Investigating leadership, gender, and coaching level using the Revised Leadership for Sport Scale.
Jambor, Elizabeth A. ; Zhang, James J.
Research often investigates the leadership styles of coaches
(Chelladurai, 1978; Chelladurai & Carron, 1978; Fry, Kerr, &
Lee, 1986; Offermann & Bell, 1992). These investigations look at
personalities, behaviors, and other related variables in an attempt to
better understand and define leadership. Chelladurai and Carron (1978)
developed a multidimensional model to better define leadership. Within
this model, these researchers provided three states of leadership
behaviors (required, preferred, and actual) that were associated with
group performance and member satisfaction. By using this leadership
model, one can investigate the multidimensional aspects of leadership by
including the environment (the setting, the competitive level), the
group members and their characteristics, and the personal
characteristics of the leader (gender, age, psychological
characteristics). It is this multifaceted focus of leadership that
allows research to better understand leadership issues.
Researchers provide contradicting evidence for the differences
between male and female leaders. Freeman and Lanning (1989) demonstrated
how males and females are similar in social power motivation (an element
of leadership). Conversely, Chelladurai and Saleh (1980) found that male
athletes preferred coaches to be more autocratic, yet more supportive
than did female athletes. Following Chelladurai and Saleh's (1980)
research on preferred leadership behavior, suggesting that behavior is
dictated by member's preferences (athlete's attitudes), it
would follow that coaches of males (typically male coaches) would be
different than coaches of females (typically female coaches).
Additionally, Lipman-Blumen (1992) reported that gender differences
exist in leadership achievement styles. At present, the literature
argues as to whether males and females differ or whether leadership
models need re-evaluation to include a broader range of variables and
behaviors (Lipman-Blumen, 1992).
Within the scope of required leader behavior and actual leader
behavior, differences might exist between coaches at different levels.
Chelladurai and Carron (1978) suggested that leader behavior occurs as
the result of organizational constraints (required behavior) and
personality, ability, and experience (actual behavior). It would follow
that coaches at the primary and secondary school levels would differ
from each other just as they would differ from coaches at the collegiate level. The organizational constraints and experience levels would
dictate differences in leadership behavior.
Chelladurai and Saleh (1980) investigated five leadership behaviors:
training and instruction, democratic, autocratic, social support, and
positive feedback. These behaviors were used to measure coaches'
perceptions of their own behavior. However, the situational leadership
theory states that leaders should vary behaviors related to members
(Hersey & Blanchard, 1977). Situational behavior is not present in
the Leadership for Sport Scale (LSS) (Chelladurai & Saleh, 1980).
Zhang, Jensen, and Mann (1996) have taken the investigation of
leadership a step further in their development of the Revised Leadership
Scale for Sport (RLSS), modeled after Chelladurai and Saleh's
(1980) LSS. The RLSS defines six different behaviors of leadership:
training and instruction; democratic; autocratic; social support;
positive feedback; and situation consideration. The RLSS adds situation
consideration to the variables used in the LSS to provide another
behavior in which leaders may operate. Leaders can be described based on
the situation in which they find themselves, offering more flexibility
to the definition. However, beyond the research by Zhang, Jensen, and
Mann (1996) in the development of the RLSS, little has been done to test
its use with various populations.
In an attempt to extend the knowledge of leadership in sport, the
purpose of this study was to determine possible differences in
leadership behaviors, using the RLSS, between male and female coaches
and among different coaching levels. The present study focused on the
three states leadership behaviors, suggested by Chelladurai and Carron
(1978), in male and female coaches among various levels of coaching. The
scales by Chelladurai and Saleh (1980) and Zhang, Jensen, and Mann
(1996) include methods for assessing athletes' perceptions and
preferences and coaches' perceptions. For this study, only the
coaches' perceptions, as measured by the RLSS, were used. First, it
was hypothesized that male and female coaches would respond differently
to the RLSS given the six dimensions in which a leader may exhibit
behaviors. This hypothesis was based on Chelladurai's (1980)
contention that certain factors (i.e., gender) set the boundaries for
leadership behaviors. This relates to Chelladurai's required leader
behavior within the multidimensional model of leadership. Second, it was
hypothesized that differences on the IRLSS would occur among the
coaching levels: junior high school, high school, and college. This
relates to the preferred leader behavior within Chelladurai's
multidimensional model of leadership, in that situational
characteristics influence preferred behavior.
