-choice format to the respondent. The
Baseball Knowledge Test has been shown to discriminate between expert
and novice baseball players regarding degree of baseball knowledge
(Nevett et al., 1994). The authors report acceptable psychometric properties of the instrument. The internal reliability coefficient (Cronbach, 1951) for the baseball knowledge scale in the present study
was .81.
Trait Sport Confidence. Utilizing a slightly modified version of
Vealey's (1986) Trait Sport Confidence Inventory, subjects
indicated their level of trait sport confidence in the baseball-softball
domain. The sport-specific trait confidence instrument has been used
extensively in the sport psychology literature. Items of the original
instrument were altered by replacing the word "sport" with the
words "baseball/softball". Vealey (1986) reports acceptable
psychometric properties of the instrument including construct validity as well as internal reliability. Cronbach (1951) alpha was .96 for the
trait sport confidence inventory.
Sport Affect. Subjects self-reported the degree of their
sport-specific enjoyment by completing a scale developed by Duda and
Nicholls (1992). The instrument consists of satisfaction/enjoyment and
boredom subscales. Duda and Nicholls (1992) report adequate psychometric
properties of the scale. Items were slightly modified by replacing the
word "sports" with the words baseball/softball". Internal
reliability coefficients of .95 and .89 were observed for the
satisfaction/enjoyment and boredom subscales, respectively.
Frequency of Sport Participation. Subjects were requested to
indicate the number of years they had played any type of organized
baseball or softball, including youth sport leagues, high school and
college athletics. Respondents also indicated how often they had
participated in baseball or softball both in the past 30 days and 6
months, as well as how often they intended to play in the next 30 days
and 6 months.
Results
Categorization of Subjects
Subjects were assigned to one of four groups in accordance with the
Markus (1977) established procedure explained above, dependent upon
their responses on the self-schemata scale. These groups were labeled as
schematics, non-schematics, aschematics, and those whose responses did
not fall into any of the other three groups. Of the original 165
subjects, a final sample yielded 21 schematics, 10 non-schematics, and
16 aschematics. The remainder of the sample failed to fall into any of
the previous three groups, and were therefore deleted from further
analysis. Accordingly, a total of 47 subjects were selected to be
included in the final analysis(1).
Results of One- Way F- Tests
In order to examine differences among the three self-schemata
groups, one-way F-tests were conducted on each of the remaining
variables in accordance with established procedure (Kendzierski, 1994;
Markus, 1977). Analysiswise alpha levels were controlled and adjusted to
alpha [less than] .006, following the suggestion of Cliff (1987).
Further probing was performed using the Student-Newman-Keuls test (SPSS Inc., 1988) to identify specific group variations. Table I displays the
means and standard deviations of the dependent measures.
Significant overall differences were found among the three
self-schemata groups in baseball knowledge, F(2,44) = 6.72, p [less
than] .003. Follow-up Student-Newman-Keuls comparisons indicated that
schematics (M = 9.43) were significantly more knowledgeable than
non-schematics in the baseball domain (M = 4.70).
Regarding trait sport confidence, overall differences were also
indicated among the three groups, F(2,44) = 8.63, p [less than] .001.
Post-hoc analysis revealed that schematics (M = 106.06) expressed
significantly more trait sport confidence than either aschematics (M =
86.14) or non-schematics (M = 76.90).
An overall significant F-test also revealed differences among the
three groups with regard to level of satisfaction/enjoyment, F(2,44) =
6.56, p [less than] .003. Follow-up Student-Newman-Keuls analysis
indicated that schematics (M = 31.03) enjoyed their participation to a
greater [TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE 1 OMITTED] extent than did
non-schematics (M = 26.20). Furthermore, aschematics (M = 30.75) also
reported significantly higher levels of satisfaction/enjoyment than did
non-schematics. No significant overall difference was observed among the
three groups on the boredom measure.
