Coping with acute stress among American and Australian basketball referees.
Anshel, Mark H. ; Weinberg, Robert S.
Psychological stress typically occurs when the environment is
appraised by an individual as taxing or exceeding personal resources
(Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Whereas chronic stress consists of
experiences that are perceived as threatening, harmful, or challenging
over an extended time period, acute stress refers to the sudden exposure
to stimuli that elicit similar perceptions. Examples of acute stressors
In sport including making a mental or physical error, experiencing pain
or injury, an opponent's successful performance and receiving a
"bad" call from an official. Acute stressors can negatively
affect numerous cognitive and psychophysiological processes such as
concentration, attentional focus, effort and arousal, and lead to
impaired motor performance (Anshel, 1990; Jones & Hardy, 1989).
Central to success in sport is the ability to enact cognitive and/or behavioral strategies to cope with acute stress.
Coping consists of psychological and behavioral efforts to master,
reduce, or tolerate demands (Folkman & Lazarus, 1985). These efforts
consist of learned behavioral and emotional responses that reduce the
importance of a dangerous or unpleasant condition. Researchers are in
uniform agreement that the ability to cope effectively with stress is a
significant factor in determining good health, well-being, and sport
performance (Gauron, 1986; Smith, 1986). Coping with acute stress in
sport is a function of the competitor's perception, or appraisal,
of a situation, that is, "...the extent to which a person believes
that he or she can shape or influence a particular stressful
person-environment relationship" (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984, p.
69).
There is evidence in the counseling psychology literature that
different stressors require various types of coping strategies (e.g.,
Endler & Parker, 1990; Terry, 1991). The most common categorizations
of coping strategies are referred to as problem-focused (behavior) and
emotion-focused (cognitive) coping (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).
Problem-focused coping is the use of one or more activities implemented
to achieve a task objective. For example, an athlete may cope with
unpleasant comments from others by placing themselves at a distant
physical proximity from the person or, conversely, by performing more
aggressively. A basketball referee may cope with the stress of a
coach's verbal abuse by giving a technical foul or warning to the
individual, or by simply walking away from the stress source.
Conversely, emotion-focused coping consists of using thoughts or
emotions to feel better about performing the task. Hence, if a referee
feels upset after making a wrong call during the contest, appropriate
emotional coping strategies may include positive self-talk (e.g.,
"Keep at it; stay calm" or "Stay ready;
concentrate"), or discounting in which the event's importance
is minimized or at least temporarily forgotten.
Another conceptual framework for studying the coping process has been
to examine a person's disposition, or tendency, to use particular
categories of strategies called approach and avoidance coping styles
(Krohne, 1993; Roth & Cohen, 1986). Approach coping reflects the
individual's preference to examine or obtain more information about
the source of stress. Avoidance coping, on the other hand, indicates a
preference to reduce the importance, or attention, toward the stressor.
For example, Krohne and Hindel (1988) found that skilled table tennis
players tend to use avoidance coping in response to performance errors
during the match. This would appear to reflect the player's need to
maintain attentional control on task, and to either discount or ignore
most stressors which may interfere with performance demands.
In a recent study, Gould, Eklund, and Jackson (1993) examined the
coping strategies of the 1988 U.S. Olympic Wrestling Team. The
wrestlers' primary coping strategies, used by 80% of them, were
categorized as thought-control (e.g., blocking distractions, perspective
taking, positive thinking, coping thoughts, and prayer). Less used
coping techniques included task-focused (e.g., narrow, more immediate
focus, concentrating on goals), emotional-control (e.g., arousal
control, visualization), and behavioral strategies (e.g., changing or
controlling the environment; following a set routine). The authors
concluded that "coping strategies are most effective if linked to
the domain in which stress is being experienced" (p. 91). Thus,
behavioral (problem-focused) coping strategies are more likely to
succeed under conditions of relatively high self-control or in rapidly
changing situations such as in open-skill, team sports, whereas
cognitive (emotion-focused) coping strategies may be more effective
under conditions of low self-control and slowly changing situations,
most evident in closed-skill, individual sports. Arguably, the most
fundamental issue in coping with acute stress is self-control. As Smith
and Nye (1989) conclude, "...learning to control such emotionality
will facilitate generalization of coping skills across stressful
situations" (p. 18). In addition to being situationally determined,
the use of selected coping techniques are also a function of personal
(dispositional) factors.
Although there has been some research investigating the use of coping
strategies employed by athletes, there is a dearth of literature
focusing on the use of coping strategies to deal with acute stressors by
sport officials. Along these lines, in their review of literature,
Weinberg and Richardson (1990) acknowledge that sport officials
experience an extensive amount of acute stress, much of which directly
influences their performance quality. Examples of acute stressors for
officials, common in the anecdotal literature (see Weinberg &
Richardson, 1990, for a review) include making a "wrong" call,
verbal abuse and threats of physical abuse from coaches, players and
spectators, pain from an injury, criticism in the media, and evaluation
by a supervisor, among others. Effective sport officiating consists of
meeting situational demands in response to an array of acute stressors
experienced during the contest, thereby maintaining optimal performance.
For example, Taylor, Daniel, Leith and Burke (1990) found that burnout among sports officials was most related to stress from interpersonal conflicts, role culture conflict, fear of failure and other evaluative
aspects of officiating. Taken together, it appears that burnout and
premature retirement may partially reflect the failure to cope
effectively with acute stress. In essence, when perceived role demands
are inconsistent with an official's abilities, goals, values or
beliefs, chronic stress, burnout and dropout from further participation
may result (Dale & Weinberg, 1990). Clearly, the ability to cope
effectively with acute stress involved in officiating is critical, not
only to effective officiating, but also as a buffer against burnout.
Besides the fact that there is little known about the coping
strategies employed by sport officials, there is even less known about
any potential cross cultural differences in the use of coping
strategies. Duda and Allison (1990) have recognized the importance of
examining cultural differences to understand better the psychological
factors that influence thoughts, emotions, and behavior, with each
culture influencing the types of experiences and perceptions of those
sport experiences. They argue that if cultural variations in sport
activities are not considered, then the evolving theoretical perspective
may be misleading. In the only study examining cross-cultural
comparisons among sport officials, Anshel and Weinberg (1995) found that
selected sources of stress differed markedly between basketball referees
from Australia and the United States. Specifically, the stressors,
"Making a Wrong Call. "Verbal Abuse By Players",
"Verbal Abuse By Spectators", and "Arguing With
Players", significantly discriminated between American and
Australian basketball referees. As an extension of this study, the
purpose of the present investigation was to cross-culturally examine the
coping responses of basketball referees to selected acute stressors. It
was hypothesized that some coping techniques (problem-focused/behavioral
vs. emotion-focused/cognitive) and coping styles (approach and
avoidance) would be used more than others for a given stressor, and that
American and Australian referees would differ in the use of coping
strategies.
