An investigation of the different motivations of marathon runners with varying degrees of experience.
Masters, Kevin S. ; Ogles, Benjamin M.
Many different theoretical perspectives acknowledge that the factors
which motivate someone to initiate an activity are often different from
the factors which motivate the individual to maintain or continue to
engage in the behavior. Researchers in the area of exercise have been
especially receptive to this position, in part because of the high drop
out rate among those who begin an exercise program. Martin and Dubbert
(1982) noted that between 50 and 70% of individuals who begin an
exercise program drop out within 12 to 24 months. Dishman (1982, 1986)
has similarly noted that about 40 to 50% drop out within the first 6
months. Lee and Owen (1986) have examined this problem from a number of
perspectives, including the cognitive-behavioral and social
psychological. They noted that behavior change is a process which occurs
in a series of stages. Consequently, from a behavioral view, early
reinforcement contingencies that influence behavior change may lose
their effect as the individual moves from the acquisition to maintenance
stage.
Along these lines, Shepherd (1985) has proposed that exercise
programs be designed to initially maximize external reinforcement until
the preliminary discomfort experienced by exercising individuals has
subsided. Subsequently these exercisers will become motivated by
internal rewards that are inherent in the exercise itself. Both
Kasimatis, Langston and Clark (1992) and Sonstroem (1988) have reported
other process models that rely more heavily on cognitive variables to
explain exercise initiation and maintenance. What these approaches have
in common is the position that the motivation for exercising changes
throughout the individual's particular exercise history.
While theories that address the general topic of exercise have merit,
Crandall (1980) has recommended that investigators concentrate their
efforts on a particular activity since the motivating factors that are
important in one activity may or may not be important in another.
Individuals who engage in marathon running and weightlifting will, for
example, differ in their reasons for participating and maintaining these
activities. Thus, it seems prudent to follow Crandall's (1980)
advice and investigate the reasons for initiating and maintaining
behaviors within specific domains.
Previous investigators have explored the development of motives for
participation with runners using retrospective self-report methods. For
example, Carmack and Martens (1979) studied 250 non-marathon runners who
were recruited at various locations and events throughout Illinois and
Indiana. The subjects completed questionnaires that inquired about their
reasons for running, outcomes of running, commitment to running,
training practices, and demographics. The investigators found that
serious runners tended to de-emphasize physical health as a reason for
running, and instead, placed increasing emphasis on psychological
reasons.
In another self-report, retrospective study Johnsgard (1985) asked a
large sample of readers of a running magazine, and a second sample of
runners over age 50 years why they began running and why they continue
to run. He found that both groups indicated a shift toward psychological
factors and away from fitness and weight control, although fitness
remained the top ranked reason. Okwumabua, Meyers, and Santille (1987)
recruited master runners (persons 40 years of age or older) from five 10
kilometer road races in the southern United States and from two 10
kilometer races in the southwest. They distributed a three part
questionnaire and achieved a response rate of 42%. The instrument
assessed demographic information and psychological aspects of running
including cognitive strategies and reasons for beginning and continuing
to run. They observed the now familiar shift from physiological to
psychological reasons for running among the master runners in this
sample.
Relatively few studies have addressed the motivation issue among
marathon runners. Summers, Sargent, Levey and Murray (1982) examined
motivational change with first time marathon runners participating in
the Melbourne Marathon. These investigators sent a four part
questionnaire by mail to 500 participants. They received 363 usable
responses. Runners were asked about demographic information, training
practices, attitudes toward running, reasons for attempting a second
marathon, and cognitive strategies. Important for purposes of the
present study is that they asked these subjects, after recently
completing their first marathon, to list their reasons for attempting a
second. By far the most significant reason was to run a faster time.
Barrell, Chamberlain, Evans, Holt and Mackean (1989) conducted
in-depth interviews with 24 marathon runners and 17 of their spouses.
