Social Physique Anxiety and Eating Disorder Correlates Among Female Athletic and Nonathletic Populations.
Hausenblas, Heather A. ; Mack, Diane E.
Leary and his colleagues (1994) have suggested that eating
pathologies stem in part from self-presentational concerns involving the
physique. Due to the demands of the sport, female divers may have strong
physical self-presentational concerns and be at-risk for developing
eating pathologies. The purpose of the present study was to examine
physical self-presentation (i.e., social physique anxiety) and eating
disorder correlates among female divers. To this end, a sample of 36
elite female divers were compared to an athletic control group (i.e.,
lacrosse, volleyball, & soccer; n = 39) and a nonathletic control
group (n = 39) on the attitudinal, behavioral, and psychological
subscales of the Eating Disorder Inventory-2 (EDI-2; Garner, 1991) and
the Social Physique Anxiety Scale (SPAS; Hart, Leary, & Rejeski,
1989). A one-way ANCOVA with Body Mass Index as the covariate revealed
that the divers had significantly lower SPAS scores (M = 30.69, SD =
8.49) compared to the athletic control group (M = 37.53, SD = 8.78) and
the nonathletic control group (M = 38.84, SD = 9.88), F(2, 106) = 3.88,
p [less than] .05. In contrast, a one-way MANCOVA with Body Mass Index
as the covariate revealed that the divers, the athletic control group,
and the nonathletic control group did not differ significantly on the
EDI-2 subscales, Wilks's Lambda = .85, F(16,45) = .95, ns.
Historical evidence from 1959 to 1988 has illustrated that there
has been a continual shift toward an increasingly thinner physique in
Western society (Garner, Garfinkel, Schwartz, & Thompson, 1980;
Wiseman, Gray, Mosimann, & Ahrens, 1992). Currently the idealized physique encompasses both a thin and physically fit physique. This
results in increased pressures for individuals--particularly women--to
conform to unrealistic physical standards. In an attempt to achieve the
'ideal' physique women may engage in pathological dieting and
eating behaviors (Brownell, 1991).
The most serious consequences of pathological dieting and eating
behaviors are eating disorders (Wilson & Eldredge, 1992). Eating
disorders are multifaceted and originate from a diverse set of
antecedents (e.g., biological, psychological, and sociocultural; cf.
Johnson, 1994). Leary, Tchividjian, and Kraxberger (1994) have suggested
that self-presentational concerns may be another antecedent of eating
disorders. Self-presentation involves the selective presentation and
omission of aspects of the self to create desired impressions and to
avoid undesired impressions (Leary, 1992). Thus, it is conceivable that
people's concerns with how they are regarded by others (i.e., thin
physique) may increase their risk of pathological eating and dieting
behaviors in an attempt to convey positive impressions.
For example, Hayes and Ross (1987) suggested that excessive
concerns about one's social image may lead women to starve or purge
themselves in an attempt to be thin. Further, women with eating
disorders also tend to a have high need for social approval, experience
high social anxiety and low self-esteem (APA, 1994; Gross & Rosen,
1988; Katzman & Wolchik, 1984). As self-esteem, social anxiety, and
the need for social approval are also concomitants of
self-presentational concerns, it is possible that a relationship exists
between eating disorders and self-presentation (Leary et al., 1994).
Thus, a self-presentational understanding of the etiology of
women's concerns with eating and dieting behaviors may have
implications in identifying the extreme manifestations of these
behaviors--eating disorders (Pliner & Chaiken, 1990).
For example, researchers have found a relationship between the
self-presentational concern of social physique anxiety (i.e., the
anxiety that results from the social evaluation of one's physique;
Hart, Leary, & Rejeski, 1989) and eating disorder correlates (Diehl,
Johnson, Petrie, & Rogers, 1995; Reel & Gill, 1996). Further, it
has been suggested that eating disordered individuals have a fear of
negative evaluation that may stem from a fear that others will perceive
their weight or physical appearance as inconsistent with social norms
(Bulik, Beidel, Duchmann, Weltzin, & Kaye, 1991). Thus, it is
conceivable that social physique anxiety may represent an additional
risk factor for the development of disordered eating, especially for
individuals who are frequently in physique evaluative situations. The
current study examined the relationship between social physique anxiety
and eating disorder correlates.