Method
Subjects
Subjects were 137 adult male basketball referees who volunteered to
participate in this study. This included 75 officials from the
southwestern United States (ages 19-45 yrs, Md age = 32.6 yrs), and 62
officials from New South Wales, Australia (ages 20-37 yrs, Md age = 29.8
yrs). Criteria for participation included age (18 yrs or older) and
officiating experience in organized, competitive basketball (minimum of
three years of officiating experience in high school in the U.S. sample,
or Level A or B competition in Australia) for which the person received
renumeration. The level of expertise was "intermediate" (Level
2) or "advanced" level (Level I) in their respective referee
organizations. Seventy-five of 116 surveys (65%) mailed to U.S. subjects
were returned. Sixty-two of the 110 Australian referees attending a
basketball referees conference (56%) returned the distributed surveys.
Generation of the Survey
According to Duda and Allison (1990), cross-cultural research may
include self-report methods using standard questionnaires provided that
the scales are valid and conceptually equivalent in the cultural
context. Consequently, the Basketball Official's Sources of Stress
Inventory (BOSSI) was developed for this study to ascertain the extent
to which the subjects used various cognitive and behavioral strategies
for coping with each of 15 sources of acute stress. The BOSSI was used
by Anshel and Weinberg (1995) to determine the level of intensity of
each acute stressor. In the present study, subjects were asked to write
their cognitive and behavioral coping responses for each stressor when
experienced at its highest intensity. A copy of the BOSSI, appears in
Appendix A. (see Anshel and Weinberg, 1995, for a description in
generating the BOSSI, including content and construct validation procedures).
Results and Discussion
The primary objective of the data analyses were to compare American
and Australian basketball referees on their respective use of behavioral
and cognitive coping strategies in response to each of 15 acute
stressors usually experienced during the game.
Frequency Analysis on Coping Strategies
The order of presenting these data reflect the sequence presented in
the survey. Frequency data consisting of the number of subjects using
various problem-focused (behavioral) and emotional-focused (cognitive)
coping strategies for U.S. and Australian subjects were compiled. The
assignment of coping strategies to behavioral and emotional categories
was accomplished using an deductive content analysis in which
subjects' responses were assigned to one of the two categories of
coping strategies for each stressor (Patton, 1990).
Two researchers independently categorized the subjects'
responses which consisted of examples of coping with the stressors.
Consensus validation was completed by the mutual agreement between
analysts on the category assignment for each strategy. One task of the
researchers was to determine if the coping categories were discrete. For
example, the referees reported that the stressor "Verbal Abuse From
Coaches" referred to name-calling or remarks of a personal nature,
but was distinct from the stressor "Making Physical Threats."
Similar to the technique used by Scanlan et al. (1989), validation
occurred after the analysts independently identified and grouped the
responses. High inter-rater reliability (r=.87) indicated strong
agreement between the researchers in this process. Table 1 lists the
different types of coping strategies used by both groups. It is
important to point out that the list of coping strategies do not reveal
the extent to which these techniques are effective at reducing the
referees' stress level or performance. Rather, they merely reflect
the subjects' thoughts and actions after experiencing any of these
stressors. Due to the uneven sample size between groups and differences
in the use of coping strategies, results are discussed in terms of the
percentage of using each coping strategy.
Abuse by Coach
Coach abuse is among the most common and, according to Anshel and
Weinberg (1995), the second most severe source of stress for basketball
referees Two group differences were of particular interest.
Specifically, far more Americans (39%) than Australians (13%) used the
coping strategy of calmly speaking to the coach. This could be partly
due to the Australians self-reported propensity to ignore or forget
coach abuse (34%), an avoidance coping style, in contrast to the
Americans (15%). In addition, more Australians (61%) than Americans
(43%) coped by expressing anger.
Abuse by Players
Giving a technical foul or warning, both behavioral strategies and
reflecting an approach coping style, were the primary responses to
player abuse. Calming or criticizing players was relatively rare,
although Americans used these strategies more so than Australians. Both
groups attributed this stressor to "part of the job" or
ignored the player, reflecting the avoidance coping strategies of
psychological distancing and discounting (Bramson, 1981). Referees
appear resigned to the fact that the emotions that accompany competitive
basketball often result in verbal expressions by players that are either
unintentional or "normal" manifestations of the situation
(Smith, 1982). Given the extent of such expression, Smith contends that
referees often appraise stressful situations as relatively harmless by
selectively ignoring most of these experiences as did 33% of the
American and 26% of the Australian referees.
Arguing With Coaches
Behavioral coping strategics differed between groups for this
stressor. Specifically, Americans preferred to ignore the coach and
remain on task (51% and 39% for U.S. and Australia, respectively),
whereas Australians preferred to discuss the coach's feelings or
answer his or her inquiries (24% and 52% for U.S. and Australia,
respectively). Nevertheless, both groups felt anger or frustration to a
similar degree (44% and 47%, respectively), a reflection of an approach
coping style. As Anshel and Weinberg (1995) found, coach confrontation
is a highly intense source of stress for basketball referees, and
consequently elicits an approach coping style.
