Psychological effects of strength training on children.
Faigenbaum, Avery ; Zaichowsky, Leonard D. ; Westcott, Wayne L. 等
Strength training is often prescribed as part of a well-rounded
exercise program for adults and its popularity among children continues
to increase. Previously deemed unsafe and ineffective for children
(American Academy of Pediatrics, 1983), strength training is now
considered an important component of youth fitness conditioning programs
(American Orthopaedic Society for Sports Medicine, 1988; Faigenbaum,
Kraemer et al., 1996) and injury prevention strategies (American College
of Sports Medicine, 1993). Improvements in various anatomical and
physiological measures have been noted in children who have participated
in appropriately designed and well-supervised strength training programs
(Faigenbaum, 1993). Related information on the psychological benefits of
youth strength training is limited, however.
Data from adult populations demonstrate that the effects of strength
training extend beyond physical measures and include improvements in
mental health and well-being (Dishman & Gettman, 1981; Doyne, et
al., 1987; Melnick & Mookerjee, 1991; Stein & Motta, 1992;
Tucker, 1982, 1983a). Tucker (1982) reported significantly higher values
for self-concept and self-esteem (1983) in college-age men who
participated in a strength training program and similar findings
involving college-age men and women were reported by Melnick and
Mookerjee (1991). Strength training has improved psychic vigor and
physical self-esteem in men (Dishman & Geltman, 1981), and strength
training appears to be just as effective as aerobic exercise in reducing
clinical depression in women (Doyne et al., 1987; Stein & Motta,
1992). In the clinical setting, adult patients with higher ratings of
arm strength self-efficacy achieved greater gains in arm strength than
did patients with lower arm strength-self efficacy ratings (Ewart,
Taylor, Reese, & DeBusk, 1983). Self-efficacy expectations appear to
be meaningful determinants of exercise behavior in adults (Bandura,
1977, 1986), and may provide important information regarding the
acquisition of new behaviors in younger populations. As such, self
efficacy was selected as an outcome measure for this study.
Children who participate in physical fitness programs are reported to
have higher self-concepts that those who are inactive (Zaichkowsky, L.,
Zaichkowsky, L., & Martinek, T., 1975; Martinek, T., Cheffers, J.,
& Zaichkowsky, L., 1978), although not all studies have reported
positive relationships between fitness and childhood self-esteem (Kay,
Felker, & Varoz, 1972). Despite these inconsistencies, a recent
review on this topic suggests that children who participate in
appropriately prescribed aerobic exercise programs have the potential to
increase their self-efficacy, creativity, self-esteem, internal locus of
control, test scores on cognitive functioning and classroom behavior
(Welsh & Labbe, 1994).
Holloway, Beuter, and Duda (1988) reported that adolescent females
who strength trained significantly improved their situationally specific
self-efficacy expectations about strength training and other
less-related tasks such as confrontation. In younger populations,
anecdotal reports from prepubescent boys and girls and their parents
suggest that strength training may positively influence socialization skills, mental discipline and self-concept (Faigenbaum, 1995). At this
time, however, controlled studies exploring the potential psychological
benefits of prepubescent strength training have not yet been performed.
Since the psychological benefits of aerobic exercise on children have
been documented in the literature (Welsh & Labbe, 1994), it is
reasonable to assume that anaerobic types of exercise such as strength
training may also provide psychological beneficence. The popularity of
strength training activities continues to increase and youth strength
training programs have become important components of public health
objectives (Department of Health and Human Services, 1990). Although
information from adult studies is useful, speculations regarding the
psychosocial benefits of strength training for children are limited
because of the psychological and physical immaturity of children.
Moreover, the type of strength training program most likely to enhance
psychological well-being remains hypothetical because the acute program
variables (e.g., choice of exercises, order of exercises, repetitions,
sets and rest periods) are not defined in most of the psychological
reports involving adults.
The specific purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of
strength training on the global self-concepts and situationally specific
self-efficacy levels of prepubescent boys and girls. The strength
training program in this study was designed to safely maximize strength
gains in children while offering a progression of mastery experiences
that might influence psychological well-being. Both physical and
psychological parameters were clearly described in order to assess the
interaction between these variables. It was hypothesized that increases
in strength would positively influence scores on self-concept and
related self-efficacy measures.
