The effects of self-esteem, self-efficacy, and audience presence on soccer penalty shot performance.
Geisler, Guido W.W. ; Leith, Larry M.
The research literature reflects a multidimensional approach to the
study of personality and sport performance. As one of the most popular
issues in sport psychology, it has been examined extensively over the
past three decades. Personality inquiry has considered a number of
theoretical approaches. Early applications of sport to personality work
adopted trait theory, a perspective based on general, enduring performer
characteristics and supported by many researchers (e.g., Allport, 1937;
Berger, 1970; Cattell, 1946, 1965; Cooper, 1969; Eysenck, 1967, 1970;
Guilford, 1959; Hardman, 1973; Kane, 1970, 1980; Morgan, 1980a, 1980b;
Ogilvie, 1968, 1976; Rushall, 1972; Vanek & Cratty, 1970). The
contemporary interactional model was applied to sport by Carron (1975)
and encompasses beth performer characteristics and situational demands
as determinants of behavior.
Another important area of study in sport psychology has been the
audience or spectator influence on performance. It has repeatedly been
shown that the effects of audience presence on task performance vary
with the individual and the circumstances. In their meta-analysis of 241
social facilitation studies, Bond and Titus (1983) reported that
numerous investigators found the presence of others to facilitate task
performance (Allport, 1924; Dashiell, 1930; Travis, 1925; Triplett,
1897). Other researchers documented performance decrements in the
presence of others (Husband, 1931; Kopfler, 1958; Pessin, 1933). Efforts
to clarify the nature of social facilitation were stimulated by
Zajonc's (1965) classic appropriation of drive theory as an
explanation for the influential nature of spectators. He concluded that
the mere presence of others was sufficient to increase drive, which
would in turn elicit an individual's dominant response tendency.
Cottrell (1968, 1972) expanded on Zajonc's (1965) review. He
postulated that it is the perception that others can observe and
evaluate performance, or evaluation apprehension, that facilitates drive
and thus, affects performance. The most recent interpretations of
audience effects have been offered by Bond (1982) and Leafy (1992).
These researchers, in discussing self-presentation processes, state that
self-presentational concerns, with their accompanying anxiety or
activation, can be instrumental in performance outcome.
The research contributions of Ganzer (1968) and Schrauger (1972)
suggested a relationship between social facilitation and personality
variables which has been extended to the sport domain (Bell & Yee,
1989; Calcote, 1974, 1977; Duncan & McAuley, 1987; Hall, 1979;
Kohfeld & Weitzel, 1969; Wankel, 1977). However, despite ample use
of self-concept measures in sport research, few empirical studies have
investigated the interaction of self-referent thought and audience
presence in the athletic environment.
Bandura (1977) has suggested that one's expectations are more
important predictors of behavior than is previous behavior. His social
learning theory employs self-efficacy as one such situational construct
to dictate response, in accordance with the contemporary interactional
model of sport personality research. An appropriately general
characteristic consistent with trait theory's rigidity is the
construct of self-esteem. These two personality variables can be
examined within the framework of two widely accepted social facilitation
theories. Both cognitive in nature, evaluation apprehension and
self-presentation theories emphasize that the presence of others is a
learned source of drive. Conditioned responses to spectator presence
should be further mediated by general sell-esteem and task
self-efficacy, as they are also partially attributed to previous
experiences. Thus, self-esteem and self-efficacy levels could strongly
reflect the magnitude of evaluation and self-presentational concerns.
Evaluation apprehension and self-presentation confidence should be
functions of general self-confidence and perceived task ability;
positive measures on these would result in performance facilitation.
These self-referent measures, independently or concurrently, may help
clarify what appear to be behavioral inconsistencies when athletes
confront the social environment created by the presence of spectators.
Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine the effects of
general self-esteem, task self-efficacy, and audience presence on the
performance of a sport skill the soccer penalty shot. With soccer having
become a more defensive game, matches are often low-scoring.
Subsequently, penalty shots within regulation time have increased in
significance, and penalty shootouts to decide outcomes are more
prevalent. To illustrate, there were no penalty shootouts in World Cup
history/prior to 1982. Since then, shootouts have been a part of every
World Cup tournament, and the 1994 championship final was in fact
decided in this manner.