Table 1
Percentage and number of Subjects Using Selected Coping Strategies
For Each Stressor(1)
STRESSORS
1. Abuse by Coach (e.g., insults/harassment, disputed calls,
accusations, threats)
Behavioral Coping US (72)(2) AUS
(61)(2)
No.(3) %(4) No.(3) %(4)
express anger 31 43 27 61
tech. foul/warning 37 51 29 47
continue play 34 47 31 36
calmly speak to coach 28 39 8 13
stop game 14 20 17 29
ignore/forget 11 15 21 34
Emotional Coping US (70)(2) AUS
(58)(2)
No.(3) %(4) No.(3) %(4)
criticize coach (self-talk) 44 63 11 19
take a deep breath/calm down 44 63 11 19
review actions/self-doubt 16 23 22 38
discount 12 17 7 12
review actions/self-doubt 16 23 22 38
2. Abuse by Players (e.g., overcalling, 5th foul, player attitude,
swearing)
Behavioral Coping US (75) AUS (62)
tech. foul/warning 56 74 52 84
calm player 11 15 7 11
criticize player behavior 8 11 3 5
ignore/continued play 25 33 16 26
Emotional Coping US (75) AUS (62)
part of job 24 32 14 23
withdraw 5 7 4 6
feel tension/upset/anger 12 16 27 44
review the call 9 12 1 1
3. Arguing With Coaches (e.g., about calls, rules, coache's
behavior)
Behavioral Coping US (75) AUS (62)
answer politely/discuss 18 24 32 52
concentrate on game 25 33 14 23
review actions 9 12 6 10
argue/tech. foul/warning 14 19 3 5
calm coach 6 8 2 3
Emotional Coping US (75) AUS (62)
feel annoyed/frustrated 33 44 29 47
stay clam/deep breaths 5 7 10 16
ignore/move on 38 51 24 39
4. Arguing With Players (e.g., about calls, player behavior, fouls)
Behavioral Coping US (22) AUS (43)
answer politely/give reasons 1 5 12 29
avoid arguing/walk away 8 37 6 14
tech. foul 20 91 18 42
warning 18 82 36 84
argue 13 59 11 26
review actions 4 2 2 4
Emotional Coping US (4) AUS (8)
feel annoyed/upset 17 77 27 63
concentrate 2 11 9 21
keep calm 5 23 15 35
5. Threats of Physical Abuse (e.g., threatened by spectators,
intimidated by coach, meet outside)
Behavioral Coping US (4) AUS (8)
tech. foul/stop game 4 100 2 25
eject person from facility 2 50 6 75
criticize behavior/argued 1 25 2 25
calm person 0 0 4 50
leave area quickly 1 25 1 12
physically challenge person 1 25 3 37
call for security/police 3 75 6 75
express anger/upset 3 75 6 75
Emotional Coping US (4) AUS (8)
keep calm/deep breath 2 50 3 37
fear for safety 2 50 8 100
dislike referee 1 25 4 50
6. Abuse By Spectators (e.g., comments, abuse, bias, overcalls)
Behavioral Coping US (67) AUS (38)
ignore/do not hear 50 74 21 55
laugh 9 13 3 8
concentration on game 59 88 33 87
focus attention elsewhere 19 28 23 60
confront person(s) 2 3 2 5
express anger 15 22 2 5
Emotional Coping US (67) AUS (38)
part of game 48 72 26 68
spectator ignorance 30 45 36 92
feel upset/humiliation 39 58 4 11
7. Working With Partner (e.g., inexperienced/uncooperative, bad
call, positioning, "tough" game)
Behavioral Coping US (5) AUS (15)
support/cover extra ground 3 60 12 80
talk with partner 1 20 2 13
get on with my job 3 60 11 73
scold partner 1 20 2 13
Emotional Coping US (5) AUS (15)
worry/feel embarrassed 1 20 5 33
unhappy 2 40 8 53
empathetic 1 20 8 53
concentrate harder 4 80 12 80
unconcerned/don't care 1 20 2 13
8. Making A Wrong Call (e.g., no foul called, costs points/game,
block vs. charge, possession)
Behavioral Coping US (13) AUS (11)
carry on/"sell" "call" 2 17 4 34
worried/tense/nervous 2 18 3 24
review actions 2 13 2 18
try to forget 1 11 9 8
Emotional Coping US (13) AUS (11)
concentrate harder 10 77 10 91
feel annoyed 1 8 6 55
refs allowed to make mistakes 5 38 2 18
lose concentration/self-blame 0 0 1 9
9. Controversial Call (e.g., hostile arguing by coach or players,
uncertainty)
Behavioral Coping US (61) AUS (49)
"sell" the call 33 54 27 55
continue the game 12 20 19 39
review actions with others 10 17 17 34
confront coach/player 16 26 18 37
Emotional Coping US (61) AUS (49)
concentration affected 13 21 15 31
think about call 5 8 12 24
nervous/hesitant 2 3 2 4
discount argument 6 10 19 39
10. Mistake in Mechanics (e.g., wrong position, incorrect
possession, violation/foul error)
Behavioral Coping US (10) AUS (9)
make correction quickly 9 90 9 100
defend actions 3 30 4 44
get on with the game 10 100 9 100
apologize 1 10 0 0
Emotional Coping US (10) AUS (9)
get it right next time 8 80 3 33
concentrate harder 4 40 3 3
feel embarrassed 1 10 0 0
feel upset with self 5 50 4 44
11. Wrong Location (e.g., "blind", too far away, anticipating play
wrong, fast break)
Behavioral Coping US (4) AUS (8)
sell call 4 100 6 75
made correction 4 100 8 100
consulted partner 1 33 3 37
Emotional Coping US (4) AUS (8)
felt annoyed/upset 4 100 7 87
try harder/get it next time 4 100 8 100
low confidence/bad call 0 0 1 13
12. Presence of Supervisor (e.g., being evaluated, hoping I perform
well, promotion)
Behavioral Coping US (7) AUS (48)
more assertive 3 43 26 54
calls more conservative 2 29 8 17
ignore/performed as usual 4 57 17 35
Emotional Coping US (5) AUS (48)
feel confident 5 100 23 48
feel intimidated 1 20 28 58
relax/have fun 2 40 4 8
13. Presence of Media (e.g., worry correct interpretation,
interviews, unfair criticism or referee)
Behavioral Coping US (3) AUS (3)
ignore presence 3 100 3 100
interview before/after game 1 33 0 0
concentrate on game 2 66 3 100
Emotional Coping US (3) AUS (3)
positive self-talk 3 100 0 0
fell intimidated/concerned 2 66 1 33
14. Experiencing an injury (e.g., experiencing pain, worry about
further injury or ability to continue)
Behavioral Coping US (36) AUS (20)
applied first aid 25 69 8 40
ignored/kept moving 18 50 15 75
removed self from game 4 11 1 5
changed positions 3 8 2 7
with partner
Emotional Coping US (36) AUS (20)
positive self-talk 19 53 13 65
felt upset/angry 31 86 10 50
relaxed 4 11 1 5
found it humorous 0 0 8 40
15. Calling a Technical Foul (e.g., abused by coach or player,
doubting corrections of my actions)
Behavioral Coping US (8) AUS (5)
became assertive 8 100 5 100
explained reason 5 63 2 40
ignored complaints 3 38 4 80
threatened with expulsions 6 75 5 100
continued with game 7 88 5 100
Emotional Coping US (8) AUS (5)
self-confident in call 8 100 5 100
felt anger 4 50 1 20
calmed down/deep breath 0 0 4 80
1 Subjects were invited to indicate more than one coping strategy
or
none at all. This explains more strategies than the number of
subjects responding to the stressor.