Method
Subjects
Twenty-four boys and girls living in a suburb of Boston,
Massachusetts volunteered to take part in this study. The subjects were
white and from predominantly middle income level families. Parental
written consent was obtained. A physician conducted a medical
examination on all subjects in order to evaluate musculoskeletal status,
document preexisting orthopedic injuries and assess maturity status
based on Tanner staging of pubic hair development (Tanner, 1962). Males
and females were combined in this study because they perform equally
well on strength tests during preadolescence (Blimkie, 1989). Moreover,
sex differences in self-concept for elementary-age boys and girls is
small (Zaichkowsky, Zaichkowsky, & Martinek, 1980).
The experimental group consisted of 11 boys and 4 girls (mean age
10.8 [+ or -] 0.4 years) who enrolled in this study prior to the
recruitment of the control group which included 3 boys and 6 girls (mean
age 10.0 [+ or -] 0.4 yrs). Administrative variables (e.g., the
availability of space, time, and equipment) precluded the enrollment of
additional subjects. Tanner staging for maturity level revealed that the
experimental group consisted of 13 subjects at Tanner stage 1 and 2
subjects at Tanner stage 2, while the control group had 8 subjects at
Tanner stage 1 and I subject at Tanner stage 9. There were no
significant differences between the two groups for age or Tanner scale
rating (p [greater than] 0.05). Subjects did not have any prior
experience with strength training. All volunteers were accepted for
participation.
Design and Procedure
All testing and training took place during the summer at a local
youth fitness center. At least two instructors, including one certified
Health/Fitness Director (American College of Sports Medicine, 1995),
supervised all sessions. Experimental and control group subjects were
taught the proper form and technique on each exercise during two
introductory sessions within a one week period. Physical strength was
assessed following the one week introductory period and at the
conclusion of the study. Details of the testing and training protocols
have been described in a previous report (Faigenbaum, Westcott et al.,
1996). Psychological measures were assessed before and after the
strength training program.
Strength Measures
Each subject's 6 repetition maximum (RM) was determined on the
leg extension and chest press. The maximum weight that could be lifted
six times using the correct form was recorded. Following a 72-hour rest
period, both 6 RM measures were reevaluated. The heaviest 6 RM load
lifted on each exercise, on either testing day, was recorded as the
child's criterion 6 RM score.
Psychological Measures
Self Concept. The Martinek-Zaichkowsky Self Concept Scale for
Children (MZSCS) (Martinek & Zaichkowsky, 1977) was used to evaluate
global self-concept development in the children. This nonverbal test
consists of 25 pairs of cartoon pictures (1-point each) and measures
intellectual, social, and physical aspects of a child's
self-concept as well as appropriate behavior. The subjects were asked to
mark the pictures which were most like them. The test has acceptable
validity, internal consistency, and stability (Martinek &
Zaichkowsky, 1977).
Self-Efficacy. A modified version of the scale developed by Ewart
(1990) was created for this study to assess each child's
self-perceived efficacy (confidence) in performing specific tasks
involving strength. The questions were presented in a Likert scale in
which the children rated their perceived weight lifting ability on an
11-point probability scale ranging from "definitely cannot do
it" (O-points) to "definitely can do it" (10-points). The
children were presented with a series of progressively more difficult
physical tasks (i.e., the ability to lift 5, 10, 15...50 pounds with
their upper body and 5, 10, 15...60 pounds with their lower body) and
were asked to rate the strength of their perceived ability to perform
the task at that specific moment. A subject's self-efficacy
construct score was the sum of the confidence estimates for successive
levels of a specific task divided by the total number of items. Previous
studies of self-efficacy have employed a similar testing methodology
(Bandura & Adams, 1977; Ewart et al., 1983; Holloway et al., 1988).
Training Program
Subjects in the experimental group trained twice per week (Monday and
Wednesday) for eight weeks under qualified adult supervision. Throughout
the training program, an instructor to subject ratio of at least 1 to 4
was maintained at all times. Daily instructional sessions focused on
proper exercise technique, and any questions from the subjects were
answered. During the first four weeks of training, subjects performed
one warm-up set of 10 repetitions followed by two sets of six to eight
repetitions on the leg extension, chest press, leg curl, overhead press
and biceps curl. During the last four weeks of training, subjects
performed three sets of six to eight repetitions on the chest press and
leg extension whereas the number of sets on the other exercises remained
unchanged. Child-size dynamic constant resistance equipment (Heartline
Fitness Equipment, Gaithersburg, MD) was used for all strength testing and training procedures. The predetermined 6 repetition maximum (RM)
load was used for the leg extension and chest press whereas initial 6 RM
training loads for the other exercises were estimated during the first
week of training. Once 8 repetitions could be performed on the last set,
the resistance was increased by 5 to 10% and the repetitions were
decreased to 6. Each session included flexibility exercises and lasted
about one hour. Subjects exercised with a partner and recorded their
data on workout logs which were reviewed daily by the instructors who
made appropriate adjustments in training loads and repetitions if
necessary. If an exercise session was missed, a makeup session was
offered during the same week. Experimental and control group subjects
were asked not to partake in any strength training activities outside of
the supervised research setting. However, all subjects were permitted to
continue participation In organized sport activities (primarily
baseball, soccer or swimming) throughout the study.
