The effect of expertise on peak performance: the case of home-field advantage.
Adams, Richard David ; Kupper, Susan Jeanne
Irving and Goldstein (1990) described home-field advantage as a
territorial effect, noted that home-field advantage appeared to increase
as the superiority of performance increased, and found home-field
advantage to have a statistically significant relationship with no-hit
major league baseball games. In this study, we describe home-field
advantage as an expertise deficiency, demonstrate that, in theory and in
practice, home-field advantage decreases as superiority of performance
increases, and demonstrate that the statistically significant
relationship between home-field advantage and no-hit major league
baseball games is not applicable for pitchers who either replicated
performance by winning two or more no-hitters or amassed a large number
of career wins. Our general finding is that home-field advantage is a
metric for the inability to maintain performance independent of
environment and that this metric is inversely related to variables of
expertise.
This paper consists of six sections. The first section provides a
review of the relevant expertise literature. The second section proposes
and provides levels of the hypothesis of an inverse relationship between
home-field advantage and performance. The third section provides a
review of Irving and Goldstein (1990). The fourth section proposes tests
of hypotheses of the expertise effect within no-hit performance. The
fifth section reports the results of the tests of these hypotheses. The
sixth section presents conclusions and suggestions for future research.
Review of Literature
Territorial superiority as described by Irving and Goldstein (1990)
is a common phenomenon in nature. However, the requirement of
multi-territorial competition creates problems when attempting to
generalize this phenomenon to sports competition. Unlike organisms which
are able to limit their competitive endeavors to territories in which
they exhibit superiority, sport competitors are generally required to
compete in multiple arenas under varying conditions. In these
situations, what appears to be a territorial superiority may actually be
an environmental dependency (i.e., a difficulty in maintaining
performance independent of environment. Some examples of environmental
dependencies in baseball are natural grass vs. astroturf, day-games vs.
night-games, and playing at home vs. playing on the road. Environmental
dependencies are expected to have a negative effect on all levels of
individual performance (i.e., game, season, and career). In the absence
of environmental dependencies, individual performance can be expected to
optimize within the constraints of individual expertise.
Expertise
Hayes-Roth, Waterman, and Lenat (1983) described expertise as
consisting of domain-relevant knowledge, a comprehension of
domain-specific problems, and skills for solving those problems. This
description was consolidated by Dreyfus and Dreyfus (1986) who described
expertise as a competence level within an operative domain. Chase and
Simon (1973) had previously demonstrated that the bounds of such a
competence level could be constrained by domain-relevant knowledge and
domain-relevant problem-solving skills, Arkes and Freedman (1984)
expanded upon this when they demonstrated that the bounds of such a
competence level could also be constrained by environmental
dependencies.
The Effect of Expertise on Competition
Garland and Barry (1990) noted that when variables of expertise are
considered, levels of competitive performance can be reliably
differentiated along cognitive dimensions. This is, in fact, the general
finding in studies of both intellectual and physical competition: Bridge
(Keren, 1987), Go (Reitman, 1976), Basketball (Allard & Burnett,
1985), Field hockey (Starkes & Deakin, 1984), and Ice hockey (Bard & Fleury, 1981). Taylor (1987) demonstrated the effects of cognition on performance across six arenas of competition. The contribution of
Irving and Goldstein (1990) is that rather than differentiating
performance, they held performance constant and differentiated upon
environment. We expand upon this contribution by differentiating upon
both environment and expertise.
Home-Field Advantage as an Inverse Relationship
Defining Home-Field Advantage
Home-field advantage (HFA) can be used either as a random probability
(historic HFA) or as a home/road performance differential (team-year
HFA). Historic HFA answers the question: Given any major league baseball
game, what is the probability the home-team won the game? Team-year HFA
answers the question: Given a game won by a specific team in a specific
year, what is the probability the game was played on their home-field?
[Mathematical Expression Omitted]
where t represents each major league team and y represents each major
league season from 1900 through 1991.
For the 1724 team-year seasons from 1900 through 1991 for the
American, National, and Federal Leagues, 133,560 games resulted in a
won-lost decision (The Baseball Encyclopedia, 1990 and The Official
Major League Baseball 1992 Stat Book, 1992). Of these, 72,468 were won
by the home team. Thus, historic HFA is .5425876.
Team-Year HFA = ([Home.Wins.sub.ty]) / ([Total.Wins.sub.ty]) (2)
where T represents a specific major league team and y represents a
specific major league season from 1900 through 1991.
While team-year HFA provides an accurate probability based on actual
games played, it provides an accurate representation of home/road
performance differential only for teams having played equal numbers of
games at home and on the road. Since this occurred in slightly less than
half of the actual team-seasons observed (i.e., 856/1724), equation (2)
must be adjusted for differences in home games versus road games.