2 All subjects in each group that identify this source of stress.
3 Subjects that indicate use of this particular coping technique.
4 Consists of the number of subjects using this technique divided
by
the number of subjects identifying this source.
Arguing With Players
Referees in both groups tended to react assertively to this stressor,
feeling annoyed or upset and giving a warning to the player (approach,
behavioral coping). However, Americans were more than twice as likely to
give a technical foul than Australians (91% and 42%, respectively), and
to argue with the player (59% and 26%, respectively). Conversely, more
Australian referees (35%) than Americans (23%) coped by keeping calm and
by answering politely (29% versus 5%, respectively).
Threats of Physical Abuse
Relatively few referees in our study experienced this stressor.
However, those who did were understandably assertive in their coping
responses. Both groups (75%) called for security or police protection
and felt angry or upset (75%), clearly behavioral, approach coping
categories. This coping response reflects the importance of remaining in
control of such an unpleasant situation. An additional finding was that
all American respondents (100%) stopped the game and/or gave a technical
foul, whereas only 25% of Australians coped in this manner. Instead,
more Australians (75%) than Americans (50%) ejected the person from the
facility. Twice as many Australians (100%) than Americans (50%) feared
for their safety. While this latter finding may appear surprising, given
the markedly higher incidence of violent crime in the U.S. as opposed to
Australia, it is possible that this contrast in societies leads to
different appraisals of experiencing a stressor that threatens a
person's physical well-being. American referees may feel less
intimidated than their Australian counterparts based on their respective
exposure to expressions of such behavior in society. As Smith (1982), an
American wrestling referee, asserts, "rules of sociability and
consideration in society that govern civilized behavior are often
discarded or forgotten during sport competition" (p. 36).
Abuse By Spectators
Most respondents reacted to this stressor by concentrating on the
game and ignoring spectator abuse. Confrontation with spectators was a
very rare source of stress in this study. Most referees used an
emotionally-focused, avoidance, coping strategy called rationalization (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), or discounting (Bramson, 1981), by
feeling that fan abuse is a part of the game. This strategy was likely
facilitated by the referees' perceptions that fans are relatively
ignorant of the facts that underlie the referee's decisions (45%
and 92% for American and Australian referees, respectively). Thus, to
many referees, spectator abuse is an integral, albeit unfortunate
component of competitive sport, and whose comments lack credit.
Working With a Partner
The primary coping strategy with this stressor was providing mutual
support, while meeting other task demands to maintain quality
performance. Problem-focused, approach coping, consisting primarily of
heightened concentration and focused attention on relevant environment
cues, was a common reaction to partner-induced stress. Slightly more
Australians (33%) than Americans (20%) felt worried or embarrassed about
their partner, while more Australians than Americans felt empathy (53%
and 20%, respectively). It was apparent that referees attempted to
maintain high controllability in situations in which their partner was
not performing adequately.
Making a Wrong Call
Responses to this stressor consisted of continuing the game or
"selling" the call, which was done twice as often by
Australians (34%) as Americans (17%). Emotion-focused, approach coping
with this stressor consisted of enhanced concentration. Far more
Australians (55%) than Americans (8%) felt annoyed at making a wrong
call. This cultural difference could be partially explained by the
tendency of American respondents to rationalize their errors, with
approximately twice as many Americans (38%) than Australians (18%)
concluding that "referees are allowed to make mistakes."
Controversial Call
Similar to making a wrong call, both groups coped with
"controversial" calls by "selling the call." This is
a phrase often used among referees meaning an attempt to explain or
justify their call to the complainer (coach or athlete, but not to
spectators). Many more Australians (34%) than Americans (17%) reviewed
their actions with others and thought about the call (24% and 8%,
respectively). Perhaps due to their attempt at self-analysis, more
Australians (31%) than Americans (21%) struggled with their
concentration after the call and, in attempting to remain on task,
discounted others' arguments (39% and 10%, respectively).
Mistake in Mechanics
This was a relatively rare stressor in this study, perhaps not
surprising given the subjects' high referee skills. Both groups
were highly similar in their coping responses, engaging in the
behavioral, approach coping strategy of continuing the game or quickly
making necessary corrections (100% in both groups). However, more U.S.
(80%) than Australian (33%) subjects used the emotion-focused, avoidance
coping strategy of focusing on future performance (e.g., "getting
it right next time").
Wrong Location
This stressor, also relatively rare, was similar to making a mistake
in that the referees in both groups coped by immediately correcting
their actions and focusing on proper subsequent performance (100% for
each group). However, fewer Australians (75%) than Americans (100%) felt
compelled to "sell the call" after experiencing this stressor.
To a lesser extent, partners were consulted to ensure the mistake did
not reoccur (33% and 37% for Americans and Australians, respectively).
Presence of Supervisor
Cultural differences were salient in responding to this stressor.
Specifically, the U.S. sample tended to ignore their supervisor
(behavioral-avoidance coping) more so than their Australian counterparts
(57% and 35%, respectively). Similarly, Americans reacted to the
presence of their supervisor by feeling more confident (100%) and
relaxed (40%) in contrast to Australians (48% and 8%, respectively).
Conversely, Australians were far more intimidated by this stressor (58%)
than Americans were (20%). As indicated earlier, this could be due to
the different evaluation techniques of the respective countries. Both
groups experience both written and observation assessments, however,
Australians rely more on the latter than do Americans for purposes of
promotion and continued employment (J. Martin, Personal Communication,
October 16, 1992).
Presence of Media
This was another relatively rare source of stress for these subjects
(N=3). Both groups (100%) coped by ignoring the media during the
contest, and instead, concentrated on the game (66% and 100%, for U.S.
and Australians, respectively). Positive self-talk was used by all
Americans experiencing this stressor (100%) and by no Australians (0%).
A likely reason for this difference was that more Americans (66%) than
Australians (33%) felt intimidated by the media's presence.
Experiencing an Injury
Behavioral coping entailed immediate application of first aid,
although this was more common for Americans (69%) than Australians
(40%). By contrast, more Australians (75%) than Americans (50%)
preferred to ignore the injury and keep moving. Groups differed markedly
on their emotional coping techniques. Specifically, Americans, using
behavioral-approach coping tendencies, were more upset or angry (86%),
and did not find the stress humorous (0%). Conversely, Australians coped
with an injury using humor (40%), and reacted with less anger than
Americans (50%). Positive self-talk was another common emotional coping
technique for both groups (53% and 65% for U.S. and Australian's
respectively). According to Heil (1993), self-talk in sport is used to
improve confidence, focus attention, or "to stop or modify thoughts
that are detrimental to performance" (p. 152). These aforementioned strategies have been shown in various studies to effectively manage pain
in the non-sport literature (see Jensen, Tumer, Romano, & Karoly,
1991, for a review ).