Results
All subjects completed the study according to the aforementioned
methodology. The strength trained subjects had an average attendance
rate of 93%.
Analysis of covariance procedures were performed on the post-test
scores for each of the dependent variables using the pretest scores for
each variable as the covariate. This procedure adjusted for initial
variability in pretest scores between subjects. Adjusted post-means are
presented in Table 1. Group data are reported as mean [+ or -] standard
error in the descriptive text.
Table 1
Adjusted Post-test Means of Psychological and Physical Measures
Experimental Group Control Group
Self Concept 21.5 20.7
Self-Efficacy (Upper Body) 9.3 9.2
Self-Efficacy (Lower Body) 9.0 8.8
6 RM Chest Press 29.2 22.0
6 RM Leg Extension 27.9 18.7
Significant F-ratios for experimental versus control groups were
noted on chest press strength, F(1, 21) = 18.68, p [less than] .0003 and
leg extension strength, F(1, 21) = 14.53, p [less than] .001. Following
the training program, the chest press strength of the experimental group
increased from 21.4 kg (1.1) to 30.2 k; (1.6) whereas the control group
values increased from 18.7 kg (0.6) to 20.4 kg (0.6). Leg extension
strength of the experimental group increased from 18.6 kg (1.1) to 28.6
kg (2.1) and control group values increased from 16.4 kg (1.5) to 17.5
kg (1.1). Control group gains are attributable to growth and maturation.
Changes in self-concept, F (1, 21) = 3.25, p = .0859, upper body
self-efficacy, F (1, 21) = .03, p = .8673, and lower body self-efficacy,
F (1, 21) = .16, p = .6942 were not significant, indicating that there
were no differences in the psychological measures between experimental
and control groups following the training program. Pre- to post-test
changes in self-concept for the experimental and control groups were
20.5 (0.8) to 21.3 (0.7) and 21.1 (0.8) to 21.0 (0.9), respectively.
Pre- to post-test changes in upper body self-efficacy scores for the
experimental and control groups were 7.6 (0.4) to 9.3 (0.3) and 7.2
(0.5) to 9.3 (0.3), respectively, whereas changes in lower body
self-efficacy scores were 7.7 (0.4) to 9.1 (0.3) and 7.3 (0.5) to 8.7
(0.4), respectively.
Discussion
The results of this study indicate that participation in a
progressive strength training program will significantly increase the
physical strength of boys and girls between the ages of 7 and 12.
Observed upper body and lower body strength gains of 53.4% and 41.1%,
respectively, are consistent with other short-term training studies
involving children (Faigenbaum, Zaichkowsky, Westcott, Micheli, &
Fehlandt, 1993; Sewall & Micheli, 1986). No injuries occurred during
the strength training program.
Despite significant increases in strength, training did not have a
significant effect on psychological measures. These results suggest that
a short-term, twice per week strength training program will not
significantly effect the perception children have of themselves or their
confidence in performance weightlifting activities. However, qualitative
statements from children who completed the strength training program
suggest the contrary. Children stated that they were more confident in
their lifting abilities and were more willing to try different sports
and activities following the training program. These children
continually commented on their noticeable gains In strength, and at the
end of the study their parents remarked on their children's
enhanced feelings of well-being.
It is conceivable that the relatively high pretest self-concept
scores (ceiling effect) in both the experimental and control groups
precluded significant gains. Data from Zaichkowsky et al. (1980)
indicate that the mean MZSCS score for fourth grade boys and girls is
approximately 17 whereas pretest MZSCS scores for the experimental and
control groups in this study were 20.5 and 21.1, respectively. Although
speculative, the results of this study suggest that strength training
will not enhance the self-concept of children who begin strength
training with above average feelings of self-worth.
This argument is consistent with data from Tucker (1983b) who noted
an inverse association between pretest measures of self-concept and
global self-concept change in adult males who participated in a strength
training program. Moreover, a meta-analysis of the relationship between
physical fitness and self-concept in children supports the contention
that the psychological effect of exercise will be most apparent in
children with emotional disturbances (Gruber, 1986).