Finally, frequency of participation was analyzed among the three
self-schema groups. A significant overall F-test indicated differences
among the groups on years of experience, F(2,44) = 9.24, p [less than]
.0001. Post-hoc analysis indicated that schematics (M = 12.05) reported
they had played the sport for more years than did either aschematics (M
= 6.10) or non-schematics (M = 2.70). Schematics reported to have
participated more times in the previous 30 days, F(2,44) = 6.24, p [less
than] .003. Post-hoc comparisons revealed that schematics (M = 5.19)
reported to have played more often in the past 30 days than either
aschematics (M = 1.63) or non-schematics (M = 0.20). Schematics also
reported to have played more often in the past 6 months, F(2,44) = 9.93,
p [less than] .0001 than the other two groups. Post-hoc analysis
indicated that schematics (M = 25.43) reported to have played more in
the past 6 months than either aschematics (M = 8.13) or non-schematics
(M = 1.20). A preliminary F-test indicated an overall difference among
the three groups regarding anticipation to play in the next 30 days,
F(2,44) = 7.11, p [less than] .002. Schematics anticipated that they
would play more often than either aschematics or non-schematics. Means
for the three groups were 5.10, 1.13, and .20 respectively. Overall
differences were also observed regarding anticipation to participate in
the next 6 months, F(2,44) = 3.77, p [less than] .0001. Post-hoc
comparisons revealed that schematics anticipated that they would play
more often in the next 6 months than either aschematics or
non-schematics. Means for the three groups were 22.25, 5.38, and 1.30
respectively. No significant differences emerged between aschematics or
non-schematics on any of the participation measures (see Table 1).
Discussion
The results demonstrated that self-schemata in sport not only
serves to facilitate accessibility to domain-specific sport knowledge,
but is also intimately associated to reliable measures of cognitive and
affective correlates of the construct. Subjects who were schematic for
baseball/softball, compared to non-schematics, were found to possess
more declarative knowledge concerning decision-making and strategies of
the game. Schematics also reported possessing higher levels of trait
sport confidence than did either non-schematics or aschematics. Both
schematics and aschematics expressed that they enjoyed their sport
participation to a greater extent than did non-schematics.
The finding that schematics possessed greater sport knowledge than
non-schematics reflects related research indicating experts in sport to
have greater declarative knowledge than novices (French & Thomas,
1987; McPherson & Thomas, 1989). The development of declarative
knowledge apparently leads to greater procedural knowledge in the form
of decision-making ability in sport (McPherson & French, 1991).
Schematics were not, however, found to express significantly more sport
declarative knowledge than aschematics, results which were not highly
unexpected. Aschematics, by definition, consider an attribute to be
moderately self-descriptive, perhaps indicative of at least some
experience in the domain. Whether this is in the form of overt participation, of a vicarious nature, or a combination of the two,
apparently it may lead to profound gains in decision-making and
strategic abilities over and above those individuals who are
non-schematic. For instance, Kendzierski (1990) reported that both
exercise schematics and aschematics expressed shorter response latencies
to exercise stimuli than did non-schematics, but were, however,
insignificantly differentiated. Thus, in aschematics, instantiation of a
moderately developed schema may serve to facilitate accessibility to
schema relevant knowledge as well. Together, these findings are
noteworthy as they enjoin the development of self-schemata to expertise
in sport.
The trait sport confidence measure was found to distinguish the
three self-schema groups in the desired direction. Kendzierski (1990)
suggests research is needed in order to examine the relationship between
self-schemata and variables such as self-efficacy in an effort to
enhance the discriminative validity of the self-schema construct.
Results indicating that schematics possessed more trait sport confidence
than either aschematics or non-schematics is quite consistent with the
literature concerning perceived ability and participation motivation in
sport (Roberts, 1984, 1992). Possession of a self-schema is said to be
partially dependent upon past participation in a given domain (Markus,
1977). It certainly follows that perceived ability, or confidence, would
be positively related to the formation of self-schemata and experience
in the sport domain. The behavioral data further support this tenet.
Significant patterns emerged regarding the self-reported behavioral
data. Schematics reported more years of participation as well as more
participation in past months than either of the other two groups.
Schematics anticipated that they would participate more in the future
than either aschematics or non-schematics. Kendzierski (1990) reported
similar results with exercise schematics. The behavioral data thus
provided preliminary evidence that ongoing participation in the sport
domain is highly associated to self-schemata. However, because both
self-schemata and frequency of participation were assessed concurrently,
whether possession of a self-schema induces participation or if ongoing
participation precludes the development of self-schemata remains an
unanswered issue.
Regarding enjoyment, both schematics and aschematics reported that
they enjoyed their sport experience to a greater extent than
non-schematics. Sport participation, dropout, and perceived competence
have all been found to be associated to sport enjoyment (Gill et al.,
1983; Gould et al., 1982). If schematics are considered to be active
participants, as the behavioral data suggest, and also express greater
confidence in the sport domain, it certainly follows that they would
report greater sport enjoyment than non-schematics.