Calling a Technical Foul
Surprisingly few referees in this study (8 Americans and 5
Australians) indicated use of a coping strategy following this stressor.
This may be because skilled basketball referees actually used calling a
technical foul as a coping strategy itself. Thus, rather than a source
of stress, calling a technical foul may be used as a coping strategy
following acute stressors (e.g., verbal abuse by coaches or players).
This explanation is supported by the finding that referees'
behavioral coping strategies consisted primarily of being assertive
followed by game continuation. In addition, explaining the reason for
the technical was employed more by American (63%) than Australian
respondents (40%). However, more Australians (80%) than Americans (38%)
used the avoidance coping style of ignoring complaints. Emotionally, all
referees (100%) reported confidence in their call, with far more
Australians (80%) than Americans (0%) coping taking deep breaths.
General Discussion
In our review of literature, two hypotheses were put forward.
Specifically, it was predicted that coping strategies (behavioral and
emotional) and coping styles (approach and avoidance) would differ among
different sources of acute stress. It was also hypothesized that the
application of coping would be influenced by cultural differences
between American and Australian referees. The results of the present
investigation generally confirmed both hypotheses.
General Coping Strategies
The importance of coping skills in dealing with acute stressors, as
well as preventing chronic stress and burnout, has been detailed in
previous theoretical and empirical work (e.g., Smith, 1986; Taylor et
al., 1990). In the present investigation, coping skills provided
referees with the necessary resources to withstand the high role demands
placed on them. In this regard, a particularly important finding was
that referees differed in the use of behavioral (problem-focused) and
cognitive (emotion-focused) coping strategies as a function of the
stressor. In addition, coping with the acute stressors in basketball
officiating consisted primarily of approach and avoidance coping styles
(Endler & Parker, 1990; Krohne, 1993; Roth and Cohen, 1986).
Behavioral strategies entailed using the approach coping styles of
covertly criticizing the coach and giving a warning or technical foul.
Cognitive strategies consisted of becoming angry or upset (approach
coping) or taking a deep breath and calming down (avoidance coping).
Abuse By Coach
Past anecdotal literature provides strong evidence that coaches are a
frequent source of stress during the contest. This finding supports the
1984 Miller Brewing Company survey on 229 officials in which disruptive behaviors by coaches was the most common source of irritation (44%) with
injured athletes (16%), disruptive fans (15%), and players arguing with
officials (14%) also mentioned (cited by Burke, 1991b). This is
supported by Holland (1979) who, using heart rate as an indicator of
somatic stress, found that coach-official confrontations were associated
with heart rate reaching 80% to 90% of maximum. In the present
investigation, it was found that officials employed approach coping
styles (e.g., expressing anger, issuing a technical foul) to cope with
this intense stressor of coach abuse.
Smith (1982) proposes that dealing with arguing or abusive coaches
may be a function of the situation regardless of the referee's
reaction. He surmises that "the more fragile and tenuous the social
structure, the less likely open negotiation and the more likely
ritualistic performance" (p. 33). Part of this ritual is actually
derived from the coach rather than from the official. Smith found that
sometimes the coach's behavior "is not meant to be taken
seriously by the referee. Its purpose is to arouse the team and perhaps
the fans...the legitimacy of the call is seldom really in questions.
What the coach wants is an opportunity to yell and then to be told to
sit down or risk being thrown out" (pp. 39-40). This may explain
the referees' frequent use of ignoring and discounting the
coach's comments.
Abuse By Players
The referees in the present study did not find player abuse too
stressful. Respondents of both groups gave a warning or technical foul
(approach coping), or conversely, ignored the player's comments
(avoidance coping). The extent to which players cause stress may be a
function of the official's skill level. Specifically, the
referee's appraisal of the stressful situation likely differs
between experienced and inexperienced, and skilled and unskilled
referees (Kaissidis & Anshel, 1993). Novices may encounter
situations that are entirely new to them, such as dealing with an angry
player or having to endure abuse from upset coaches or spectators. For
example, when Smith (1982, p. 36) first began his career as a wrestling
referee, he reported a willingness "to accept (abusive treatment)
if l had made a decision which they thought was wrong ... (but) was not
... ready for the abusive form in which such criticism was
expressed." He was also surprised that coaches, the most abusive
sources of stress which he experienced, would often interact with him
outside the sport context" as if nothing had happened. It appears,
then, that the referee's and coach's respective appraisals of
a stressful encounter during the contest may differ, thereby influencing
the use of coping strategies.
Arguing With Coaches
The referees' coping strategies differed between this stressor
and the stressor "Abuse by Coach". Whereas referees tended to
give a technical foul or warning in response to abusive actions, the
officials were more likely to respond less aggressively to coach
inquiries or concerns (also see Burke, 1991a). Cebulski (1987),
reporting on the frequent use of this coping strategy, contends that
"If a coach says something obscene, we're supposed to say
something like, 'What did you say?' or "Are you talking
to me?' Most of the time, the coach will say, 'I wasn't
talking to you' "(p. 33). This technique allows the coach to
rescind an emotionally-initiated, often regretted, remark without
further incident. At the same time, the referee does not lose
credibility and respect.
Arguing With Players
Whereas abusive players tended to receive a technical foul or
warning, referees treated arguing with players less aggressively. For
example, issuing a warning (approach, behavioral coping) was far more
common, as was ignoring the player (avoidance, behavioral coping). Not
surprisingly, however, American referees were more than twice as likely
than Australians to give a technical foul for arguing. The players'
request for information or clarification carries far more respect and
credibility by the officials than the players' use of profanity and
other berating remarks. Similar to other stressors, it appears that the
official's appraisal of the player's actions explains the
relatively low intensity of this stressor.
Threats of Physical Abuse
It was apparent that this stressor was of highly unpleasant,
finishing first among Australians. Results indicated that both stopping
the game and giving a technical foul formed the primary behavioral
coping strategies. These reactions are compatible with the anecdotal
literature. For example, Tapp (1987) contends that the best way to
combat stress is to "be fearless." In particular, he suggests
that being impervious to the emotions of others, not losing control, and
remaining self-confident under adverse conditions are key elements for
success sports officiating. Stoddart (1986) suggests that Australian
sports fans are usually somewhat less restrained than spectators in many
other countries. This may explain why Australian referees more likely
feared for their safety than their American counterparts, and moved
quickly to obtain assistance. Still, behavioral, approach coping was the
consistent strategy applied by both groups.