Adj. Team-Year HFA = (Home.WLP) / (Home.WLP + Road.WLP) (3)
where Home.WLP = Home.Wins / (Home.Wins + Home.Losses), and Road.WLP
= Road.Wins / (Road.Wins + Road.Losses)
Defining Performance
Competitive performance at the team level, in the absence of tied
games, is measured by won-lost percentage (WLP).
Team-Year WLP = (Games.Won) / (Games.Played) (4)
Just as team-year HFA was adjusted for differences in the numbers of
home games and road games, team-year WLP must also be adjusted:
Adj. Team-Year WLP = (Home.WLP + Road.WLP) / 2 (5)
The Relationship between Home-Field Advantage and Performance
An analysis of equations (3) and (5) indicates the following:
(1) ADJ. TEAM-YEAR HFA varies directly with HOME.WLP and inversely
with ROAD.WLP;
(2) ADJ. TEAM-YEAR WLP varies directly with both HOME.WLP and
ROAD.WLP;
(3) When the maximum ADJ. TEAM-YEAR WLP of 1.000 is achieved by a
team winning all its home games and all its road games, ADJ. TEAM-YEAR
HFA is equal to .500;
(4) In order to achieve the minimum ADJ. TEAM-YEAR HFA of 0, a team
must lose all of the home games and will, therefore, have an ADJ.
TEAM-YEAR WLP less than or equal to .500; and
(5) In order to achieve the maximum ADJ. TEAM-YEAR HFA of 1, a team
must lose all of its road games and will, therefore, have an ADJ.
TEAM-YEAR WLP less than or equal to .500.
Evaluating the Relationship
The relationship between Adjusted Team-Year Home-Field Advantage and
Adjusted Team-Year Won-Lost Percentage is constrained by the concave
function of Maximum Adjusted Team-Year Home-Field Advantage. Irving and
Goldstein (1990) noted that home-field advantage appeared to increase as
the superiority of performance increased. If, as we propose, home-field
advantage is a deficiency, then home-field advantage should decrease as
the superiority of performance increases. In support of this
proposition, we hypothesize that the slope of the following regression
equation will be negative: ADJ. TEAM-YEAR WLP = ADJ. TEAM-YEAR HFA
H1.0: The slope of the regression equation will be equal to or
greater than zero.
H1.1: The slope of the regression equation will be less than zero.
We also propose that high performance teams (i.e., teams with high
adjusted team-year won-lost percentages) are able to maintain
performance independent of environment while low performance teams
(i.e., teams with low adjusted team-year won-lost percentages) are not.
In support of this proposition, we hypothesize that, in a between-groups
analysis, high performance teams will exhibit a lower variance for
adjusted team-year home-field advantage than will low performance teams.
H2.0: The variance for adjusted team-year home-field advantage for
high performance teams will be equal to or greater than the
corresponding variance for low performance teams.
H2.1: The variance for adjusted team-year home-field advantage for
high performance teams will be less than the corresponding variance for
low performance teams.
Hypothesis 1
A simple linear regression of the 1724 team seasons produced the
following regression equation:
ADJ. TEAM-YEAR WLP = 0.68692 - 0.34387 * ADJ. TEAM-YEAR HFA.
The 95% confidence interval for [Beta] is -0.25351 [is less than]
[Beta] [is less than] -0.43424. Therefore, the slope of the regression
equation is negative and the null hypothesis can be rejected at an alpha
level of .05.
Hypothesis 2
For this hypothesis, we extracted a data set consisting of all team
seasons where adjusted won-lost percentage was either less than 0.400
(n=230) or greater than 0.600 (n=211). First, a test of dispersion parameters (Moses, 1963) was applied to confirm the difference in
variances were 0.001124 and 0.003676, respectively. This resulted in a
z-score of 5.0636 and a corresponding p[is less than].0001. Second, a
test of extreme values (Hollander, 1963) was applied to confirm the
lower variance of the high performance teams could be attributed to
central tendency. The result was a z-score of 4.8387 and a corresponding
p[is less than].0001. Therefore, the null hypothesis can be rejected at
an alpha level of .05.
A Review of Irving and Goldstein
Introduction
Irving and Goldstein (1990) collected data on no-hit games from The
Sport Encyclopedia of Baseball (Neft & Cohen, 1988) and predicted
that a significant number of no-hitters occurred on the home-field of
the winning pitcher. They excluded no-hitters of less than nine innings and no-hitters which were lost in extra innings. They determined the
number of no-hitters won at home versus on the road to be 111 to 64.
They used a chi-square goodness of fit with an expected probability of
.500. Their results are summarized in Table 1.