An argument could be advanced that self-schema and its associated
correlates merely reflect experience in sport. Markus (1977), however,
eloquently argues that although experience is deemed necessary for the
formation of self-schemata, it is by no means sufficient. According to Markus, it is the perceived importance of the dimension, as well as
experience, which separates those individuals who do and do not possess
self-schemata. Thus, although two individuals may have similar
experience, it is the person who perceives a domain to be important to
their self-image who is self-schematic. Given this line of reasoning, it
follows that interventions directed at developing self-schemata in those
who are aschematic and altering self-schemata of those who are
non-schematic would be a realistic goal for future inquiry.
In order to increase the importance of the sport domain, value
should be placed on teaching motor skill improvement rather than
focusing on social comparison and performance outcome, in order to
induce a sense of perceived ability (Duda, 1992). Providing meaningful
experiences in sport can facilitate the development of confidence and
subsequently enhance the importance of sport for those, who are as of
yet, not schematic in the sport domain. Enhancing the image one has of
self as a sport participant may not only serve to increase sport
importance but also instill greater confidence in sport, which as the
results suggest, are intimately associated to self-schemata.
In summary, this research provides a promising avenue of inquiry
for understanding the role self-knowledge plays in the regulation of
sport behavior. From a practical standpoint, it also serves to provide
insight into functional interventions with the goal to increase
participation in organized sport. Through the identification of those
individuals in need of attention or development of self-schema in sport,
this line of research has the potential to provide a meaningful
contribution. For those who presently disdain their self-image in sport
and seek to change it, the implications of this research are
far-reaching.
Note
1 The final sample consisted of 32 male and 15 female subjects. A
one-way F-test indicated no significant gender differences on all
variables of interest.
References
Abernathy, B., Burgess-Limerick, R., & Parks, S. (1994).
Contrasting approaches to the study of motor expertise. Quest, 46,
186-198.
Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill.
Psychological Review, 89, 369-406.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Sidera, J. A. (1982). The
effects of a salient self-schema on the evaluation of proattitudinal
editorials: Top-down versus bottom-up message processing. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 324-338.
Chalip, L., Csikszentmihalyi, M., Kleiber, D., & Larson, R.
(1984). Variations of experience in formal and informal sport. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 55 (2), 109-116.
Chi, M. T. H., & Rees, E. T. (1983). A learning framework for
development. In M. T. H. Chi (Ed.), Contributions in human development
(Vol. 9, pp. 71-107). Basel: S. Karger.
Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (1988). The nature of
expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Chiesi, H. L., Spilich, G. J., & Voss, J. F. (1979).
Acquisition of domain-related information in relation to high and low
domain knowledge. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18,
257-273.
Cliff, N. (1987). Analyzing Multivariate Data. San Diego, CA:
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Cronbach, L. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure
of tests. Psychometrika, 16, 297-334.
Duda, J. L. (1992). Motivation in sport setting: A goal perspective
approach. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in Sport and Exercise (pp.
3-30). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Duda, J. L., & Nicholls, J. G. (1992). Dimensions of
achievement motivation in schoolwork and sport. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 84, 290-299.
Feltz, D. L. (1982). Path analysis of the causal elements in
Bandura's theory of self-efficacy and an anxiety-based model of
avoidance behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42,
764-781.
Feltz, D. L. (1988). Gender differences in the causal elements of
self-efficacy on a high avoidance motor task. Journal of Sport
Psychology, 10, 151-166.
Feltz, D. L. (1992). Understanding motivation in sport: A
self-efficacy perspective. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in Sport
and Exercise (pp. 93-105). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Feltz, D. L., & Mugno, D. A. (1983). A replication of the path
analysis of the causal elements in Bandura's theory of
self-efficacy and the influence of autonomic perception. Journal of
Sport Psychology, 5, 263-277.
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1984). Social cognition. New
York: Random House.
French, K. E., & Thomas, J.P. (1987). The relation of knowledge
to children's basketball performance. Journal of Sport Psychology,
8, 15-32.
Gill, D. L., Gross, J. B., & Huddleston, S. (1983).
Participation motivation in youth sport. International Journal of Sport
Psychology, 14, 1-14.
Gould, D., Feltz, D., Horn, T., & Weiss, M. (1982). Reasons for
sport attrition in competitive youth swimming. Journal of Sport
Behavior, 5, 155-165.
Hastie, R. (1981). Schematic principles in human memory. In E. T.
Higgins, C. P. Herman, & M.P. Zanna (Eds.), Social cognition: The
Ontario symposium (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Kendzierski, D. K. (1988). Self-schemata and exercise. Basic and
Applied Social Psychology, 9 (1), 45-59.