One population that may be at-risk for both self-presentational
concerns regarding the physique and eating disorders are competitive
divers. In regards to self-presentational concerns, divers compete and
train in situations where self-presentational concerns regarding the
physique may be more pronounced. For example, divers must train and
compete in revealing attire that places the physique on evaluative
display and often magnifies bodily flaws (Reel & Gill, 1996).
Second, diving is a sport where success is determined through subjective
evaluation by judges (Thompson & Sherman, 1993). Therefore, besides
obvious physical skill, divers ability to convey impressions of grace,
strength, agility, and attractiveness can affect their evaluation by the
judges (Brownell & Steen, 1992). Third, the individual (versus team)
nature of the sport activity, in which accountability for performance is
readily discernable, may result in heighten emphasis of the physique
(Carron & Prapavessis, 1997). Thus, due to the heightened focus on
appearance, divers may experience social physique anxiety in an attempt
to convey positive impressions. The present study explored this
possibility.
In regards to eating disorders, it has been suggested that divers
represent a population at-risk for eating pathologies due to the
aesthetic and subjective nature of the sport (Thompson & Sherman,
1993). However, despite suggestions that divers are at high-risk for
eating disorders, this hypothesis has received little empirical
attention. When divers have been examined in relation to eating disorder
correlates they have been studied within a larger pool of athletes
(e.g., Petrie, 1996; Sundgot-Borgen, 1994). Thus, the examination of
eating disorder correlates within a large population of divers is
warranted.
In general, although studies have found athletes to be at high-risk
for eating disorders, just as many studies have come to opposite
conclusions (e.g., Sundgot-Borgen, 1993; Wilkins, Boland, &
Albinson, 1991). Inherent in this equivocality is a host of
methodological and conceptual problems such as the use of invalidated instruments (Stein, 1991), inappropriately matched controls (Ashley,
Smith, Robinson, & Richardson, 1996), inconsistent classification of
sports into various categories, and failure to consider actual physical
size as a covariate (Petrie, 1996).
In summary, the purpose of the present study was to examine
self-presentational concerns related to the physique (i.e., social
physique anxiety) and eating disorder correlates among female divers.
First, a well-validated psychological scale was used to assess eating
disorder correlates--the Eating Disorder Inventory-2 (Garner, 1991).
Second, appropriate match controls for age and sex were examined.
Specifically, divers, who compete in a judged event where appearance
(i.e., body weight and shape) plays a central role in the athlete's
outcome, were compared with an athletic control group (i.e., volleyball,
soccer, & lacrosse) where appearance does not directly influence
performance, and a nonathletic control group. Finally, an estimate of
physical size was used as a covariate to control for the potential
influence of physical size in regards to the eating disorder correlates
(Petrie, 1996) and social physique anxiety (Hart et al., 1989).
It was hypothesized that due to the performance evaluative nature
of diving, competitive divers would experience greater social physique
anxiety than the athletic control group and nonathletic control group.
Second, due to the inconsistent results in the literature no a priori hypotheses were made regarding the correlates of eating disorders among
the divers, the athletic control group, and the nonathletic control
group. Finally, it was hypothesized that social physique anxiety would
be predicted by the attitudinal, psychological, and behavioral
correlates of eating disorders (as measured by the EDI-2 subscales).
Method
Participants
Participants were 114 volunteer female athletes and nonathletes.
The female divers were 36 elite athletes competing at the national or
provincial level (age M = 16.33 years, SD = 2.44). The athletic control
group was comprised of 39 elite athletes competing in either volleyball,
lacrosse, or soccer at the regional or provincial level (age M = 17.35
years, SD = 2.85). Approximately 87% of the athletes were attending high
school. The nonathletic control group (n = 39) were high school students
who were not currently engaging in more than one hour of physical
activity a week (age M = 17.38 years, SD = .49). The three groups did
not significantly differ in regards to age, F(2, 1ll) = 2.80, ns.