Despite its general description as relatively simple in mechanical
terms, the penalty shot is often associated with performance anxiety and
variability. It is widely considered to be a psychological rather than
mechanical skill, and success is a function of optimum arousal and focus
or concentration. This is largely due to the fact that performance
outcome is of considerable importance, especially in close matches.
Consequently, common characteristics are frequently attributed to
consistent penalty shooters. However, these popular assumptions have not
been validated empirically. It was the aim of this study to identify
potential factors associated with penalty shot success.
Player confidence and willingness are prime determinants of shooter
selection; thus, the self-referent measures selected for investigation
may have instrumental roles in the delineation of successful shooters.
For example, competent penalty shooters could conceivably exhibit higher
levels of self-esteem or task-specific self-efficacy than other players,
regardless of role or status within the team. These attributes could
then engender a predisposition to the task. Indeed, low profile or
substitute players are frequently enlisted for penalty shot situations
in lieu of more skilled or high profile teammates. Moreover,
unsuccessful penalty shots occur with players of all abilities, a
phenomenon most recently (and popularly) evidenced by the 1994 World Cup
final match - Franco Baresi and Roberto Baggio, both prominent
international figures, failed in their penalty shot attempts for Italy
and occasioned Brazil's championship title. To be sure, the
self-referent variables of self-esteem and self-efficacy may be more
significant factors in penalty shot performance than general playing
skill or status.
Additional information may also stem from spectator influences. The
penalty shot is never performed in isolation; it attracts the undivided attention of all observers present, including players, coaches,
officials, and spectators. At the elite level, the shot is also on
display for thousands of attending spectators and millions of television
viewers. The shooter is cognizant of the audience, and the interactions
of the aforementioned player attributes with observer presence may
further reveal the constituents of successful penalty shots.
Methods and Procedures
Subjects
Forty male intercollegiate soccer players (range in age of 19-34
years, M = 23.8, SD = 3.5) served as subjects for the investigation.
Participants were recruited from the University of Toronto, York
University, and Carleton University varsity soccer teams, all of whom
are members of the Ontario Universities Athletic Association (O.U.A.A.).
All participants were either current players or recent, active alumni.
Each alumnus had graduated within the last 10 years, and was therefore
still deemed to be within "playing age." In total, 18 alumni
players were employed in the study, while the remaining 22 subjects were
current University of Toronto players (subjects were considered current
if they were team members at either the onset or completion of testing).
All subjects resided in the Toronto area and, as varsity players, were
considered skilled in terms of the soccer penalty shot. Thus, the
experimenter regarded the task to be measured as simple and well-learned
for all participants.
Research Design
The study employed a 2 x 2 x 2 analysis of variance (ANOVA). The
dependent variable was assessed as either goal or no goal on each
penalty shot, and every subject performed 10 shots. Level of
self-esteem, level of self-efficacy, and no audience/audience presence
served as the main effects and as the independent variables of the
investigation. An independent t test was also performed to determine if
self-reported subject activation levels differed significantly between
the no audience and audience conditions. A criterion level of .05 was
set for the study.
Questionnaires
Subjects were categorized according to the following criteria:
High/low self-esteem, high/low self-efficacy, and audience/no audience
presence during task performance. Self-esteem measures were obtained
using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965, 1979). The
questionnaire consists of 10 items which are answered on a scale of 1 to
4 (strongly agree, agree, disagree, strongly disagree, respectively).
Scoring of this scale is carded out relative to other subjects in the
sample; the upper half of scores is considered to denote high
self-esteem, while the lower half represents low self-esteem (Rosenberg,
1965).
Self-efficacy was assessed using a 10-point Likert scale to indicate
subjects' perceived competence in the task relative to other
O.U.A.A. varsity soccer players. The scale ranges from 1 (not as good as
most other players) to 10 (better than most other players). Self-ratings
of 6 or higher were deemed high self-efficacy; ratings of 5 or less
constituted low self-efficacy.
Activation level was determined through one item of Orlick's
(1986) scale, "Knowing Your Competitive Self". This question
employs an 11-point Likert rating, from 0 (no activation - mentally and
physically flat) to 10 (highly activated - mentally and physically
charged).