Table 1
Results per Irving & Goldstein (1990)
Where Expected Distribution [X.sup.2] p [is less than]
Won Probability Expected Actual
HOME .500 87.5 111 6.31
ROAD .500 87.5 64 6.31
TOTAL 1.000 175.0 175 12.62 .00039
Replicating Irving and Goldstein (1990)
When we replicated Irving and Goldstein (1990), differences were
found in both data and choice of methods. First, home-team wins versus
road-team wins were found to be 117 to 58 rather than 111 to 64. This
difference was confirmed via multiple sources (i.e., Neft & Cohen,
1988; The Baseball Encyclopedia, 1990; Palmer, Thorn, & Reuther,
1989; Dickey, 1976). Second, the home-field advantage probability of
.500 used by Irving and Goldstein (1990) was found to be inaccurate. The
historic home-field advantage for major-league baseball, as previously
shown, is .5426. Third, since the variable under observation is binomial (an event being won at home or won on the road), the binomial
probability is known (.5426), and the events are independent of each
other, a binomial probability distribution is more appropriate than a
chi-square goodness of fit (Hays, 1988). The results, after corrections,
are summarized in Tables 2 and 3. When reviewing these tables, please
note that the results are still statistically significant.
Table 2
Results After Corrections
Where Expected Distribution
Won Probability Expected Actual [X.sup.2] p [is less than]
HOME .5426 94.95 117 5.12
ROAD .4574 80.05 58 6.07
TOTAL 1.0000 175.00 175 11.19 .00083
Table 3
Results Using Binomial Probability
No-hit At On Expected p [is less than]
Games Home Road Probability
175 117 58 .5426 .00048
The Effect of Expertise
Hypotheses
Two metrics, well-established in the expertise literature (Wallace,
cited in Phelps & Shanteau, 1978; Trumbo, Adams, Milner, &
Schipper, cited in Shanteau, 1988; Phelps & Shanteau, 1978;
Shanteau, 1988) are proposed for classifying winning pitchers of no-hit
games:
(1) the ability to replicate specific performance; and (2) the
ability to sustain career performance.
Given a general hypothesis that home-field advantage and expertise
have an inverse relationship when these metrics are applied to the data,
two effects should be observable. We state them here in hypotheses 3 and
4:
H3.0: Home-field advantage will not be statistically significant for
repeat performance pitchers.
H3.1: Home-field advantage will be statistically significant for
repeat performance pitchers.
H4.0: Home-field advantage will not be statistically significant for
high career performance pitchers.
H4.1: Home-field advantage will be statistically significant for high
career performance pitchers.
Since our purpose is to demonstrate the contrary effect of an
additional variable, rather than attempting to reject the null
hypothesis at P = .05, we will attempt to fail to reject the null
hypothesis at P = .15 (Greenwald, 1975).
Data and Method
The data set for these hypotheses was extracted from The Baseball
Encyclopedia (1990), and career wins for current pitchers was updated
from The Official Major League Baseball 1992 Stat Book (1992). The data
set consists of all major league no-hitters from 1900 to 1988 using
criteria set by Irving and Goldstein (1990) (i.e., no-hitters of less
than nine innings and no-hitters which were lost in extra innings are
excluded). Each observation included the league, the teams, the date of
the game, the winning pitcher, his careerwins, whether the home team won
or lost, and whether or not the winning pitcher won other no-hitters.
Since expertise is an effect rather than a correlation (Hayes-Roth,
Waterman, & Lenat, 1983), the appropriate method is to test to the
effect in homogeneous subsets of the data rather than to test for a
correlation across full data sets (Arkes & Freedman, 1984).
Therefore, two data sets were extracted, one for each hypothesis. The
data set for repeat performance pitchers consists of 48 observations.
The data sets for high career performance were created by rank ordering
the data in descending order of careerwins of the winning pitcher and
extracting the upper 20% (n=35) of the observation.
Results
Hypothesis 3
Forty-eight no-hitters were won by 21 pitchers who won two or more
no-hitters. Of these, 28 games were won on the home-field of the winning
pitcher. The null hypothesis cannot be rejected at an alpha level of .15
(p [is less than] .338).
Hypothesis 4
Thirty-five no-hitters were won by pitchers in the upper 20% of the
rank ordered data set. Of these, 20 games were won on the home-field of
the winning pitcher. The null hypothesis cannot be rejected at an alpha
level of .15. (p [is less than] .434).
Table 4
Repeat Performance Pitchers
No-hit At On Expected p[is less than]
Games Home Road Probability
48 28 20 .5426 .3381
175 102 73 .5426 .1603
Table 5
High Career Performance Pitchers
No-hit At On Expected p[is less than]
Games Home Road Probability
35 20 15 .5426 .4338
175 100 75 .5426 .2461
Considering the Effect of Data Set Size
Nonparametric methods, in general, and the binomial probability, in
particular, are sensitive to data set size. Given a constant ratio, the
binomial probability will increase as the number of trials decreases.