In the present study, self-efficacy scores increased for both the
exercise and control groups indicating that both groups became more
confident in their weightlifting abilities. Since self-efficacy involves
specific appraisals of personal abilities, a child's perceived
ability to lift more weight following eight weeks of strength training
was expected. Children in the exercise group gradually increased the
resistance as they got stronger, which in turn seemed to enhance their
confidence and sense of achievement. Moreover, these children received
positive reinforcement from respected fitness instructors and had the
opportunity to observe boys and girls their own age weight train.
Yet the control group's increase in self-efficacy was comparable
to that of the exercise group. It is possible that positive feedback
regarding post-test performances provided the control group with a false
sense of accomplishment, thereby engendering self-efficacy improvements
not directly related to their self-efficacy gains. Furthermore, unlike
adult populations, children are growing and will therefore continually
get stronger without specifically training for strength. The control
group's increase in growth-related strength may also explain, in
part, their enhanced self-efficacy ratings.
Although pilot data regarding youth strength measures were used in
the development of the self-efficacy questionnaire, the statements on
this form underestimated the experimental group's gain in strength.
Thus the experimental group subjects did not have the opportunity to
express their confidence in lifting weights over 60 pounds even though
the strength tests revealed they had the physical capabilities to do so.
For example, 7 children in the exercise group lifted over 60 pounds on
the leg extension 6 RM follow-up test (maximum post-test leg extension 6
RM was 91 pounds), whereas the self-efficacy questions regarding lower
body strength peaked at 60 pounds. During the administration of the
self-efficacy questionnaire, several subjects asked about the purpose of
the form and were unsure about assessing their physical capabilities.
Although speculative, the cognitive abilities of prepubescent boys and
girls, as opposed to older populations, may limit their understanding
and appreciation of self-efficacy as it relates to motor skills.
In conclusion, empirical evidence suggests that the potential
benefits of youth strength training may include improvements in
psychological well-being, though scientific data from this short-term
investigation do not support this claim. The self-efficacy questionnaire
employed in this study underestimated the observed gains in strength,
and children in this study began strength training with relatively high
levels of self-concept which may have precluded significant findings.
Since this was not a randomized study, the experimental and control
groups may have been influenced by some unmeasured characteristics, thus
the generalizability of these findings are limited. Longer term
randomized trials with larger sample sizes are recommended for future
studies.
Author Notes
The authors gratefully acknowledge the staff at the South Shore YMCA,
Quincy, Massachusetts, for their technical assistance and support
throughout this study.
For further Information, please contact:
Avery D. Faigenbaum Department of Human Performance and Fitness
University of Massachusetts 100 Morrissey Boulevard Boston, MA
02125-3393
References
American Academy of Pediatrics. (1983). Weight training and weight
lifting: Information for the Pediatrician. Physician and Sportsmedicine,
11(3), 157-161.
American College of Sports Medicine. (1993). The prevention of sports
injuries of children and adolescents. Medicine and Science in Sports and
Exercise, 25 (Suppl. 8), 1-7.
American College of Sports Medicine. (1995). ACSM's Guidelines
for Exercise Testing and Prescription (5th ed.). Baltimore: Williams
& Wilkins.
American Orthopaedic Society for Sports Medicine. (1988). Proceedings
of the Conference on Strength Training and the Prepubescent. Chicago:
American Orthopaedic Society for Sports Medicine.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191 -215.
Bandura, A, & Adams, N. (1977). Analysis of self-efficacy theory
of behavioral change. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1, 287-308.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A
Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Blimkie, C. (1989). Age- and sex-associated variation in strength
during childhood: anthropometric, morphologic, neurologic,
biomechanical, endocrinologic, genetic, and physical activity
correlates. In C. Gisolfi and D. Lamb (Eds.), Perspectives in Exercise
Science and Sports Medicine, Vol. 2: Youth, Exercise, and Sport. (pp.
99-163). Indianapolis, IN: Benchmark.
Department of Health and Human Services. (1990). Healthy People 2000:
National health promotion and disease prevention objectives. (DHHS Publication No. PHS 91-50212).
Dishman, R., & Gettman, L. (1981). Psychological vigor and
self-perceptions of increased strength. Medicine and Science in Sports
and Exercise, 13, 73-74. (Abstract).
Doyne, E., Ossip-Klein, E., Bowman, K., Osborn, I., McDougall-Wilson,
& Neimeyer, R. (1987). Running versus weight lifting in the
treatment of depression. Journal of Consulting Clinical Psychology, 55,
748-754.
Ewart, C., Taylor, C., Reese, L., & DeBusk, R. (1983). Effects of
early post myocardial infarction exercise testing on self-perception and
subsequent physical activity. American Journal of Cardiology, 51,
1076-1080.