Confidentiality of all results and questionnaire scores was
guaranteed to participants through the use of a numbered coding system.
Subjects were assigned a personal number, and these numbers were used to
identify subjects for all recording, tabulation, and analysis purposes.
This coding system was adopted primarily to ensure honesty in
questionnaire responses, reducing the likelihood that responses would
reflect subjects' perceived social desirability.
Penalty Shot Task
All testing was carded out at the University of Toronto's
Varsity Stadium. The stadium is fully enclosed and therefore enabled the
subjects and goalkeeper to be free of all disturbances or influences
during the no audience condition. The same soccer ball was used for all
testing, and the experimenter attempted to keep the ball at a consistent
level of inflation. All subjects performed their shots on the same goal.
Audience Condition
In the audience condition the experimenter and 4 other subjects
served as the test subject's audience. In order to eliminate
interactive and uncontrollable variables, the audience was set up to be
noninteractive. During the no audience condition, the subjects not being
tested, as well as the experimenter, remained out of view in a closed
changeroom.
Experimental Procedure
The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale was administered several weeks prior
to the start of task performance to obtain a self-esteem measure removed
from the soccer environment. The self-efficacy rating was obtained at
the same time, with instructions that it was to reflect perceived
competence, relative to other O.U.A.A. players, on penalty shots only.
Both questionnaires were administered personally by the experimenter.
Through partitioning of questionnaire scores, 20 subjects were
classified under high self-esteem, and 20 subjects under low
self-esteem. In addition, 20 subjects were designated as high
self-efficacy, and 20 subjects as low self-efficacy. Thirdly, random
assignment exposed 20 subjects to the audience condition, and 20
subjects to the no audience condition.
Task performance testing was done on 8 subgroups of 5 subjects per
session. These subgroups were formed on the basis of participant
availability and audience/no audience assignment, not on questionnaire
scores. This format was designed to minimize the amount of time
participants were required to devote to a session. The activation scale
was administered to each subject immediately after testing, to assess
the activation level that was attained just prior to shooting.
One goalkeeper was used in the study, as a means of eliminating the
variable of goalkeeper ability. He was a current member of the varsity
team. The entire testing protocol was carded out over a 6-month period,
thereby reducing the possibilities of staleness and overexposure on
behalf of the goalkeeper.
Upon arrival at Varsity Stadium, the testing location, the goalkeeper
was asked to warm up on the field. The procedure was then as follows:
Audience subgroups. The first subject completed five warmup penalty
shots without the goalkeeper, while the remaining four subjects took
practice shots on the goalkeeper at the other end of the field. Once
this preparatory phase was over, the four nontesting subjects watched
from either side of the penalty area (two per side). The experimenter
joined two of the audience subjects while the goalkeeper positioned
himself in goal. The audience members were instructed not to make any
overt gestures or comments. The subject then completed 10 penalty shots,
from the penalty spot, in this competitive setup. The shooter was
instructed to attempt to score as often as possible, and that the totals
for all players would be compared. The goalkeeper was told that he
should try to stop every shot with a consistent effort. Upon completion
of his test shots, each player was administered the activation scale.
The remaining subjects were then tested in the same manner. However,
during their warmup periods the goalkeeper simply waited at the side of
the field, with his back turned. He was able to relax or stretch as
necessary.
No audience subgroups. The preparatory phase was as that described
above. However, once this phase was completed, only the subject and the
goalkeeper remained on the field. The other subjects and the
experimenter moved to the closed changeroom. The aforementioned
procedure was then followed, with similar instructions. After finishing
the 10 shots, both the subject and the goalkeeper returned to the
changeroom to report the total. The next subject was then sent out to
warm up. Following this, he summoned the goalkeeper, who returned to the
field, and the same testing procedure ensued. Again, activation level
was measured after testing for each subject. In addition, all players
were instructed not to discuss goalie behavior or weaknesses with fellow
subjects.
Results
Scores on the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale produced a range of 27-39
out of a possible score of 40. The upper and lower halves of obtained
scores were divided in order to dichotomize subjects into high
self-esteem and low self-esteem groups, as per the method advised by
Rosenberg (1965). Scores of 35 and higher fell into the high self-esteem
category (M = 36.75). Scores of 34 and lower were categorized as low
self-esteem (M = 31.65). Self-efficacy perceptions were scored more
directly. On the 10-point Likert scale, ratings of 6 or higher denoted
high self-efficacy (M = 7.50). Scores of 5 or lower indicated low
self-efficacy (M = 4.45).