Measures
Eating Disorder Inventory-2 (EDI-2). The 8 subscales of the EDI-2
that reflect the attitudinal, behavioral, and psychological correlates
of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa were used (Garner, 1991). The
Drive for Thinness subscale assesses excessive concerns with dieting,
preoccupation with weight, as well as the extreme pursuit of thinness.
The Bulimia subscale provides a measure of the tendency to engage in
binging that may be followed by an impulse to vomit. The Body
Dissatisfaction subscale measures dissatisfaction with the shape of body
parts such as hips, buttocks, and the belief that these parts are too
big or fat. The Ineffectiveness subscale measures feelings of
worthlessness and lack of control over one's life. The
Perfectionism subscale measures the extent to which one believes that
personal achievements should be superior. The Interpersonal Distrust
subscale assesses an individual's general feeling of alienation and
reluctance to form close relationships. The Interoceptive Awareness
subscale assesses confusion and apprehension in recognizing and
accurately responding to emotional states. And the Maturity Fears
subscale measures the desire to retreat to the security of childhood.
High scores of these subscales relative to published norms, indicate
that an individual may be at greater risk for developing eating
disorders.
The EDI-2 has acceptable psychometric properties. Measures of
internal consistency have been reported to range from 0.80 to 0.92
(Garner, 1991), while test-retest reliabilities were between 0.65 to
0.97 (Wear & Pratz, 1987).
Social Physique Anxiety Scale (SPAS). The SPAS is a 12-item
self-report inventory developed by Hart et al. (1989) to measure social
physique anxiety. Participants are asked to indicate the degree to which
statements are characteristic or true of them using a 5-point Likert
scale. The SPAS has demonstrated adequate construct validity (Hart et
al., 1989; McAuley & Burman, 1993), test-retest reliability,
internal consistency (alpha = .90), and minimal social desirability bias (Hart et al., 1989). [1]
Body Mass Index (BMI) and Demographic Information. Participants
provided information concerning their current height, weight, and age.
The BMI was calculated from the participants self-reported measures of
height and weight because a direct measurement of body fat could not be
obtained. The BMI has been suggested as a better measure of nutritional
status (i.e., under or overweight) than standard weight tables (Garrow,
1981; in Beaumont, A1-Alami, & Touyz, 1988) because it is both
objective and easy to calculate and is a useful criterion for
identifying those with eating disorders (Beaumont et al., 1988). While
BMIs' based on self-reports could conceivably be unreliable,
previous research has indicated high correlations between self-reported
and actual measures of height and weight (Davis, 1990).
Procedure
Participants were informed that they were participating in a
research study whose stated purpose was to assess attitudes towards
their physique and eating. Participants were informed that completion of
the questionnaire was voluntary and confidential. If less than 18 years
of age, confirmation of parental/guardian consent was obtained. Once
consent was obtained the participants completed the questionnaires and
either returned them directly to the experimenters or placed them in a
sealed envelope to be mailed to the experimenters.
Results
Reliability of Measurement
Cronbach's alpha was calculated on the SPAS and EDI-2
subscales. The SPAS was found to have satisfactory internal consistency
with an alpha coefficient of .88. The 8 subscales of the EDI-2
demonstrated sufficient internal consistency with an alpha coefficient
ranging from .72 for the Maturity Fears and Bulimia subscales to .92 for
the Body Dissatisfaction subscale.
EDI-2, SPAS, and BMI Scores
Means and standard deviations of the SPAS, EDI-2 subscales, and BMI
for the divers, the athletic control group, and the nonathietic control
group are presented in Table 1. For comparison purposes, normative
scores for a female clinical population are also presented (Garner,
1991). Single sample t-tests revealed that the mean subscale scores for
the female clinical population from Garner (1991) were higher than all
the mean subscale scores for the divers, the athletic control group, and
the nonathletic control group at p [less than].05. The only exception
was that no significant differences were found between the clinical
population and the nonathietic and athletic control group on the desire
to retreat to the security of childhood as measured by the Maturity
Fears subscales. The significantly lower EDI-2 subscale scores of the
divers and athletic control group compared to the female clinical
population illustrated that the female athletes were not at-risk for
eating disorder correlates.