They noted that becoming a committed marathon runner was a long process,
one that was often slow and difficult. Important to this process was the
evolution of motivation among the runners. For example, they noted that
most of the runners cited keeping fit or staying healthy as reasons for
their initial involvement in the sport. While these motives remained
important among the highly committed runners, other factors such as
achieving a sense of freedom, being challenged, winning, or attaining
relaxation were also important sources of motivation for them. Both of
the studies conducted with marathon runners have provided meaningful
information, however, neither study included instruments with acceptable
psychometric properties.
Since individuals often fail to adhere to even the most rudimentary exercise programs, marathon runners, in contrast to less serious
exercisers, must endure extremely challenging circumstances during both
training and competition, and thus provide a compelling model of
exercise motivation and adherence. The purpose of this study was to
document the motivation of marathon runners who varied in their marathon
participation experience by utilizing an instrument with demonstrated
psychometric properties rather than open-ended questions or untested
measures. It was hypothesized that veteran marathon runners, more than
their less experienced peers would derive their motivation to exercise
more from psychological factors such as self-esteem, and coping with
stress, and competition. Further, runners participating in their second
or third marathon would be motivated primarily by the desire for
personal achievement, i.e., to perform better. In keeping with the
distinction between initiation and maintenance, it was hypothesized that
first time marathon runners would have different motives than those who
were more experienced. For the rookies it was anticipated that physical
and health reasons would be more important than psychological or
emotional factors, but their desire to achieve would be the most
important factor.
Subjects were 472 marathon runners (80% male) who participated in one
of three Midwestern marathons. They were divided into three groups based
on their marathon experience: rookies (first marathon), mid-level
(second or third marathon), and veterans (more than three marathons).
The rookies (N = 95) ranged in age from 16 to 60 years (M = 32.9; SD =
8.7) and trained an average of 44.85 miles per week (SD = 20.49). The
mid-level marathon runners ranged in age from 18 to 61 years (M = 35.7;
SD = 8.63) and trained an average of 45.3 miles per week (SD = 13.59).
Their previous best marathon finish times ranged from 2:54:00 to 5:36:59
with a median of 3:48:00. Finally, the veteran marathon runners had an
age range of 20 to 63 years (M = 40.47; SD = 8.57) and trained an
average of 51.77 miles per week (SD = 21.7). Their previous best
marathon finish times ranged from 2:28:12 to 5:31:58 with a median of
3:27:00.
Procedure
Subjects were recruited during race registration the day prior to the
marathon. They were asked to complete a demographic sheet containing
training, performance, and experience variables, along with the
Motivations of Marathoners Scales (MOMS; Masters, Ogles, & Jolton,
1993). The instruments were returned in a postage paid envelope. A
response rate of 47% was obtained.
Motivations of Marathoners Scales (MOMS)
The MOMS was designed, based on research literature, to
comprehensively assess reasons for marathon running (Masters, Ogles,
& Jolton, 1993). It consists of 56 individual items that are
organized into nine separate scales (listed in Table 1). The scales were
further classified as belonging to one of the following general
categories of motives: psychological (Self-esteem, Psychological Coping,
Life Meaning), physical (Health Orientation, Weight Concern), social
(Affiliation, Social Recognition), or achievement (Competition, Personal
Goal Achievement). Subjects are asked to rate each item ranging from 1
("not a reason") to 7 ("a very important reason")
for training and running a marathon. Item scores are summed to produce
scale scores.
Satisfactory psychometric data has been collected on the MOMS
(Masters, Ogles, & Jolton, 1993). Alpha coefficients ranged from .80
to .93 demonstrating the internal consistency of the scales. Retest reliability, assessed by intraclass correlations over 3-4 months, was
also acceptable (range from R = .71 to R= .90). Finally, evidence for
the construct, factorial, and discriminant validity of the scales has
been presented.