The data were analyzed using a 2 x 2 x 2 analysis of variance. The
results are summarized in Table 1. Level of self-esteem (high/low),
level of self-efficacy (high/low), and no audience/audience presence
were the three factors under examination.
At the .05 criterion level, the significant value for F(1,32) = 4.15.
In examining self-esteem, no significant main effect was obtained,
F(1,32) = 0.01, p [greater than] .05. This result suggests that general
self-esteem did not have any bearing on penalty shot performance in the
investigation.
Examination of self-efficacy revealed no significant main effect,
F(1,32) = 0.88, p [greater than] .05. The implication of this finding is
that task self-efficacy, as a situational measure, was also not
instrumental in performance of the penalty shot.
Finally, examination of no audience/audience presence failed to find
a significant main effect, F (1,32) = 1.63, p [greater than] .05.
Penalty shot performance, therefore, was not influenced by either the
presence or absence of an audience.
In addition, the analysis indicated that there were no significant
interaction effects. Thus, none of the independent variables described
had any concurrent or combination effects on penalty shot performance.
In this investigation, level of general self-esteem, level of task
self-efficacy, and the presence or absence of an audience all failed to
show any significant effects or interactions during performance of the
soccer penalty shot for male intercollegiate soccer players. A summary
of mean performance values and standard deviations is provided in Table
2.
Table 1
Complete Analysis of Variance Summary Table
for Penalty Shot Performance
Source of Variation SS df MS F
A: Self-esteem 0.03 1 0.03 0.01
B: Self-efficacy 3.03 1 3.03 0.88
C: No audience/ 5.63 1 5.63 1.63
audience
A x B 0.03 1 0.03 0.01
A x C 0.23 1 0.23 0.07
B x C 0.03 1 0.03 0.01
A x B x C 3.00 1 3.00 0.88
Error 110.80 32 3.46
*p [less than] .05.
Table 2
Mean Number of Goals and Standard Deviations
for All Experimental Measures
Classification M(*) SD
High self-esteem 6.35 1.46
Low self-esteem 6.30 2.08
High self-efficacy 6.60 2.09
Low self-efficacy 6.05 1.39
No audience 6.70 1.78
Audience 5.95 1.73
* Maximum number of goals = 10
Anecdotal evidence was also gathered from participants to assess
activation levels generated by the testing environment. The activation
scale yielded minimal response differences between subjects performing
in the no audience and audience conditions. On a scale from 0 to 10, the
mean activation rating by subjects in the no audience condition was 5.45
(range of 2-9). Subjects exposed to the audience condition reported a
mean rating of 6.05 (range of 5-8). Thus, the difference in testing
environments produced a mean difference in subject activation reports of
0.60 on the scale. An independent t test on this data revealed that the
activation levels created by the no audience and audience conditions
were not significantly different (t = -1.15; t .05,38 =1.69; p [greater
than] .05). From this evidence it appears that the environment
manipulation was unsuccessful in creating an arousing audience
condition. The possibility also exists that athletes at the
intercollegiate level may not be significantly affected by the presence
of peer group audiences, or fellow intercollegiate players in
particular.
Discussion
The research findings do not support the hypotheses that penalty shot
performance in soccer is affected by levels of general self-esteem or
task self-efficacy, nor by spectator presence or absence. Thus, we were
unable to identify common player characteristics associated with penalty
shot success.
However, the literature favors contentions that prior learning and
performance history, as well as personality characteristics of
performers, are important variables in social facilitation study (e.g.,
Bell & Yee, 1989; Calcote, 1974, 1977; Cottrell, 1968; Ganzer, 1968;
Kohfeld & Weitzel, 1969; Schrauger, 1972; Wankel, 1977).
Furthermore, the examination of competitive task performance has renewed
its foundation in personality and self-referent thought (Fisher, 1984;
Martens, Vealey & Burton, 1990; McAuley, 1992; Vealey, 1990).