A one-way multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) with BMI as
the covariate was undertaken to determine if the EDI-2 subscale scores
differed across the divers, the athletic control group, and the
nonathletic control group. The MANCOVA, conducted with the eight EDI-2
subscales as the dependent variables and the three groups as the
independent variables was nonsignificant, Wilks's Lambda = .85,
F(16,45) .95, ns. Thus, no separate univariate analyses were conducted.
Therefore, the divers, the athletic control group, and the nonathletic
control group did not differ significantly on the EDI-2 subscales.
A one-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) indicated that SPAS
scores differed significantly across the three groups, F(2,106) = 3.88,
p [less than].05. In contrast to the hypothesis, post hoc analyses
revealed that the divers (M = 30.69, SD = 8.49) had significantly lower
SPAS scores than the athletic control group (M = 37.53, SD = 8.78), and
the nonathletic control group (M = 38.84, SD =9.88).
Relationship Between SPAS and EDI-2 Subscales
Two separate analyses were conducted to examine the predictive
relationship between SPAS and the EDI-2 subscales. First, to test if
social physique anxiety could be predicted by the behavioral and
attitudinal correlates of eating disorders (i.e., Bulimia, Drive for
Thinness, and Body Dissatisfaction), stepwise multiple-regression was
performed. The results of this analysis indicated that the attitudinal
and behavioral subscales of the EDI-2 combined to account for 66% of the
total variance in SPAS scores, F(3,109) = 70.81,p [less than].01.
However, only the Body Dissatisfaction and the Drive for Thinness
subscales contributed significantly to the model.
Second, to test if social physique anxiety could be predicted by
the psychological correlates of eating disorders (i.e., Maturity Fears,
Ineffectiveness, Interpersonal Distrust, Interoceptive Awareness, and
Perfectionism), stepwise multiple-regression was performed. The results
of the analysis revealed the psychological subscales accounted for 35%
of the total variance in SPAS scores, F(5, 103) = 11.15, p[less
than].01. However, only the Ineffectiveness subscale, which assesses
individual's general feelings of alienation, contributed
significantly to the model.
Discussion
The present study produced several noteworthy findings regarding
eating disorder correlates and social physique anxiety in female divers,
an athletic control group, and a nonathletic control group. First,
contrary to expectations, female divers reported significantly less
social physique anxiety than the athletic control group and the
nonathletic control group. Given divers low social physique anxiety
scores, it appears that they are comfortable with their physical
self-presentation, and have minimal self-presentational concerns
regarding their bodies. Thus, even though competitive divers compete and
train in revealing attire and are evaluated subjectively by judges, they
did not report heighten social physique anxiety.
One possible explanation for this finding, is that divers through
repeated exposure of their bodies in training and competition become
desensitized to self-presentational concerns (Eklund & Crawford,
1994). That is, after involvement in competitive diving,
self-presentation regarding the physique may become routine. A post hoc
analysis was conducted to examine this possibility. However, no
significant correlation was found between years competing in diving and
SPAS scores, r = .06, ns. Thus, repeated exposure does not appear to
account for divers' lower SPAS scores.
Another possible explanation for this finding may be that those
individuals with high social physique anxiety may have selectively
retired from competitive diving because they lack the 'ideal'
physique. That is, to remain competitive in a specific sport, a variety
of physical and psychological factors are required. In regard to the
physique, researchers have found that individuals with a particular body
type tend to gravitate towards and be most successful in certain sports
(Hollings & Robinson, 1991; Ross, Brown, Yu, & Faulker, 1977).
Thus, individuals who perceive that their physique represents a
disadvantage to successful performance may retire from the sport before
aspiring to elite levels of competition. As well, athletes in certain
sports may be subject to greater external pressures (e.g., coach) to
have a particular physique. Perhaps divers without the stereo-typical
thin build may have been persuaded out of the sport before the elite
level.