Abuse By Spectators
Understandably, by far the most common coping response to spectator
abuse was to ignore the spectators or perceive their behavior as
irrelevant. However, Anshel and Weinberg (1995) found that relatively
few Australian referees were upset about these experiences (ranked 14th)
in contrast to their American counterparts (ranked 3rd). This result may
at least partially explain the manner in which each group copes with
spectator's remarks. Specifically, Australians perceive spectators
as relatively uninformed about the game (e.g., "abusive fans are
ignorant"), in general, and about the situation, in particular
(Stoddart, 1986). Consequently, because Australian referees generally do
not perceive spectators as capable of making proper judgments about the
official's decisions, they are more likely to use avoidance coping
techniques, such as discounting, ignoring, and psychological distancing
(Bramson, 1981). Almost three times as many American referees became
upset with or humiliated by abusive fans. Thus, perhaps one may surmise
that perceived stress among basketball referees is at least partly a
function of perceived audience characteristics. This conclusion is
supported by Purdy and Snyder (1986) who found that 56% of 689 U.S. high
school basketball officials perceived their role as unpopular among
spectators, "that they can never be completely correct in their
decisions...and that they are often blamed for a team's
defeat" (pp. 61-69).
Working With A Partner
Results indicated that referees coped with partner-related stress by
continuing to do their own job while simultaneously supporting or
covering for their partner. The sense of altruism among officials is not
forced, since establishing a trusting relationship with your partner is
an integral part of game preparation (Mano, 1980). Before the contest,
he suggests that game officials establish a personal relationship and
exchange general (e.g., informally revealing interests and previous
experiences) and game-related information (e.g., coverage, roles, team
tendencies). In addition, he strongly supports offering each other
praise and assistance during the contest.
Making a Wrong Call
According to Strom (1990), a former professional basketball referee
in the National Basketball League in the United States, officials who
perceive that they made an error should change the call. Strom feels
that "If you (make a wrong call), you just have to swallow it, or
change it if you discover your mistake right away...you'll get a
lot more respect if you admit you blew one than if you try to even
things out by (purposely making a wrong penalty call against the other
team)" (p. 130). However, in a 1984 survey (see Burke, 1991b), 40%
of 229 officials indicated they "almost never" condone compensating a team by "making an intentional offsetting
judgment" (p. 7). Strom's views are not supported by the
results of this study. Specifically, no respondent indicated that he
changed the call, and instead, either rationalized or justified (i.e.,
"selling") the call to the coach or player, or continued with
the game. Thus, either Strom's view is not widely held by others,
or there may be a discrepancy between the referees' beliefs and
their actual coping strategies during the contest.
Controversial Call
Calling penalty in response to an infraction may not always be the
appropriate call in every situation. For example, in response to a loose
ball, Strom might "call someone for a foul, then realize it really
isn't the best call because of other things that happened
simultaneously" (p. 130). Instead, the foul will be dismissed, and
instead, call for a "jump ball." This view is supported by a
study of 689 high school basketball referees by Snyder and Purdy (1987).
The authors' focus was determining the most important areas of
competence in basketball officiating as perceived by the officials
themselves, with particular attention to the application of
"unwritten" rules and norms. The first area of competence was
the officials' control of the game (social control) (e.g., when
someone commits an indisputable violation). The second area was
supplementation of rules (i.e., the officials' subjective
decision-making which, although possibly violating the rules themselves,
are in the best interests of the game. One-third of their sample
regularly overlooked infractions depending on the game situation. Thus,
"unwritten rules," where appropriate, helps officials cope
with various stressful situations.
Snyder and Purdy also found that 73% of basketball referees viewed
consistency as and the best indicant of skilled referee performance -
even more important than following the rules to the letter. According to
Weinberg and Richardson (1990), consistency is the ability to make the
same decisions "in identical or similar circumstances," and
"apply the rules equally to both opponents" (p. 6). They
indicate that consistency, among other characteristics (e.g., rapport,
decisiveness, poise, integrity, judgment, and confidence), is a relevant
feature of top sports officials.
Mistake in Mechanics
Anshel and Weinberg (1995) found that making mechanical errors was a
stress source of relative low intensity. Thus, it is not surprising that
avoidance coping strategies (e.g., making corrections quickly and
getting on with the game) were implemented by virtually 100% of all
respondents who experienced this stressor. After initially feeling
upset, most referees coped emotionally by "promising to get it
right next time." These findings support Krohne and Hindel (1988)
who contend that "successful (participants) are characterized by
few interfering ... reactions..., little vigilant (approach) coping, and
an extended use of cognitively avoidant self-regulatory techniques"
(p. 225). In a rapidly performed sport environment such as basketball, a
mistake in mechanics must be quickly put in the past to minimize
subsequent attentional demands, coping strategies inherent in
Anshel's (1990) COPE model.
Wrong Location
Coping with this stressor also followed Anshel's (1990) COPE
model. In sequence, the referees first perceived the problem, appraised
it in need of immediate attention (often manifested by annoyance or
feeling upset), immediately corrected their actions, and used
self-reinforcing statements such as "get it right next time,"
a strategy called positive reinterpretation, or reappraisal (Lazarus
& Folkman, 1984), and planning (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub,
1989).
Presence of Supervisor
The coping reactions to this stressor were clearly a function of the
group's reliance on observational assessments by their supervisor
for examining competence. As indicated earlier, although both groups
combined written examinations with personal observations for performance
evaluation, the Australian system is far more dependent on supervisor
presence then the U.S. system (J. Martin, Personal Communication,
October 16, 1992). Their respective coping strategies reflected these
differences, with more Australians than Americans feeling intimidated
and increasingly assertive when the supervisor was present. In addition,
far more Americans than Australians reported feeling relaxed and having
fun during this period. Although unexplored in this study, it is
possible that coping strategies reflected differences in the
groups' appraisals of their respective supervisors, especially if
the supervisor's presence was appraised as challenging rather than
threatening (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). For example, Bouffard, and
Crocker (1992), in their study of disabled athletes, found that
"perceived challenge was characterized by high levels of
problem-focused coping...and positive affective states" (p. 415).