Consequently, we must consider that self-esteem and self-efficacy may
not mirror evaluation apprehension or self-presentation confidence as
posited. Application of both theories to social facilitation research
has upheld their claims. Conversely, self-referent measures have
received scant attention in this context. The absence of empirical
support currently supersedes any valid intimations of association
between self-esteem, self-efficacy, self-presentation confidence, and
evaluation apprehension.
Subsequent studies may be wise to address self-presentation style
more directly. Despite the pervasiveness of self-presentational motives,
little direct attention has been devoted to the self-presentational
aspects of sport (Leary, 1992). In contrast, as Leafy (1992) confirms,
research questions have emphasized measures of the private self. In
self-presentation processes, people monitor and control how they are
perceived by others (Schlenker, 1980). Accordingly, more relevant and
reliable measures may fall under indices of social anxiety, an affective state whereby individuals doubt they will successfully cast desired
impressions on others (Schlenker & Leary, 1982). Measures of anxiety
in competitive or performance situations, such as the SCAT (Martens,
1977) and the CSAI-2 (Martens, Burton, Vealey, Bump & Smith, 1983),
may be more predictive of the personality and performance relationship.
Such measures would accurately assess competitive or social anxiety and,
under audience conditions, may better reflect evaluation apprehension
and self-presentational concerns. Relevance to soccer penalty shots
could also hold true for other outcome-dependent sport skills, such as
basketball foul shots, baseball pitches, golf putts, and football
field-goal kicks.
The construct of self-esteem engendered an additional potential
research ambiguity in the investigation. In terms of the Rosenberg
Self-Esteem Scale, the items can be somewhat susceptible to socially
desirable responses (Rosenberg, 1965). The test, however, does not
employ a social desirability or lie scale. Furthermore, and of
scientific pertinence, Rosenberg score distributions tend to produce
"low" self-esteem groups that display relatively high
self-esteem levels in absolute terms (Rosenberg, 1965). Given that group
classification is relative to the scores of the sample population, it is
possible that the subjects in the study did not adequately differ in
self-esteem levels. As a result, effects which could be attributed to
self-esteem variance may have been lost due to undetected subject
homogeneity.
The lack of performance differences between high and low self-esteem
subjects can be further explained by the relationship between
self-esteem and anxiety. Subjects high in self-esteem have reported less
general anxiety than subjects low on this measure (Fiedler, Dodge, Jones
& Hutchins, 1958; Worchel, 1957). High anxiety levels have also been
found to impair complex task performance (Spence, Farber & McFann,
1956). Low self-esteem subjects in the study, therefore, would be
expected to score fewer goals. However, the penalty shot task was
considered simple and well-learned, as all participants were varsity
players. Thus, general anxiety associated with the low self-esteem
subjects was apparently of little consequence, since the skill was not
complex or novel. Situational anxiety may have been more influential in
performance (Schrauger, 1972), but it was not necessarily related to
self-esteem. Furthermore, situational activation reports showed that
activation was relatively constant for all subjects (t = -1.15, p
[greater than] .05). Hence, the nature of the task may have helped
minimize the possible effects of general activation and, possibly, of
general anxiety. This provided a further reason for the failure of
self-esteem to affect penalty shot performance in the investigation.
It was also believed that subjects high in self-efficacy would
perform significantly better than their low self-efficacy counterparts.
However, this hypothesis was rejected. As with self-esteem, reduced
subject variability may have resulted from dichotomizing the
self-efficacy measures. In addition, the hypothesis was based in part on
research which demonstrated performance decrements in low self-efficacy
individuals (Schrauger, 1972). These decrements were attributed to
higher levels of anxiety specific to a test situation. Since activation
levels in this study proved to be similar across all subjects, it could
also be assumed that anxiety levels were accordingly similar. Hence,
there was little reason to expect corresponding performance differences.
The foregoing discussion must not be interpreted to undermine the
value of self-esteem and self-efficacy examination. Vealey (1980)
suggests that there are correlations between competition, intrapersonal variables, and self-presentation confidence. McAuley (1992) calls for
additional, sport-oriented research into the relationships between
self-efficacy, audience presence, and anxiety. This study's
self-referent measures, alone and under social influence, remain
significant and relevant lines of inquiry. By reason of the documented
measurement limitations and lack of empirical precedence, further study
in the athletic setting is indeed recommended.