Another possible explanation for the observed differences on the
SPAS between the divers and the athletic control group may be a function
of the differences in competitive levels. The divers were competing at
the national/provincial level, whereas the athletic control group was
competing at the provincial/regional level. Social physique anxiety has
been found to be negatively correlated with confidence in one's
physical self-presentation in gymnasts (McAuley & Burman, 1993) and
in an undergraduate population (Martin & Mack, 1996). Thus, it is
possible that the lower levels of social physique anxiety found in the
divers may, in part, be due to the higher confidence these elite
athletes have in their physical self-presentation.
Second, no differences were found between the divers, the athletic
control group, and the nonathletic control group in regards to the
correlates associated with eating disorders. The suggestion that divers
may represent a group of athletes that are at-risk of developing eating
pathologies was not supported (Thompson & Sherman, 1993). Also, the
suggestion that female athletes in general are under increased risk of
developing eating disorders was not supported (e.g., Burkes-Miller &
Black, 1988). Our findings support Ashley et al. (1996) conclusion that
neither athletics in general nor a particular type of athletics
predispose individuals to exhibit the correlates associated with eating
disorders.
Finally, the present study supports the suggestion that
self-presentational concerns regarding the physique would be predicted
by the correlates associated with eating disorders. Results revealed
that dissatisfaction with the shape of the body and an extreme concern
with dieting and thinness were strong predictors of social physique
anxiety. Body dissatisfaction is generally viewed as a major factor
responsible for initiating and then sustaining the weight controlling
behaviors of those with eating disorders (Garner, 1991). And the intense
drive for thinness or fear of fatness, as measured by the Drive for
Thinness subscale, has been described as the core psychopathology of
both anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa (Garner, 1991).
As well, the Ineffectiveness subscale which assesses feelings of
general inadequacy, insecurity, worthlessness, emptiness, and lack of
control over one's life was a strong predictor of social physique
anxiety (Garner, 1991). Ineffectiveness is conceptually very similar to
poor self-esteem or negative self-evaluation, which are associated with
self-presentational concerns. As Leary et al. (1994) suggested,
self-presentational motives may be hazardous to one's health in
that impression management may play a role in disordered eating. Further
research is necessary to examine the causal relationship of this
finding. If self-presentational concerns regarding the physique place
some individuals at-risk for developing eating disorders, assessing
social physique anxiety may enable early detection of individuals
at-risk for eating disorders. However, to further examine this
possibility social physique anxiety must be assessed with eating
disordered populations.
In conclusion, the results of the present study suggest that
despite the aesthetic performance demands and self-presentation of the
physique, female divers experience minimal levels of social physique
anxiety and were not at-risk for eating disorder correlates. Thus,
involvement in activities that emphasize physical self-presentation does
not necessarily place females at-risk for developing eating disorder
correlates. It is important to note that the data collected were from a
volunteer sample that relied exclusively on self-reports for assessment.
Future research is needed to verify the results of this study using more
diverse and reliable methods (e.g., behavioral observations).
Note
(1.) There has been on ongoing debate regarding the psychometric
and conceptual properties of the SPAS. Originally the SPAS was proposed
as a unidimensional construct (Hart et al., 1989), and further analysis
have confirmed this (McAuley & Burman, 1993). Subsequently, however,
Eklund, Mack, and Hart (1996) have provided empirical support for a
multidimensional SPAS. Most recently, Martin, Rejeski, Leary, McAuley,
and Bain (1997) have argued both empirically and conceptually that the
SPAS should be viewed as a unidimensional scale. Consequently, three
sets of analysis were undertaken with (a) the unidimensional SPAS
(12-item; Hart et al., 1989), (b) the multidimensional SPAS (Eklund et
al., 1996). and (c) the unidimensional SPAS (9-items; Martin et al.,
1997). In the three sets of analysis, the results were similar. The
results reported in the current study are from the unidimensional
12-item SPAS (Hart et al., 1989). The results from the multidimensional
SPAS and the 9-item SPAS are available upon request fr om the first
author.
Address Correspondence To: Heather A. Hausenblas, Department of
Exercise and Sport Sciences, University of Florida, PO Box 118205, Room
146 Florida Gym, Gainesville, FL 32611-8205. Phone: 352-392-0584; Fax:
352-392-5262; E-mail: heatherh@hhp.ufl.edu
References
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and
statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC:
Author.