Kendzierski, D. K. (1990). Exercise self-schemata: Cognitive and
behavioral correlates. Health Psychology, 9 (1), 69-82.
Kendzierski, D. K. (1994). Schema theory: An information processing
focus. In R. K. Dishman (Ed.), Advances in exercise adherence (pp.
137-159). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Kupier, N. A., & Rogers, T. B. (1979). Encoding of personal
information: Self-other differences. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 37, 499-514.
Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about
the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 499-514.
Markus, H., Crane, M., Berstein, S., & Siladi, M. (1982).
Self-schemas and gender. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
42, 38-50.
Markus, H. Hamill, R., & Sentis, K. P. (1987). Thinking fat:
Self-schematas for body weight and the processing of weight relevant
information. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17, 50-71.
McAuley, E., & Gill, D. (1983). Reliability and validity of the
physical self-efficacy scale in a competitive sport setting. Journal of
Sport Psychology, 5,410-418.
McPherson, S. L. (1994). The development of sport expertise:
Mapping the tactical domain. Quest, 46, 223-240.
McPherson, S. L., & French, K. E. (1991). Changes in cognitive
strategies and motor skill in tennis. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 13, 26-41.
McPherson, S. L., & Thomas, J. R. (1989). Relation of knowledge
and performance in boys' tennis: Age and expertise. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 190-211.
Nevett, M. E., French, K. E., Spurgeon, J. H., Rink, J. E., &
Graham, K. C. (1994, May). Skill and cognitive contributions to
children's baseball performance across age and player positions.
Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Alliance for
Health, Physical Education, Recreation, and Dance, Washington, DC.
Roberts, G. C. (1984). Achievement motivation in children's
sport. In J. G. Nicholls (Ed.), Advances in motivation and achievement:
Vol. 3 The development of achievement and motivation (pp. 251-281).
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Roberts, G. C. (1992). Motivation in sports and exercise:
Conceptual constraints and consequence. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.),
Motivation in Sport and Exercise (pp. 3-30). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Rogers, T. B., Kupier, N. A., & Kirker, W. S. (1977).
Self-referenced and the encoding of personal information. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 677-688.
Rumelhart, D. L. (1980). Schemata: The building blocks of
cognition. In R Spiro, B. Bruce, & W. Brewer (Eds.), Theoretical
issues in reading comprehension (pp. 161-188). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Rumelhart, D.E., & Ortony, A. (1977). The representation of
knowledge in memory. In R. C. Anderson, R. J. Spiro, & W. E.
Montague (Eds.), Schooling and the Acquisition of Knowledge (pp.
99-135). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Scanlan, T. K., & Lewthwaite, R. (1986). Social psychological
aspects of competition for male youth sport participants: IV. Predictors
of sport enjoyment. Journal of Sport Psychology, 8, 25-35.
Scanlan, T. K., & Simons, J.P. (1992). The construct of sport
enjoyment. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in Sport and Exercise (pp.
199-215). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
SPSS Inc. (1988). SPSS-X User's Guide (3rd ed.). Chicago, IL.
Strube, M. J., Berry, J. M., Lott, C. L., Fogelman, R., Stienbert,
G., Morgan, S., & Davidson, L. (1986). Self-schematic representation
of type A and B behavior patterns. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 51, 170-180.
Taylor, S. E., & Crocker, J. (1981). Schematic bases of social
information processing. In E. T. Higgins, C. P. Herman, & M.P. Zanna
(Eds.), Social cognition: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Thomas, K. T. (1994). The development of sport expertise: From
Leeds to MVP legend. Quest, 46, 199- 210.
Thomas, J. R., French, K. E., & Humphries, C. A. (1986).
Knowledge development and sport skill performance: Directions for motor
behavior research. Journal of Sport Psychology, 8, 259-272.
Vealey, R. S. (1986). Conceptualization of sport confidence and
competitive orientation: Preliminary investigation and instrument
development. Journal of Sport Psychology, 8, 221-246.
Wankel, L. M., & Kreisel, P.S. J. (1985). Factors underlying
enjoyment of sport and age group comparisons. Journal of Sport
Psychology, 7, 51-64.
Weinberg, R., Gould, D., & Jackson, A. (1979). Expectations and
performance: An empirical test of Bandura's self-efficacy theory.
Journal of Sport Psychology, 1, 320-331.
Weiss, M. R., Wiese, D. M., & Klint, K. A. (1989). Head over
heels with success: The relationship between self-efficacy and
performance in competitive youth gymnastics. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 11, 444-451.