Social and environmental influences were also factors of interest in
this investigation. There is some evidence (e.g., Landers &
McCullagh, 1976) that performance quality (as opposed to speed or
endurance) of either simple or complex tasks is not affected by the
presence of others. This assertion could be viewed as partially
supportive of the experimental findings, since task objectives were
qualitative in nature. However, as recognized in the literature, the
magnitude of environmental manipulations is a strong determinant of
social facilitation effects. A more complete and probable account for
the absence of main audience effects in this study lies in the nature of
the audience condition.
Research regarding the influence of audience size has been
contradictory. Studies have demonstrated that the number of spectators
is relatively unimportant in motor performance (Bond & Titus, 1983;
McCullagh & Landers, 1976; Wankel, 1977), although arousal levels
have been positively correlated with the number of observers present.
Edwards (1979) infers that audience density and intimacy may be more
influential than audience size. Other research, especially field study,
suggests that performance level increases with audience size, most
notably for home teams (Schwartz & Barsky, 1977). The latter
proposal has implications for this study's results. The size of the
audience assembled, with five members, was reasonably large with respect
to previous research we reviewed. Although some investigators have
recruited in excess of five observers (e.g., Dube & Tatz, 1991;
Kohfeld & Weitzel, 1969; MacCracken & Stadulis, 1985; Paulus
& Cornelius, 1974), most experimenters have created audience
conditions with fewer than five spectators (e.g., Bell & Yee, 1989;
Calcote, 1974, 1977; Carver & Scheier, 1978; Forgas, Brennan, Howe,
Kane & Sweet, 1980; Hollifield, 1982; Murray, 1983; Schrauger,
1972). It appears that the audience size employed here did not increase
arousal, as shown by mean activation scores of 6.05/10 with an audience
and 5.45/10 without an audience. Thus, it is possible that a larger
audience would have increased subject arousal and thereby altered the
effects of self-esteem and self-efficacy.
A final research recommendation submits to the cognitive bases of
social facilitation theory. It is plausible that the presence of
arousing spectators, when combined with other situational variables, may
influence penalty shooters. The operant influences, nevertheless, are
likely attributed to these situational and self-referent thought
processes. As the experimental evidence suggests, there are situations
in which drive theory fails to explain audience effects. Cox (1990)
states that it may be necessary to turn to Easterbrook's (1959) cue
utilization theory in such situations. This theory is based on
constructs of audience distraction and attentional focus. Additional
explanations are offered by Duval and Wicklund (1972). These theorists
propose that the presence of others raises the individual's
objective self-awareness. Both theories reflect cognitive foundations in
their accounts of social facilitation effects. While drive
interpretations have received extensive consideration in the social
facilitation literature, cognitive interpretations may be equally
relevant. This is especially true when activation or drive is seemingly
unaffected by experimental treatments, as in this investigation. Future
studies may wish to make greater use of these or other cognition-based
theories.
This research was unable to identify common characteristics
associated with penalty shot success. Accepted practice in the soccer
community is to select shooters on the basis of their confidence and
willingness. Essentially, these shooters select themselves. In the
absence of definitive data to contest this procedure, the authors
recommend its continuation.
References
Allport, F.H. (1924). Social psychology. Cambridge, MA: Riverside.
Allport, G.W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation.
New York: Holt.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.
Bell, P.A., & Yee, L.A. (1989). Skill level and audience effects
on performance of a karate drill. Journal of Social Psychology, 129,
191-200.
Berger, B.C. (1970). Effect of three sport environmental factors upon
selected personality characteristics of athletes. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Teachers College, Columbia University.
Bond, C.F., Jr. (1982). Social facilitation: A self-presentational
view. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 1042-1050.
Bond, C.F., Jr., & Titus, L.J. (1983). Social facilitation: A
meta-analysis of 241 studies. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 265-292.
Calcote, LG. (1974). The interaction of extraversion, neuroticism,
and audience presence in the performance of a choice reaction time task.
Unpublished master's thesis, Northwestern State University.