Ashley, C. D., Smith, J. F., Robinson, J. B., & Richardson, M.
T. (1996). Disordered eating in female collegiate athletes and
collegiate females in an advanced program of study: A preliminary
investigation, International Journal of Sport Nutrition, 6, 391-401.
Beaumont, P., Al-Alami, M., & Touyz, S. (1988). Relevance of a
standard measurement of undernutrition to the diagnosis of anorexia
nervosa: Use of Quetelet's body mass index (BMI). international
Journal of Eating Disorders, 7, 399-405.
Brownell, K. D. (1991). Dieting and the search for the perfect
body: Where physiology and culture collide. Behavior Therapy, 22, 1-12.
Brownell, K. D., & Steen, S. (1992). Weight cycling in
athletes: Effects on behavior, physiology, and Health. In K. D.
Brownell, J. Rodin, & J. H. Wilmore (Eds.), Eating, body weight and
performance in athletes. Disorders of modern society, (pp. 172-190).
Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
Bulik, C. M., Beidel, D. C., Duchmann, E., Weltzin, T. E., &
Kaye, W. E. (1991). An analysis of social anxiety in anorexic, bulimic,
social phobic, and control women. Journal of Psychopathology and
Behavioral Assessment, 13, 199-211.
Burkes-Miller, M., & Black, D. (1988). Male and female college
athletes: Prevalence of anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Athletic Training,
23, 137-140.
Carron, A. V., & Prapavessis, H. (1997). Self-presentation and
group influence. Small Group Research, 28, 500-516.
Davis, C. (1990). Body image and weight preoccupation: A comparison
between exercising and non-exercising women. Appetite, 15, 13-21.
Diehl, N., Johnson, C. E., Petrie, T. A., & Rogers, R. (1995,
September). Physique anxiety and disordered eating: What's the
connection? Paper presented at the meeting of the Association of the
Advancement of Applied Sport Psychology, New Orleans, LA.
Eklund, R. C., & Crawford, S. (1994). Active women, social
physique anxiety, and exercise. Journal of Sport & Exercise
Psychology, 16,431-448.
Eklund, R. C., Mack. D., & Hart, E. (1996). Factorial validity
of the Social Physique Anxiety Scale for females. Journal of Sport &
Exercise Psychology, 18,281-295.
Garner, D. M. (1991). Eating Disorder Inventoiy-2. Professional
Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Garner, D. M., Garfinkel, P. E., Schwartz, D., & Thompson, M.
(1980). Cultural expectations of thinness in women. Psychological
Reports, 47, 483-491.
Gross, J., & Rosen, J. C. (1988). Bulimia in adolescents:
Prevalence and psychological correlates. International Journal of Eating
Disorders, 7, 51-61.
Hart, E. A., Leary, M. R., & Rejeski, W. J. (1989). The
measurement of social physique anxiety. Journal of Sport & Exercise
Psychology 11,94-104.
Hays, D., & Ross, C. E. (1987). Concern with appearance, health
beliefs, and eating habits. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 28,
120-130.
Hollings, S.C., & Robinson, G. J. (1991). Body build and
performance characteristics of male adolescent track and field athletes.
The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 31, 178-182.
Johnson, M. D. (1994). Disordered eating in active and athletic
women. Clinics in Sports Medicine, 13, 355-369.
Katzman, M. A., & Wolchik, S. A. (1984). Bulimia and binge
eating in college women: A comparison of personality and behavioral
characteristics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
52,423-428.
Leary, M. R. (1992). Self-presentational processes in exercise and
sport. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 14, 399-351.
Leary, M. R., Tchividjian, L. R., & Kraxberger, B. E. (1994).
Self-presentation can be hazardous to your health: Impression management
and health risk. Health Psychology 13, 461-470.
Martin, K. A., & Mack, D. (1996). Relationships between
physical self-presentation and sport competition in trait anxiety: A
preliminary study. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology 18, 75-82.
Martin, K. A., Rejeski, W. J., Leary, M. R., McAuley, E., &
Bain, S. (1997). Is the Social Physique Anxiety Scale really
multidimensional? Conceptual and statistical arguments for a
unidimensional model. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology 19,
357-367.