Methods
Participants
The participants were 162 coaches (male = 118; female = 44) at the
junior high school (n = 25), high school (n = 99), and college level (n
= 38). All participants participated on a volunteer basis only.
Instrument
The Revised Leadership Scale for Sport (Zhang, Jensen, & Mann,
1996) was used to measure six leadership behaviors. The behaviors were:
1. Training and Instruction: planning training practices and
evaluating the performance of the athletes; having knowledge and being
responsible.
2. Democratic: encouraging involvement of the athletes; admitting
mistakes and confronting problems.
3. Autocratic: making independent decisions; using commands and
punishment.
4. Social Support: helping athletes with personal problems; making
sport part of enjoyment of an athlete's life.
5. Positive Feedback: encouraging an athlete after making a mistake;
correcting the behavior rather than blaming the athlete.
6. Situation Consideration: setting up individual goals and
clarifying ways to reach goals; differentiating coaching methods at
different maturity stages and skill levels.
The 60 statements were preceded by "In coaching, I:" and
followed by a Likert scale of 1-5: 1 = never; 2 = seldom; 3 =
occasionally; 4 = often; and 5 = always. Each of these relates to the
percentage scale (0-100%) used by on the Leadership Scale for Sport
(Chelladurai & Saleh, 1978).
Procedures
Scales were given in a variety of settings: classrooms, gymnasiums,
practice fields, and offices. Participants were asked to respond
honestly to each statement, describing their own coaching behavior.
Confidentiality was stressed so that the coaches might feel more at ease
in responding. Participants were informed, after completing the scale,
as to the nature of the study. Any participant who requested feedback
from the results of their responses was provided with that feedback.
Results
Reliability Measures for the RLSS
Reliability was tested for each behavior section of the RLSS to lend
support to the following analyses. The internal consistency for each
section was: .84 for training and instruction; .66 for democratic; .70
for autocratic; .52 for social support; .78 for feedback; and .69 for
situational consideration. These measures of reliability were believed
to be significant enough to use the RLSS in investigating possible
gender differences in leadership behavior.
RLSS and Gender
A MANOVA was used to determine the existence of significant
differences between male and female coaches when looking at the
leadership behaviors in total. Results indicated no significant
differences between male and female coaches ([Lambda] = .77; p [less
than or equal to] .23). Male and female coaches gave similar responses
for the combined six leadership behaviors.
When examining the six behaviors individually, males and females
differed on only one leadership behavior. social support (F (1,160) =
4.47; p [less than or equal to] .05). Females scored significantly
higher on this behavior than did the males, indicating a greater
tendency for helping athletes with personal problems and making sport
part of the enjoyment of an athlete's life.
RLSS and Coaching Level
A MANOVA was used to determine the existence of significant
differences between three different levels of coaching (junior high
school, high school, and college) when looking at the leadership
behaviors in total. Results indicated significant differences between
the three coaching levels ([Lambda] = .61; p [less than or equal to]
.02). The three different levels of coaching did not respond similarly
to the six leadership behaviors of the RLSS.
When examining the six behaviors individually, the three coaching
levels differed on three behaviors. Democratic leadership behaviors were
significantly different (F (2, 158) = 4.14; p [less than or equal to]
.009). As indicated by a Scheffe post hoc test, college coaches and high
school coaches were significantly different from one another with high
school coaches indicating a higher degree of democratic behaviors.
Training and instruction behaviors were significantly different (F
(2,158) = 3.46, p [less than or equal to] .02). As indicated by a
Scheffe post hoc test, junior high school coaches were significantly
lower in training and instruction behaviors than were the high school
and college coaches. Finally, social support behaviors were
significantly different (F (2,158) = 3.66, p [less than or equal to]
.02). As indicated by a Scheffe post hoc test, junior high school
coaches reported significantly less social support behaviors than did
high school and college coaches.