Calcote, L.G. (1977). The interaction of introversion, neuroticism,
and the presence of an evaluative audience on simple reaction time
performance (Doctoral dissertation, University of Southern Mississippi,
1977). Dissertation Abstracts International, 5003B.
Carron, A.V. (1975). Personality and athletics: A review. In BS.
Rushall (Ed.), The status of psychomotor learning and sport psychology
research. Dartmouth, N.S.: Sport Science Associates.
Carver, C.S., & Scheier, M.F. (1978). Self-focusing effects of
dispositional self-consciousness, mirror presence, and audience
presence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 324-332.
Cattell, R.B. (1946). Description and measurement of personality.
Yonkers-on-Hudson, NY: World.
Cattell, R.B. (1965). The scientific analysis of personality.
Baltimore: Penguin Books. Cooper, L. (1969). Athletics, activity, and
personality: A review of the literature. Research Quarterly, 40, 17-22.
Cottrell, N.B. (1968). Performance in the presence of other human
beings: Mere presence, audience, and affiliation effects. In E.C.
Simmel, R.A. Hoppe, & S.A. Milton (Eds.), Social facilitation and
imitative behavior. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Cottrell, N.B. (1972). Social facilitation. In C.G. McClintock (Ed.),
Experimental social psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Cox, R.H. (1990). Sport psychology: Concepts and applications (2nd
ed.). Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown.
Dashiell, J.F. (1930). An experimental analysis of some group
effects. Abnormal Social Psychology, 25, 190-199.
Dube, S.K., & Tatz, S.J. (1991). Audience effects in tennis
performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 73, 844-846.
Duncan, T., & McAuley, E. (1987). Efficacy expectations and
perceptions of causality in motor performance. Journal of Sport
Psychology, 9, 385-393.
Duval, S., & Wicklund, RA. (1972). A theory of objective
self-awareness. New York: Academic.
Easterbrook, J.A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization
and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183-201.
Edwards, J. (1979). The home field advantage. In J.H. Goldstein
(Ed.), Sports, games, and play: Social and psychological viewpoints.
Hillsdale, N J: Halstead.
Eysenck, H.J. (1967). The biological basis of personality.
Springfield, IL: Thomas.
Eysenck, H.J. (1970). The structure of human personality (3rd ed.).
London: Methuen.
Fiedler, F.E, Dodge, J.S., Jones, RE., & Hutchins, E.B. (1958).
Interrelations among measures of personality adjustment in nonclinical
populations. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 56, 345-351.
Fisher, A.C. (1984). New directions in sport personality research. In
J.M. Silva & R.S. Weinberg (Eds.), Psychological foundations of
sport (pp. 70-80). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Forgas, J.P., Brennan, G., Howe, S, Kane, J.F., & Sweet, S.
(1980). Audience effects on squash players' performance. Journal of
Social Psychology, 111, 41-47.
Ganzer, V.J. (1968). Effects of audience presence and test anxiety on
learning and retention in a serial learning situation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 8, 194-199.
Guilford, J.P. (1959). Personality. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Hall, E.G. (1979). Interaction of locus of control, sex, competition
and coaction during performance of a novel motor task (Doctoral
dissertation, University of Virginia, 1977). Dissertation Abstracts
International, 39, 4121A.
Hardman, K. (1973). A dual approach to the study of personality and
performance in sport. In H.T.A. Whiting, K. Hardman, L.B. Hendry, &
M.G. Jones (Eds.), Personality and performance in physical education and
sport. London: Kimpton.
Hollifield, N.L. (1982). Effect of prior performance experience
before audiences on a dominant and nondominant motor response. Journal
of Sport Psychology, 4, 317-323.
Husband, R.W. (1931). Effects of social stimulation on human maze
learning. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 39.
Kane, J.E. (1970). Personality and physical abilities. In G.S. Kenyon
(Ed.), Contemporary psychology of sport: Second International Congress
of Sports Psychology. Chicago: The Athletic Institute.
Kane, J.E. (1980). Personality research: The current controversy and
implications for sport studies. In W.F. Straub (Ed.), Sport psychology:
An analysis of athlete behavior (2nd ed.). Ithaca, NY: Mouvement.