The authors contend that behavioral coping strategies help individuals
manage events that may be perceived as personally beneficial or valuable
and allow that person to actively manage the situation.
Presence of Media
This area was a virtually nonexistence source of stress. Hence,
coping by ignoring the media and concentrating on the game is
understandable. One likely explanation of this reaction comes may be
derived from a 1984 Miller Brewing Company survey, discussed earlier,
indicating that 50% of American officials do not "think most print
and broadcast media understand the roles of officials and
referees." This reaction reflects the use of a benign appraisal (in
contrast to stress or challenge appraisals), and reflects a discounting
(avoidance) coping strategy (Bramson, 1981; Lazarus & Folkman,
1984). This suggests the significant role of appraisal in the coping
process, and strong link to the subsequent use of coping strategies and
(approach and avoidance) coping styles.
Experiencing an Injury
Researchers have found a significant link between stress and injury
for athletes low in both coping skills and social support (Smith et al.,
1990). Their results indicated that highly stressed athletes with poor
coping skills and low social support are less able to participate in
sport for more days than individuals with relatively better coping
skills and higher social support. Although research in this area with
referees is lacking, our data did not indicate evidence for social
support as a coping strategy. Perhaps, unlike the longer time period for
recovery from injury, experiencing an injury during the game may
preclude the importance, or practicality, of social support strategies
among officials. Instead, the subjects in this study tended to use
"on-task" coping strategies. These included external focusing
(maintaining on-task performance, avoidance coping) and anger, a
cognitive (self-talk) strategy to overcome discomfort by psyching up.
Calling a Technical Foul
In a study of 110 Division I American basketball officials, Brennan
(reported by Smith, 1988) found that "officials knew when coaches
were trying to get technical fouls or were 'working'
officials...75 percent of the officials viewed the verbal communication
as a 'natural course' of the game" (p. 68). This may
partially explain why many subjects in this study reported using
emotion-focused coping in feeling more self-confidence in their call,
coupled with relatively low anger. Behavioral coping consisted of being
assertive, threatening the coach or player with expulsion, and quickly
continuing the game after the call. It is likely that referees feel
relatively little stress after calling a technical foul because of their
perception that the coach or player deserves. Based on other data in
this study, is also apparent that calling a technical foul is itself a
behavioral-approach coping technique in response to other stressors.
Cross-Cultural-Comparisons
Comparisons between American and Australian basketball referees on
the use of coping strategies indicated more similarities than
differences. This general finding is similar to Weinberg, Grove, and
Jackson (1992) who reported far more similarities than differences
between Australian and American tennis coaches' in their use of
different strategies to build self-effacacy among their athletes.
Group differences in the present study were apparent for the
self-reported use of behavioral and emotional coping strategies. For
instance, in response to an abusive coach, both groups were similarly
punitive. However, Americans were more likely to speak calmly to the
coach than were Australians. There is an absence of empirical research explaining these differences.
Cultural differences of the referees' responses to verbal abuse
from players was contrary to their responses to verbal abuse from
coaches. The primary coping strategy of both groups was to give a
technical foul. Further, similar to the American referees' response
to abusive coaches, Australian officials perceived player comments as
"part of the job," and consequently, "moved on to the
next play" or tried to reason with them. Officials in both groups
had relatively little tolerance for argumentive player comments.
However, Americans were more likely to give a technical foul (91%) to
give a technical foul (91%) than Australians (42%), while both groups
tended to warn the players (84% and 82% for U.S. and Australia,
respectively).
Thus, Duda and Allison's (1990) call for recognizing cultural
variation in style and meaning of participating in sport and physical
activity was still supported by our results. Culture influences
perceptions and affective responses. This was noted in the present
investigation in terms of the referees' appraisals of acute stress
during the contest. At least some of the differences between American
and Australian referees may reflect cultural background and experiences
which, in turn, have direct implications for the content in effective
stress management training programs in different societies.
Conclusions
The present findings indicate that selected coping strategies used by
both groups, as categorized by problem (behavior)-focused and emotion
(cognitive)-focused (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), and approach and
avoidance coping styles (Krohne, 1989, 1993; Roth & Cohen, 1986),
were identified as a function of two factors, the type of stressor and
cultural differences. Similar to Gould et al. (1993), in their study
of-U.S. Olympic wrestlers, it appears that an array of behavioral and
cognitive strategies are employed by referees in response to situational
demands. The researchers concluded that "the process of coping is a
dynamic complex process involving any number of strategies, often in
combination" (p. 91). This certainly applies to the current sample
of referees.
Many of the coping strategies for basketball referees appear to lean
more toward behavioral-focused than emotion-focused categories. The
official's position mandates observed control of the environment
and change in the behaviors of selected athletes. Whether coping
strategies are selected as a function of the individual's personal
coping style, as proposed by some researchers (Endler & Parker,
1990; Krohne, 1993; Roth & Cohen, 1986), is situationally dependent
(Gould et al., 1993), or is a combination of both personal and
situational factors, reflecting the transactional coping model (Terry,
1991), awaits additional research. However, results of the present study
suggest the latter. Finally, although selected differences between U.S.
and Australian coping responses were found, our data did not indicate
consistent cross-cultural differences in the preferred use any
particular coping style (i.e., approach or avoidance, behavior and
emotion-focused). Rather, it appears that effective basketball
officiating consists of appropriate coping techniques in order to
minimize stress and to optimize concentration and performance. This
conclusion is consistent with a relatively early study of the
psychological and behavioral tendencies of effective basketball
officials (Alker, Straub, & Leary, 1973). As Jones and Hardy (1989)
note, successful coping in sport requires regaining composure,
establishing the proper mental set (i.e., the psychological readiness to
respond to subsequent stimuli), and maintaining optimal arousal and
concentration.
Along these lines, Burke and Miller (1990) argue that almost all of
the training involved for sports officiating is restricted to the
technical aspect of their performance with relatively little
psychological training typically available in coping with stress. Given
the extent to which sports officials are exposed to acute stress, and
the potential deleterious effects of stress on the performance and
retention of officials in sport, it is essential for researchers to
study the coping process in sports officiating and to test the
effectiveness of stress management programs.
References
Alleer, H.A., Straub, W.F., & Leary, J. (1973). Achieving
consistency: A study of basketball officiating. Journal of Vocational
Behavior, 3, 335-343.
Anshel, M.H. (1990). Toward validation of a model for coping with
acute stress in sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 21,
58-83.
