Family structure effects on parenting stress and practices in the African American family.
Combs-Orme, Terri
The predominant approach to African-American parenting research
focuses on disadvantages associated with single parenthood to the
exclusion of other issues. The current research suggests that this does
not represent the diversity in family structure configurations among
African-American families, nor does it give voice to the parenting
resilience of single mothers. We argue that rather than marital status or family configuration, more attention needs to be given to the
inadequacy of resources for this population.
In the current study, we examined the parenting of infants by
African-American mothers and found that mothers' marital status and
family configuration did not affect parenting stress or practices. This
suggests, then, that single mothers parent as well as their married,
partnered, and multigenerational counterparts. It seems that the
economic status and parenting perceptions of mothers contributed more to
parenting stress than did marital status or family structure. Our study,
then, challenges the accepted wisdom in our political and popular
culture that has insisted upon the centrality of the nuclear family to
all aspects of familial and even national health. Instead, we have shown
that a true commitment to strong families and healthy children begins
with a focus on the debilitating effects of poverty in the
African-American community.
Key words: African-American, infant parenting, parenting stress
**********
Parenting an infant is a stressful occupation, even under ideal
circumstances. Infants require constant attention and, particularly in
the first few months of life, must have their every need met constantly
(see Combs-Orme, Wilson, Cain, Page & Kirby, 2003, for a discussion
of the essentials of parenting specific to infants). Thus it is clear
that the full-time job of parenting an infant ideally involves more than
one parent. Yet the reality is that many children grow up in homes that
do not have two parents present. This is especially true in the
African-American community, in which nearly 50% of children are born
outside of marriage (or live at some time in a home without one parent
or the other) (Andersen, 2000). This study seeks to identify the family
structure circumstances that make parenting more or less stressful and
successful, and thus seeks to contribute to knowledge that might support
parents and families in their efforts to provide the best care possible
for every child.
Current research on parenting practices within the African-American
community is limited due to an absence of longitudinal research; a
severe lack of attention to intragroup variability; a disregard for the
inherent diversity in the African-American community; and a minimization
of the staggering effects of economic deprivation, racism and social
stratification on processes and functioning in the African-American home
(Garcia-Coll, Lamberty, Jenkins, McAdoo, Crnic, Wasik & Garcia,
1996). When race is the focal point of research on parenting,
between-group differences are paramount in analysis (i.e.
European-American vs. African-American), and researchers typically
employ a cultural-equivalent theoretical framework (employing
theoretical shifts from Allen, 1978). This framework touts
European-American, middle-class values and practices as the ideal or
norm, and compares African-American values and practices to that norm.
African-Americans that are more acculturated and exhibit the values and
behaviors of the normed group are depicted as legitimate and are
highlighted. However, much of the "race-comparative" research
encourages the documentation of unfavorable outcomes of African-American
children and families (McLoyd, 1990), and conclusions often concentrate
on how African-American children are abnormal, deficient, or incompetent when compared to the middle-class European-American mainstream
(Barbarin, 1993; Garcia-Coll, et al., 1996; McLoyd, 1990; Myers, Rana
& Harris 1979; Washington & McLoyd, 1982). Thus, the current
state of literature on African-American children and their families has
as its core the explanation of developmental deviations in comparison to
European-American, middle-class norms. Critics of this approach assert
that race-comparative research often blames African-American parents for
not transmitting the "right" educational, moral, and ethical
values to their children, while ignoring situational, contextual, and
systemic factors (McLoyd & Randolph, 1984; Spencer, 1990).
Franklin and Boyd-Franklin (1985) contend that traditional African
values and beliefs have been transmitted from generation to generation
and continue to influence African-American parenting. One of the most
important of these traditions is that parents traditionally view
childrearing as a communal task to be shared by all members of the
community (Franklin & Boyd-Franklin, 1985; Garcia-Coll, Meyer &
Brillon, 1995; McAdoo, 1978). Thus, historically African-American
parents have relied on extended family networks (i.e. relatives,
neighbors, fictive kin, and church members) to share in child care
(Wilson, 1989). Research that examines communal childrearing practices
in the African-American home asserts that this tradition may mitigate some of the negative effects of single parenthood (Jackson, 1993; Young,
1970).
The traditional Western model of the family includes two married
parents and their children (the model nuclear family). However, not all
families can or want to model themselves after this norm. Baca Zinn and
Eitzen (1999) report that only 10% of U.S. families fit the ideal of a
two-parent family in which the male works and the female stays home to
care for the children. Thus, in order to properly study parenting, we
must ask who exactly provides love and support to children. And, for
African-American families, marital status and extended kin networks are
significant variables in the study of parenting.