Results
A one-way multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with
"experience"' as the independent variable and MOMS scores
as the dependent variables was statistically significant, Wilks'
Lambda = .90, F(18, 930) = 2.91, p [less than] .001. To further
understand how these groups differed we examined the discriminant
functions. The first function had an eigenvalue of .08, a canonical
correlation of .27, and accounted for 65.8% of the variance. This
function was significant; Wilks' Lambda = .89, [[Chi].sup.2] (18,
N=472) = 52.52, p [less than] .001. The second function had an
eigenvalue of .04, a canonical correlation of .20, and accounted for
34.2% of the variance. It was also significant; Wilks' Lambda =
.96, [[Chi].sup.2] (8, N=472) = 18.17, p [less than] .05.
Inspection of the structure coefficients (Table 1) suggested that the
first function reflected an affiliative dimension that was strongly
oriented toward competition and recognition. Health orientation was also
demonstrated to be important on this function. It was not characterized
by an internal desire to perform better. The function was named
"Marathon Identity". The second function reflected an
intra-individual focus characterized by psychological effects of running
and personal performance variables. This dimension was personal and not
oriented toward other runners. It was named "Internal Focus."
Figure 1 depicts group centroids plotted as a function of experience
level. As can be seen, the veterans scored significantly higher on
Marathon Identity than did the mid-level or rookies. On Internal Focus
it was the mid-level group scoring highest followed by the veterans with
rookies last. Table 2 provides per item means and standard deviations for all groups on the MOMS.
Table 1
Discriminant Function Analysis
Structure Coefficients
MOMS Scale Function 1 Function 2
Competition .64 .41
Affiliation .59 .05
Social Recognition .41 -.25
Health Orientation .41 .13
Weight Concern .31 -.18
Self-esteem .20 .12
Personal Goal Achievement .03 .51
Psychological Coping .16 .50
Life Meaning .28 .42
Table 2
Per item means and standard deviation on MOMS for each group
Rookies Mid Exp. Veteran
MOMS Scale M SD M SD M SD
Competition 2.14 1.27 2.63 1.43 2.89 1.30
Affiliation 2.42 1.30 2.58 1.38 2.95 1.45
Social Recognition 2.95 1.51 2.82 1.41 3.24 1.56
Health Orientation 4.64 1.52 4.83 1.31 5.04 1.34
Weight Concern 4.01 1.81 3.91 1.74 4.27 1.70
Self-esteem 4.58 1.44 4.71 1.26 4.79 1.36
Personal Goal
Achievement 4.42 1.30 4.81 1.27 4.62 1.32
Psychological Coping 3.11 1.53 3.57 1.44 3.46 1.48
Life Meaning 2.90 1.56 3.31 1.39 3.32 1.51
Discussion
The results suggest that motivations characteristic of marathon
runners differ as a function of their experience. The most experienced
veterans group adopted a social identity as a marathon runner that
included both competitive and health aspects. Contrary to predictions,
they were motivated more by social and competitive reinforcements than
by personal accomplishment or internal psychological rejuvenation. As
expected, the mid-level experience group was primarily motivated by
personal performance enhancement and psychological rewards, whereas
marathon identity played a secondary role. They may have realized social
benefits of marathon running but had not entirely adopted a marathon
identity. The first time rookie runners were not characterized by either
function. That is, they appeared less motivated by a marathon identity
and, since they had not yet realized marathon goal accomplishment, they
were apparently less concerned with performance improvement.
Veteran Marathon Runners
The veteran marathon runners were primarily motivated by variables
concerned with social identity, recognition, affiliation, health, and
competition, termed the "marathon identity". These individuals
constitute a unique subset of experienced runners who are distinguished
by their repeated participation in marathons and a unique set of motives
for running. Since previous research has suggested that exercise
motivation moves from extrinsic to intrinsic (Shepherd, 1985), and from
physical to psychological motivation for runners (Carmack & Martens,
1979; Johnsgard, 1985; Okwumabua, Meyers & Santille, 1987), we were
surprised by the external/social characteristics that motivated the
veteran marathon runners.