Kohfeld, D.L., & Weitzel, W. (1969). Some relations between
personality factors and social facilitation. Journal of Experimental
Research in Personality, 3, 287-292.
Kopfler, P.H. (1958). Influence of social interactions on learning
rates in birds. Science, 128, 903.
Landers, D.M., & McCullagh, P.D. (1976). Social facilitation of
motor performance. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 4, 125-162.
Leary, M.R. (1992). Self-presentational processes in exercise and
sport. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 14, 339-351.
MacCracken, M.J., & Stadulis, R.E. (1985). Social facilitation of
young children's dynamic balance performance. Journal of Sport
Psychology, 7, 150-165.
Martens, R. (1977). Sport competition anxiety test. Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics.
Martens, R., Burton, D., Vealey, R.S., Bump, L.A., & Smith, D.E.
(1983). Competitive state anxiety inventory-2. Unpublished manuscript,
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
Martens, R., Vealey, R.S., & Burton, D. (1990). Competitive
anxiety in sport. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
McAuley, E. (1992). Self-referent thought in sport and physical
activity. In T.S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology. Champaign,
IL: Human Kinetics.
McCullagh, P.D., & Landers, D.M. (1976). Size of audience and
social facilitation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 42, 1067-1070.
Morgan, W.P. (1980a). Sport personology: The credulous-skeptical
argument in perspective. In W.F. Straub (Ed.), Sport Psychology: An
analysis of athlete behavior (2nd ed.). Ithaca, NY: Mouvement.
Morgan, W.P. (1980b). The trait psychology controversy. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 51, 50-76.
Murray, J.F. (1983). Effects of alone and audience on motor
performance for males and females. International Journal of Sport
Psychology, 14, 92-97.
Ogilvie, B.C. (1968). Psychological consistencies within the
personality of high-level competitors. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 205, 780-786.
Ogilvie, B.C. (1976). Psychological consistencies within the
personality of high-level competitors. In A.C. Fisher (Ed.), Psychology
of sport. Palo Alto, CA: Mayfield.
Orlick, T. (1986). Coaches training manual to psyching for sport.
Champaign, IL: Leisure.
Paulus, P.B., & Cornelius, W.L. (1974). An analysis of gymnastic
performance are under conditions of practice and spectator observation.
Research Quarterly, 45, 56-63.
Pessin, J. (1933). The comparative effects of social and mechanical
stimulation upon memorizing. American Journal of Psychology, 45,
263-270.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image.
Princeton, N J: Princeton University Press.
Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books.
Rushall, B.S. (1972). Three studies relating personality variables to
football performance. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 3,
12-24.
Schlenker, B R. (1980). Impression management: The self-concept,
social identity, and interpersonal relations. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Schlenker, B.R., & Leary, MR. (1982). Social anxiety and
self-presentation: A conceptualization and model. Psychological
Bulletin, 92, 641-669.
Schrauger, J.S. (1972). Self-esteem and reactions to being observed
by others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23, 192-200.
Schwartz, B, & Barsky, S.F. (1977). The home advantage. Social
Forces, 55, 641-661.
Spence, K.W., Farber, I.E., & McFann, H.H. (1956). The relation
of anxiety (drive) level to performance in competitional and
noncompetitional paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 52, 296-305.
Travis, L.E. (1925). The effect of a small audience upon eye-hand
coordination. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 20, 142-146.
Triplett, N. (1897). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and
competition. American Journal of Psychology, 9, 507-553.
Vanek, M, & Cratty, B.J. (1970). Psychology and the superior
athlete. London: Macmillan.
Vealey, R.S. (1990). Advancements in competitive anxiety research:
Use of the Sport Competition Anxiety Test and the Competitive State
Anxiety Inventory-2. Anxiety Research, 2, 243-261.
Wankel, LM (1977). Audience size and trait anxiety effects upon state
anxiety and motor performance. Research Quarterly, 48, 181-186.
Worchel, P. (1957). Adaptability screen of flying personnel:
Development of a self-concept inventory for predicting maladjustment.
(Tech. Rep. No. 56-62) United States Air Force, School of Aviation
Medicine.
Zajonc, R.B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269-274.
For further information, please contact:
Guido W. W. Geisler University of Toronto School of Physical and
Health Education Toronto, Canada