Ewart, C., Stewart, K., Gilligan, R., & Keleman, M. (1986)
Self-efficacy mediates strength gains during circuit weight training in
men with coronary artery disease. Medicine and Science in Sports and
Exercise, 18, 531-540.
Ewart, G. (1989). Psychological effects of resistive weight training:
Implications for cardiac patients. Medicine and Science in Sports and
Exercise, 91 (6), 683-688.
Ewart, C. K. (1990). A social problem-solving approach to behavior
change in coronary heart disease. In S. Schumaker, J. Okene, & E.
Schron (Eds.), The Handbook of Health Behavior Change (pp. 153-190). New
York: Springer.
Faigenbaum, A. (1993). Prepubescent strength training: A guide for
teachers and coaches, National Strength and Conditioning Association
Journal, 15 (5), 20-29.
Faigenbaum, A. (1995). Psychosocial benefits of prepubescent strength
training. Strength and Conditioning, 17 (2), 28-32.
Faigenbaum, A., Kraemer, W., Cahill, B., Dziados, J., Elfink, L.,
Forman, E., Gaudiose, M., Micheli, L., Nitka, M., & Roberts, S.
(1996). Youth resistance training: Positions statement paper and
literature review. Strength and Conditioning, 18(6), pp. 62-75.
Faigenbaum, A., Westcott, W., Micheli, L., Outerbridge, A., Long, C.,
LaRosa Loud, R. & Zaichkowsky, L. (1996). The effects of strength
training and detraining on children. Journal of Strength and
Conditioning Research, 10(2), pp. 109-114.
Faigenbaum, A., Zaichkowsky, L., Westcott, W., Micheli, L., &
Fehlandt, A. (1993). The effects of a twice per week strength training
program on children. Pediatric Exercise Science, 5(4), 339-346.
Fitzsimmons, P., Landers, D., Thomas, J., & van der Mars, H.
(1991). Does self-efficacy predict performance in experienced
weightlifters? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 62 (4),
424-431.
Gruber, J. (1986). Physical activity and self-esteem development in
children: A meta-analysis. In G.A. Stull & H.M. Eckert (Eds.),
Effects of physical activity on children (pp. 3048). Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics.
Holloway, J, Beuter, A., & Duda, J. (1988). Self-efficacy and
training for strength in adolescent girls. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 18 (8), 699-719.
Kay, R., Felker, D., & Varoz, R. (1972). Sports interests and
abilities as contributors to self-concept in junior high school boys.
Research Quarterly, 43, 208-215.
Martinek, T., Cheffers, J., Zaichkowsky, L. (1978). Physical
activity, motor development, and self-concept: Race and age differences.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 6(1), 147-154.
Martinek, T., & Zaichkowsky, L. (1977). Manual and scale for the
Martinek Zaichkowsky self-concept scale for children. Jacksonville,
III.: Psychologists and Educators.
Melnick, M., & Mookerjee, S. (1991). Effects of advanced weight
training on body cathexis and self-esteem. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
72, 1335-1345.
Ross, C., & Hayes, D. (1988). Exercise and psychologic well-being
in the community. American Journal of Epidemiology, 127, 762-771.
Sewall, L., & Micheli, L. (1986). Strength training for children.
Journal of Pediatric Orthopedics, 6(2), 143- 146.
Stein, P., & Motta, R. (1992). Effects of aerobic and nonaerobic
exercise on depression and self-concept. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 4,
79-89.
Tanner, J. (1962). Growth at Adolescence. 2nd ed. Oxford: Blackwell
Scientific.
Tucker, L. (1982). Effect of a weight training program on the
self-concepts of college males. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54,
1055-1061.
Tucker, L. (1983a). Weight-training: A tool for the improvement of
self and body concepts of males. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 9,
31-37.
Tucker, L. (1983b). Effect of weight training on self-concept: A
profile of those influenced most. Research Quarterly in Exercise and
Sport, 54, 389-397.
Welsh, C. & Labbe, E. (1994). Children and aerobic exercise: A
review or cognitive and behavioral effect. Journal Of Experimental Child
Psychology, 58(3), 405-417.
Zaichkowsky, L., Zaichkowsky, L., & Martinek, T. (1975).
Self-concept and attitudinal differences in elementary age school
children after participation in a physical activity program. Mouvement,
7, 243-245.
Zaichkowsky, L., Zaichkowsky, L., & Martinek, T. (1980). Growth
and development. St. Louis: C.V. Mosby.