RLSS, Gender, and Coaching Level
A MANOVA was used to determine the possible interaction of gender and
coaching level when looking at the leadership behaviors in total.
Results indicated no significant interactions ([Lambda] = .66; p [less
than or equal to] .06). For the present sample, the combination of
gender and level of coaching was not reflected in self-reported
leadership behavior.
Discussion
The results of this study did not support the first hypothesis that
male and female coaches would respond differently to the behaviors of
the Revised Leadership for Sport Scale. Differences in preferred
coaching style is often related to an athlete's gender (Chelladurai
& Saleh, 1980; Zhang & Smith, 1994), yet does not distinguish
between the genders of coaches. The only behavior in which differences
did exist, when viewing the six leadership components separately, was
the social support behavior. Social support behavior was denoted as
helping athletes with personal problems and making sport part of
enjoyment of an athlete's life. In the present study, female
coaches reported providing more support in an athlete's personal
life and making sport more enjoyable. This finding relates to earlier
work by Salminem, Liukkonen, and Telama (1990). These researchers
reported female coaches to be more supportive and rewarding than male
coaches.
The present research supports previous studies that have concluded
that males and females are not inherently different in their leadership
styles, but rather that a new model of leadership is needed to define
leadership behaviors. Lipman-Blumen (1992) suggests that the traditional
masculine leadership model is not enough for the integrated world. This
idea of integration is especially true within the athletic world. To say
that a male is a different leader than a female is an out-dated method
of leadership evaluation. Additionally, as many investigators of
leadership have suggested, the members and the situation play important
roles in defining leadership (Chelladurai, 1990; Chelladurai &
Carron, 1978). This lack of gender differences additionally supports
Chelladurai's (1990) multidimensional model of leadership which
includes leader characteristics yet not gender. Therefore, one should
not expect gender differences, as supported by the present results.
The second hypothesis, differences on the RLSS would occur among the
three different coaching levels, was supported by the results. The
coaching levels (junior high, high school, and college) were
significantly different when viewing leadership in total. When
investigating leadership in its six components, the coaching levels
differed on three behaviors: democratic, training and instruction, and
social support. Democratic leadership behavior was defined as
encouraging involvement of the athletes, admitting mistakes, and
confronting problems. Training and instruction leadership behavior was
described as planning training practices and evaluating the performance
of the athletes, as well as having knowledge and being responsible.
Social support leadership behavior was defined as helping athletes with
personal problems and making sport part of the enjoyment of an
athlete's life.
Within the area of democratic leadership behavior, high school (HS)
coaches reported this behavior to a significantly higher degree than did
college coaches. Perhaps the differences within each environment, where
college athletes are at a different level of involvement than are the HS
athlete, requires differences in leadership.
When looking at training and instruction, junior high school (JH)
coaches indicated this behavior at a significantly lesser amount than
did HS and college coaches. One explanation might be the "feeder system" used within the school environment in which the study took
place. In the feeder system, coaches at the JH level are often directed
to train athletes in a manner similar to that of the HS at which the JH
athlete will attend. Because much of the training and instruction at the
JH level is directed by the HS level, JH coaches may not perceive
themselves as having the leadership behavior defined as training and
instruction. College coaches are vastly different than JH coaches in
that their success is dependent upon their own training and instruction.
Further, college coaches within this study had greater autonomy in
developing and evaluating performances than the JH coaches within the
feeder system.
As with the training and instruction leadership behavior, JH coaches
reported significantly less social support leadership behavior than did
HS and college coaches. With the differing commitments at the three
levels, JH coaches may not have the additional time to interact with
athletes on a personal level. JH coaches in this study had greater
teaching commitments and athlete to coach ratios than did the HS or
college coaches. This lack of time may impact the amount of involvement
coaches have with their athletes.
The differences found at the various coaching levels supports earlier
research by Chelladurai (1990) who suggested that leadership is only
significant within the context of the group. Further, leaders are
required to behave in relation to the demands and parameters of the
environment. The members of the different athletic environments and the
environments themselves vary greatly. What is an important, or vital,
behavior in one athletic environment may not be in another.