McAuley, E., & Burman, G. (1993). The social physique anxiety
scale: Construct validity in adolescent females. Medicine and Science in
Sports and Exercise, 25, 1049-1053.
Petrie, T. A. (1996). Differences between male and female college
lean sport athletes, non-lean sport athletes, and nonathletes on
behavioral and psychological indices on eating disorders. Journal of
Applied Sport Psychology, 8, 218-230.
Pliner, P., & Chaiken, S. (1990). Eating, social motives, and
self-presentation in women and men. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 26, 240-254.
Reel, J. J., & Gill, D. L. (1996). Psychosocial factors related
to eating disorders among high school and college female cheerleaders.
The Sport Psychologist 10, 195-206.
Ross, W. D., Brown, S. R., Yu, J. W., & Faulkner, R. A. (1977).
Somatotype of Canadian figure skaters. Journal of Sports Medicine, 17,
195-205.
Stein, D. M. (1991). The prevalence of bulimia: A review of the
empirical research. Journal of Nutrition Education, 23, 205-213.
Sundgot-Borgen, J. (1993). Prevalence of eating disorders in elite
female athletes. International Journal of Sport Nutrition, 3, 29-40.
Sundgot-Borgen, J. (1994). Risk and triggers for the development of
eating disorders in female elite athletes. Medicine and Science in
Sports and Exercise, 2,414-419.
Thompson, R. A., & Sherman, R. T. (1993). Helping athletes with
eating disorders. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Wear, R. W., & Pratz, 0. (1987). Test-retest reliability for
the Eating Disorder Inventory. International Journal of Eating
Disorders, 6, 767-769.
Wilkins, J. A., Boland, F. J., & Albinson, J. (1991). A
comparison of male and female university athletes and nonathletes on
eating disorder indices: Are athletes protected? Journal of Sport
Behavior, 14, 129-143.
Wilson, G. T., & Eldredge, K. L. (1992). Pathology and
development of eating disorders: Implications for athletes. In K. D.
Brownell, J. Rodin, & J. H. Wilmore (Eds.), Eating, body weight and
performance in athletes. Disorders of modern society, (pp. 115-127).
Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
Wiseman, C. V., Gray, J. J., Mosimann, J. E., & Ahrens, A. H.
(1992). Cultural expectations of thinness in women: An update.
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 11, 85-89.
Means and Standard Deviation Scores for the Body Mass Index (BMI),
Social Physique Anxiety Scale (SPAS) and Eating Disorder Inventory-2
Subscales (EDI-2) for the Divers, Athletic Control Group, Nonathletic
Control Group, and Female Clinical Population
Variable Divers Athletic Nonathletic Female
Control Control Clinical Population
n = 36 n = 39 n = 39 N = 889
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
BMI [**] 19.8(2.3) 21.8(2.2) 22.4(4.7) --
SPAS 30.8(8.5) 37.7(8.7) 39.3(9.8) --
EDI-2
Bulimia 1.2(1.7) 1.7(8.7) 1.2(1.9) 10.5(5.5) [*]
Drive for Thinness 3.3(4.0) 6.0(5.2) 5.5(5.6) 14.5(5.6)
Body Dissatisfaction 7.4(6.3) 11.6(8.1) 12.6(8.6) 16.6(8.3)
Ineffectiveness 2.5(3.1) 3.6(8.1) 4.7(5.5) 11.3(7.8)
Interoceptive Awareness 2.8(3.5) 4.9(6.2) 4.0(4.0) 11.0(6.9)
Interpersonal Distrust 3.7(2.1) 2.9(3.0) 4.0(4.3) 5.8(4.7)
Maturity Fears 3.4(2.1) 4.4(4.5) 4.1(3.1) 4.5(6.9)
Perfectionism 5.1(3.9) 6.5(4.5) 6.5(4.5) 8.9(4.9)
Note. Mean scale scores for the female clinical population are from
Garner (1991).
(*.)N = 760 (does not include anorexia nervosa-restricting type).
(**.)The divers had significantly lower BMI scores that the
athletic and nonathletic control groups, F(2, 107) = 6.00, p[less
than].01.