In trying to understand the motivations of marathon runners, it seems
that those who continue with the event focus on it as an important part
of their personal and social identity and they desire recognition from
others based on their marathon running. This finding is understandable
when the time and effort involved in continued marathon participation
are considered. Perseverance in a venture that requires this much
involvement will certainly be enhanced by, and may even require, social
reinforcement. Barrell et al. (1989) provide a detailed discussion on
how marathon runners must work to make available the time necessary for
training. They report that for many of these runners the cooperation of
a number of individuals, including family and friends, is often
necessary for them to be able to continue running marathons. In many
cities of the U.S. there is a running culture where many of the same
participants are involved in numerous events and get to know each other,
sometimes even belonging to the same road running or track club. With
this in mind, it was interesting to note that when we asked the runners
to list how many marathon runners they knew personally we found that the
veterans reported knowing an average of 19.52 as compared to 9.6 for the
mid-level runners and 5.0 for the rookies. So it seems that those
individuals who run many marathons depend to some extent on motivation
generated from others in their social network who recognize them as
marathon runners and support them in these efforts. Fellow marathon
runners may also provide a crucial amount of affiliative or social
support that also helps to sustain the activity.
The veterans were also competitive. Previous investigators (Barrell
et al.; 1989; Masters & Lambert, 1989) have found that competitive
motivation and marathon finish times are negatively correlated. That is,
this suggests that those who are more skilled will also be more likely
to persevere. In fact, within our sample the more experienced runners
ran significantly faster than the other groups (p [less than] .001).
Mid-level Marathon Runners
The findings of this study largely agree with those of previous
investigators when considering the motivation of the mid-level marathon
runners. Both Johnsgard (1985) and Okwumabua, et al. (1987) found that
more experienced runners were more motivated by psychological reasons
than by physical benefits. Summers, et al. (1982), studying runners
after they completed their first marathon, documented that reasons for
running a second marathon were largely oriented around performance
enhancement as well as psychological variables. These results agree with
our findings pertaining to the mid-level marathoners. It was this group
that was most motivated by a desire to perform better than they had
previously and who derived the most psychological benefit from marathon
running. Their focus was internal, centering on psychological
beneficence and intrapersonal performance enhancement.
Rookie Marathon Runners
The discriminant analysis provided little information regarding the
reasons why subjects attempt their first marathon since the rookies
scored low on both discriminant functions. To acquire preliminary data
on this question we examined the per item means on each scale (see Table
2). This suggested that health and weight concerns, self-esteem, and
personal goal achievement may all be relevant to the motivational
experience of first time marathon runners. These findings, particularly
those pertaining to health, weight, and personal goal achievement, are
what we expected based on previous research (Carmack & Martens,
1979; Clough, Shepherd, & Maughan, 1989; Johnsgard, 1985, 1989;
Summers, et al., 1982; Summers, Machin, & Sargent, 1983) which has
consistently identified these factors as being important for the
initiation of exercise and marathon running. The importance of
self-esteem may be unique to first time marathon runners and may be a
distinguishing feature of their motivation as compared to other
exercisers or runners of shorter distances.
Implications and Limitations
The pattern of results across the three groups indicates differences
in the motivations of more and less experienced marathon runners. This
suggests the possibility of a change in motivation from a personal
performance, psychologically-based and internally focused stage to an
externally focused, competitive and socially-oriented "marathon
identity" phase. However, due to the cross-sectional design of this
study, developmental sequencing is only one of several hypotheses.
Another possible interpretation is that there is a subset of marathon
participants who begin running for competitive and social reasons and
continue to run for these same reasons while becoming veteran runners.
Longitudinal studies could provide the critical test for these rival
interpretations as with the present design it is not possible to know
which rookie runners will continue and become mid-level or veteran
runners and which mid-level runners will become veterans.
Similar to Crandall's (1980) work, it is questionable how far
the results could be generalized beyond marathon runners to other
runners or exercisers. Particularly among the veteran marathon runners
we found results that are not entirely consistent with expectations
based on work with other populations. Nevertheless, the findings are
worth considering in light of how they may apply to other groups.