Anshel, M.H., & Weinberg, R.S. (1995). Sources of acute stress in
American and Australian basketball referees. Journal of Applied Sport
Psychology, 7, 11-22.
Bouffard, M., & Crocker, P. (1992). Coping by individuals with
physical disabilities with perceived challenge in physical activity: Are
people consistent? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 63,
410-417.
Bramson, R.M. (1981). Coping with difficult people. New York:
Doubleday.
Burke, K. (September-October, 1991a). Dealing with sport officials.
Soccer Journal, 36, pp. 45-48.
Burke, K.L. (May-June, 1991b). Dealing with sport officials. Sport
Psychology Training Bulletin, 2, 1-6.
Burke, K., & Miller, M. (September, 1990). Sports officials: The
neglected participants. Paper presented at the Association for the
Advancement of Applied Sport Psychology annual conference, San Antonio,
Texas.
Carver, C.S, Scheier, M.F., & Weintraub, J.K. (1989). Assessing
coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology. 56, 267-283.
Cebulski, M. (January, 1987). A panel of experts offers guidance on
how to keep your head when those about you are losing theirs. Referee,
12, pp. 29-31.
Dale, J., & Weinberg, R. (1990). Burnout in sport: A review and
critique. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 2, 67-83.
Duda, J.L., & Allison, J.T. (1990). Cross-cultural analysis in
exercise and sport psychology: Avoid in the field. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 12, 114-131.
Endler, N. S., & Parker, J. D. A. (1990). Multidimensional assessment of coping: A critical evaluation. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 58, 844-854.
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R.S. (1985). If it changes it must be a
process: Study of emotion and coping during three states of a college
examination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 150-170.
Gauron, E.F. (1986). The art of cognitive self-regulation. Clinics in
Sports Medicine, 5, 95-101.
Gould, D., Eklund, R.C., & Jackson, S.A. (1993). Coping
strategies used by U.S. Olympic wrestlers. Research Quarterly For
Exercise and Sport, 64, 83-93.
Heil, J. (1993). Mental training in injury management. In J. Heil
(Ed.), Psychology of sport injury (pp. 151-174). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Holland, J.C. (1.979). Heart rate responses of high school basketball
officials. The Physician and Sportsmedicine, 7, 78-87.
Jensen, P.J, Turner, J.A, Romano, J.M., & Karoly, P. (1991).
Coping with chronic pain: A critical review of the literature. Pain, 47.
249-283.
Jones, J.G., & Hardy, L. (1989). Stress and cognitive functioning in sport. Journal of Sport Science. 7, 41-60.
Kaissidis, A., & Anshel, M.H. (1993). Sources of and responses to
acute stress between adult and adolescent Australian basketball
referees. Australian Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 26,
22-32.
Krohne, H.W. (1993). Vigilance and cognitive avoidance as concepts in
coping research. In H.W. Krohne (Ed.), Attention and avoidance (pp.
51-69). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.
Krohne, H. W. (1989). The concept of coping modes: Relating cognitive
person variables to actual coping behavior. Special Issue: The role of
individual differences in stress and stress management. Advances in
Behavior Research and Therapy, 11, 235-248.
Krohne, H. W., & Hindel, C. (1988). Trait anxiety, state anxiety,
and coping behavior as predictors of athletic performance. Anxiety
Research, 1, 225-234.
Lazarus, R.S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and
coping New York: Springer.
Lehman, D.R., & Reifman, A. (1988). Spectator influence on
basketball officiating. Journal of Social Psychology, 127, 673-675.
Mano, B. (October, 1980). The art of being an official. Referee, 5,
14-15.
Quain, R.J., & Purdy, D.A. (1988). Sport officials: Who are these
people? Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 3, 60-74.
Patton, M.Q. (1990). Qualitative evaluation and research methods (2nd
ed.). Newberry Park, CA: Sage.
Purdy, D.A., & Snyder, E.E. (1986). A social profile of high
school basketball officials. Journal of Sport Behavior, 8, 54-65.
Rotella, R., McGuire, R., & Gansneder, B. (1985). Stress and
basketball officials: Impact on health, performance, and retention.
Unpublished manuscript, University of Virginia, Charlottsville.
Roth, S, & Cohen, L. J. (1986). Approach, avoidance, and coping
with stress. American Psychologist, 41, 813-819.
Scanlan, T.K., Stein, G.L., & Ravizza, K. (1989). An in-depth
study of former elite figure skaters: II. Sources of enjoyment. Journal
of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11, 65-83.
Smith, A. (1988, April). Special report: The Brennan study. Referee,
13, 68.
Smith, C. W. (1982). Performance and negotiations: A case study of a
wrestling referee. Qualitative Sociology, 5, 33-46.
Smith, R. E. (1986). Toward a cognitive-affective model of athletic
burnout. Journal of Sport Psychology, 8, 36-50.
Smith, R. E., & Nye, S. L. (1989). Comparison of induced affect
and covert rehearsal in the acquisition of stress management coping
skills. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 17-23.
Smith, R.E., Smoll, F.L., & Ptacek, J.T. (1990). Conjunctive moderator variables in vulnerability and resiliency research: Life
stress, social support and coping skills, and adolescent sport injuries.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 360-370.
Snyder, E.E., & Purdy, D.A. (1987). Social control in sport: An
analysis of basketball officiating. Sociology of Sport Journal, 4,
394-402.
Stoddart, B. (1986). Political football: The politics of Australian
sport. In B. Jones (Ed.), Saturday afternoon fever (pp. 56-82). Sydney,
Australia: Angus & Robertson.
Strom, E. (November 1990). How to call 'em like a pro. Sports
Illustrated, 7, 125-196; 129-132; 135.
Tapp, J. (May, 1987). The only thing you have to fear is fear itself:
Common fears and what officials have done to cope with them. Referee,
12, pp. 28; 30-32; 34.
Taylor, A.H., Daniel, J.V., Leith, L., & Burke, R.J. (1990).
Perceived stress, psychological burnout and paths to turnover intentions
among sport officials. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 2, 84-97.
Terry, D. J. (1991). Coping resources and situational appraisals as
predictors of coping behavior. Journal of Personality and Individual
Differences, 12, 1031-1047.
Weinberg, R.S., Grove, R., & Jackson, A. (1992). Strategies for
building self-efficacy in tennis players: A comparative analysis of
Australia and American coaches. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 3-13.
Weinberg, R.S., & Richardson, (1990). Psychology of officiating.
Champaign, IL: Leisure.
Zoller, S. (1985, May). Learning how to live with stress. Referee,
10, 48-49, 51.