This raises the possibility that the loss of significance was due to the
partitioning of the original data set into smaller data sets. Therefore,
we questioned whether or not the null hypothesis would have been
rejected had the size of the extracted data sets been equal to the size
of the original data set (n = 175) at p = .15. Although linear
transformations of the data are not viewed as statistically valid,
rejection of the null hypothesis after a linear transformation would
support an argument that the loss of significance had been due to data
set size. Thus, we have provided a second entry on Tables 4 and 5 (p [is
less than] .1603 and p [is less than] .2461, respectively) to indicate
what the results would have been had the ratio of home wins to road wins
remained constant for a data set with 175 observations and to
demonstrate that such an argument is not supported by the data.
Conclusions and Future Research
The result of tests on our first two hypotheses support our position
that home-field advantage is inversely related to won-lost percentage
and that high performance teams exhibit lower variances in and a central
tendency for home-field advantage. Failure to reject the second two
hypotheses indicates that, based on the available data, the performance
of the repeat no-hit pitchers with relatively high career wins were not
significantly affected by home-field advantage.
These findings are consistent with the findings of Garland and Barry
(1990) that when variables of expertise are considered, levels of
competitive performance can be reliably differentiated along cognitive
dimensions. They are also consistent with the findings of Irving and
Goldstein (1990) that there is a significant relationship between
home-field advantage and no-hit performance. However, these findings
extend Irving and Goldstein by demonstrating that the relationship
between home-field advantage and peak performance is inversely related
to variables of expertise.
This study and Irving and Goldstein (1990) rely upon the no-hitter as
a surrogate for peak performance. Whether or not a no-hitter is a valid
surrogate for peak performance is an empirical question which neither
study addressed. If the use of peak performance is essential to sports
behavior research, then further research is necessary to develop
well-calibrated metrics for peak performance.
References
Arkes, H. R. & Freedman. M. R. (1988). A demonstration of the
costs and benefits of expertise in recognition memory. Memory &
Cognition, 12(1), 84-89.
Allard, F. & Burnett. N. (1985). Skill in sport. Canadian Journal
of Psychology, 2, 14-21.
Bard, C. & Fleury, M. (1981). Considering eye movement as a
predictor of attainment. In I. M. Cockeral & W. W. MacGillivary
(Eds.) Vision and sport, Cheltenham, England: Stanley Thornes.
Baseball Digest. (1992). 51(1), 72-91.
The Baseball Encyclopedia. (1990), New York: Macmillan.
Chase, W. G. & Simon. H .A., (1973), Perception in chess.
Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55-81.
Dickey, G. (1976). The great no-hitters. Radnor, PA: Chilton.
Dreyfus, H. L. & Dreyfus. S. E., (1986). Mind over machine. New
York: The Free Press.
Garland, D. J. & Barry. J. R. (1990). Sport expertise: The
competitive advantage. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 70, 1299-1314.
Greenwald, A. G. (1975). Consequences of prejudice against the null
hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 1-20.
Hayes-Roth, F., Watermall. D. A., & Lenat, D. B. (1983). An
overview of expert systems. In Hayes-Roth, Waterman, & Lenat (Eds.),
Building Expert Systems. New York: Addison-Wesley.
Hays, W. L. (1988). Statistics (4th ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart
& Winston.
Hollander, M. (1963). A nonparametric test for the two-sample
problem. Psychometrika, 28, 395-403.
Irving, P. G. & Goldstein. S. R. (1990). Effect of home-field
advantage on peak performance of baseball pitchers. Journal of Sports
Behavior, 13(1), 23-27.
Keren, G. B. (1987). Facing uncertainty in the game of bridge.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 39(1), 98-114.
Moses, L. E. (1963). Rank tests of dispersion. Annals of Mathematical
Statistics, 34, 973-983.
Neft & Cohen (Eds.). (1988). Sports Encyclopedia of Baseball. New
York: St. Martin's.
The Official Major League Baseball 1992 Stat Book. (1992), New York:
Macmillan.
Palmer, P., Thorn, J. & Reuther, D. (Eds.). (1989). TotaI
Baseball. New York: Warner Books.
Phelps, R. H. & Shanteau, J. (1978). Livestock judges: How much
information can an expert use. Organizational Behavior and Human
Performance, 21, 209-219.
Reitman, J. (1976). Skilled perception in GO: Deducing memory
structures from inter-response times. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 336-356.
Shanteau, J. (1988). Psychological characteristics and strategies of
expert decision makers. Acta Psychological, 68, 203-215.
Starkes, J. L. & Deakin. J. (1984). Perception in sport: A
cognitive approach to skilled performance. In W. F. Straub & J. M.
Williams (Eds.), Cognitive sports psychology (pp. 115-128), Lansing, NY:
Sport Science Association.
Taylor, J. (1987). Predicting athletic performance with
self-confidence and somatic and cognitive anxiety as a function of motor
and psychological requirements in six sports. Journal of Personality,
33, 139-153.