For example, the results of this study imply that adherence to
exercise may be enhanced if initial efforts are directed toward
psychological variables. These could include careful and flexible goal
setting (Martin et al., 1984), emphasis on the relaxation and mood
enhancement effects often associated with exercise (Morgan &
Goldston, 1987), and talking to participants about exercise as part of a
healthy philosophy of life including what this may mean in terms of
other important areas of functioning such as job and family. The program
could then introduce social reinforcers that would facilitate the
acquisition of an identity based on the activity being performed
(Dubbert, Rappaport, & Martin, 1987). Examples include public
rewards for achieving certain milestones, announcements in company or
agency publications, and the ever popular, yet still valued, awarding of
t-shirts displaying emblems or logos that identify the wearer as a
particular type of exerciser. The development of perceived and actual
competence is also important throughout the program and must be
considered at each phase.
References
Barrell, G., Chamberlain, A., Evans. J., Holt, T., & Mackean, J.
(1989). Ideology and commitment in family life: a case study of runners.
Leisure Studies, 8, 249-262.
Carmack, M. A., & Martens, R. (1979). Measuring commitment to
running: A survey of runners' attitudes and mental states. Journal
of Sport Psychology, 1, 24-42.
Clough, P., Shepherd, J., & Maughan, R. (1989). Motives for
participation in recreational running. Journal of Leisure Research, 21,
297-309.
Crandall, R. J. (1980). Motivations for leisure. Journal of Leisure
Research, 12, 45-54.
Dishman, R. K. (1982). Compliance/adherence in health-related
exercise. Health Psychology, 3, 237-267.
Dishman, R. K. (1986). Exercise compliance: A new view for public
health. The Physician and Sports Medicine, 14, 1:27-145
Dubbert, P. M., Rappaport, N. B., & Martin, J. E. (1987).
Exercise in cardiovascular disease. Behavior Modification, 11, 329-347.
Johnsgard, K. W. (1985). The motivation of the long distance runner:
II. Journal of Sports Medicine, 25, 140-143.
Johnsgard, K. W. (1989). The exercise prescription for depression and
anxiety. New York: Plenum.
Kasimatis, M., Langston, C. A., & Clark, L. F. (1992). Toward a
process model of exercise adherence. Paper presented at the 64th Annual
Meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, IL.
Lee, C., & Owen, N. (1986). Uses of psychological theories in
understanding the adoption and maintenance of exercising. The Australian
Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 18, 22-25.
Martin, J, E., & Dubbert, P.M. (1982). Exercise applications and
promotion in behavioral medicine: Current status and future directions.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 1004-1017.
Martin, J. E., Dubbert, P.M., Katell, A. D., Thompson, J. K.,
Raczynski, J. R., Lake, M., Smith, P. O., Webster, J. S., Sikora, T.,
& Cohen, R. E. (1984). Behavioral control of exercise in sedentary adults: Studies 1-6. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52,
795-811.
Masters, K. S., & Lambert, M. J. (1989). The relations between
cognitive coping strategies, reasons for running, injury, and
performance of marathon runners. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 17, 161-170.
Masters, K. S., Ogles, B. M, & Jolton, J. A. (1993). The
development of an instrument to measure motivation for marathon running:
The Motivations of Marathoners Scales (MOMS). Research Quarterly for
Exercise and Sport, 64, 134-143.
Morgan, W. P., & Goldston, S. E. (Eds.). (1987). Exercise and
mental health. New York: Hemisphere.
Okwumabua, T. M., Meyers, A. W., & Santille, L. (1987). A
demographic and cognitive profile of master runners. Journal of Sport
Behavior, 10, 212-220.
Shepherd, R. J. (1985). Factors influencing the exercise behavior of
patients. Sports Medicine, 2, 348-366.
Sonstroem, R. J. (1988). Psychological models. In R. K. Dishman
(Ed.), Exercise adherence: Its impact on public health, 125-153.
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Summers, J. J., Sargent, G. I., Levey, A. J., & Murray, K. D.
(1982). Middle-aged, non-elite marathon runners: A profile Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54, 963-969.
Summers, J. J., Machin, V. J., & Sargent, G. I. (1983).
Psychosocial factors related to marathon running. Journal of Sports
Psychology, 5, 314-331.