Additionally, there may be some outside influences which directly, or
indirectly, impact leadership that are not measured by the present
study. The RLSS, however, was able to distinguish between the coaching
levels, as did the LSS (Chelladurai & Saleh, 1978), but not
discriminate between the genders. The present study provides support for
use of the RLSS in future leadership studies. Future research using the
RLSS might focus on possible differences among sports at the same
competitive level. As suggested by Cribben (1981), the group's
perception of the leader is more important than the effectiveness of the
leader's behaviors. Chelladurai (1990) put forth that situational
characteristics and the environment impact a leader's behavior. The
environment of one sport may be much different than another and require
different behaviors from its leaders. Differences in athletes and their
respective sports may better facilitate and be facilitated by specific
leadership behaviors. Perhaps differences in leadership behavior are
influenced by the specific sport environment in which the leadership
takes place.
Other lines of research might focus on the personality traits
associated with specific leadership behaviors. Chelladurai (1990)
suggests that along with ability and experience, personality has a
strong influence on leader behavior. Certain personality characteristics
might enable a leader to succeed in one type of environment and fail in
another. Research is needed to provide a better understanding of the
leadership-personality link. Finally, research using the RLSS is needed
to investigate athletes' perceptions and preferences in relation to
the coaches' perceptions of leader behavior. This has been
successfully done with the LSS (Chelladurai, 1978; Chelladurai &
Carron, 1978). To provide more support for the RLSS, a better
understanding of the perceptions and interactions of coaches and
athletes is required.
The present study also supports the use of the RLSS as a viable
measurement of leadership behavior. It does not discriminate between
genders, yet is able to distinguish between environments. It can be a
useful tool in the defining and understanding of leadership behavior in
the sporting world.
References
Chelladurai, P. (1990). Leadership in sports: A review. International
Journal of Sport Psychology, 21, 328-354.
Chelladurai, P. (1978). A contingency model of leadership in
athletics. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of Management
Sciences, University of Waterloo, Canada.
Chelladurai, P., & Carron, A. V. (1978). Leadership. Ottawa:
Sociology of Sport Monograph Series, Canadian Association for Health,
Physical Education, and Recreation.
Chelladurai, P., & Saleh S. D. (1980). Preferred leadership in
sports. Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 34-45.
Cribben, J. J. (1981). Leadership: Strategies for organizational
effectiveness. New York: AMACOM.
Freeman, B., & Lanning, W. (1989). A multivariate analysis of the
relationship between social power motivation and personality
characteristics in college students. Journal of College Student
Development, 30(6), 522-527.
Fry, L. W., Kerr, S., & Lee, C. (1986). Effects of different
leader behaviors under different levels of task interdependence. Human
Relations, 39(12), 1067-1082.
Hersey, P., & Blanchard, H. K. (1977). Management of
organizational behavior. (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, N J:
Prentice-Hall.
Lipman-Blumen, J. (1992). Connective leadership: Female leadership
styles in the 21st-century workplace. Sociological Perspective, 35(1),
183-203.
Offermann, L. R., & Bell, C. (1992). Achievement styles of women
leaders and their peers: Toward an understanding of women and
leadership. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 16(1), 37-56.
Salminem, S., Liukkonen, J., & Telama, R. (1990, July). The
differences in coaches' and athletes' perception of leader
behavior of Finnish coaches. Presented at the AIESEP Congress,
Loughborough, England.
Zhang, J. J., Jensen, B. E, & Mann, B. L. (1996). Modification
and revision of the Leadership for Sport Scale. Journal of Sport
Behavior, 19(4).
Zhang, J. J. & Smith, D. W. (1994, November). An investigation on
leadership qualities necessary for effective intercollegiate athletic
administration. Presented at the annual meeting for the Texas
Association for Health, Physical Education, Recreation, And Dance,
Austin, TX.
For further information, please contact:
Elizabeth A. Jambor Dept. of HPER Texas tech University P.O. Box
41121 Lubbock, TX 79409-3011