Encouraged by a review of studies on African-American mothers
(Murray, Bynum, Brody, Willert & Stephens, 2001) the current study
employs a risk and resilience approach to the study of African-American
parenting practices from a cultural-variant theoretical perspective
(Allen, 1978). The cultural-variant perspective views African-American
families as different, yet functional in their own right. The
perspective is encapsulated by bell hooks' infamous charge to bring
marginalized groups, "from margin to center" (hooks, 1984),
and this is the basis of an Afrocentric and risk and resilience
approach. To place African-Americans in the center of their reality, we
will offer a critical examination of intragroup variability in infant
parenting practices.
The research on co-caregiving influences on parenting practices in
the African-American home is sparse. In particular, there is very little
research on parenting practices in intact, two-generation
African-American families. Much of the research focuses on single
African-American mothers and young African-American mothers parenting
with the assistance of their mothers. Moreover, more diverse family
structures (i.e. co-residence with unmarried partners) are often not
examined due to the use of marital status as a proxy for family
structure (Murray et al, 2001).
The literature reveals that race-homogeneous studies find that
maternal grandmothers are typically the co-caregivers with single,
African-American mothers (Brody, Flor & Neubaum, 1998; Hunter, 1997;
Pearson, Hunter, Ensminger & Kellam, 1990). However, Hunter,
Pearson, Ialongo, and Kellam (1998) found that biological fathers are
more frequently nominated for co-caregiving in a race-comparative
sample. Hunter, et al. (1998) suggests that family proximity and
residence in the rural South are primary considerations for the election
of maternal grandmothers as the co-caregivers in single-mother,
African-American families. Maternal age is also associated with
co-resident co-caregiving with grandmothers. The youngest mothers are
more inclined to parent with the assistance of their mothers due in part
to reduced options. However, Chase-Lansdale, Books-Gunn and Zamsky
(1994) found that the age at first birth was not a significant predictor
of maternal parenting quality, nor were grandmothers more effective
parents than co-residing adolescent mothers.
The relationship between the co-caregiver and the African-American
mother appears to have an effect on the quality of parenting provided by
the mother. Barbarin and Soler (1993) found that, with the exception of
mother-grandmother combinations, the beneficial effect of living in a
two-adult household holds true only for biological parents. Tolson and
Wilson (1990) found that two-generation African-American families tend
to be more organized than multigenerational families. Several studies
(Chase-Lansdale et al., 1994; Jackson, Gyamfi, Brooks-Gunn & Blake,
1998; Wakschlag, Chase-Lansdale & Brooks-Gunn, 1996) have
established that maternal co-residence with grandmothers was negatively
associated with parenting, whereas maternal maturity and autonomy was
associated with competent parenting. Jackson (1998) and Jackson and her
colleagues (1998) speculate that the instrumental support provided by a
co-residing grandmother can be a source of distress for the mother
because of differing views on parenting practices and boundary issues.
And, Brody, Flor and Neubaum (1998) found that co-caregiver conflict was
associated with less mother-child involvement.
In related research, mothers were significantly more likely to
parent appropriately when the mothers and grandmothers did not live
together (Wakschlag et al., 1996). This may be due to modeling effects.
That is, modeling appropriate parenting practices "from a
distance" is thought to be more effective because multigenerational
relationships are more individuated when mothers and grandmothers live
separately (Wakschlag et al., 1996, 2141). Modeling is thought to be
less effective in co-residing conditions because grandmothers'
participation in the parenting role may contribute to tensions between
mothers and grandmothers (due to differing views on parenting practices)
and mothers and children (due to blurred boundaries between the parental
sphere and the child sphere). In addition, there may be a
"selection effect" (Wakschlag et al., 1996, p. 2141) in that
more mature (i.e. older, more autonomous) mothers may live separately
from their mothers, be more open to learning from their mothers, and
practice more appropriate parenting. Clearly, the age of the mother must
be taken into account in examining the effects of co-residence on
parenting. Inversely, positive co-caregiving behaviors practiced by
mothers and grandmothers in the African-American home, such as the
provision of instrumental support and practical assistance, have been
found to be positively associated with high levels of parental control,
the use of physical punishment, and affectionate behaviors toward
children (Brody, Flor & Neubaum, 1998). Wilson, Kohn, Curry-El and
Hinton (1995) also found that having more adults in the home
significantly influenced maternal perceptions of punishment behavior.
That is, the more adults in the home, the more favorable maternal
perceptions about punishment (spanking and yelling).
Intragroup studies on maternal co-caregiving in the
African-American family suggest that maternal grandmothers are most
often selected as co-caregiving partners. We investigated co-caregiving
partners in the present study and extended the scope of co-caregivng to
include unmarried partners, married partners, and maternal grandmothers.
We also compared parenting with a co-caregiver to parenting alone.
Contrary to popular perceptions about the positive effects of
multigenerational households on young mothers' parenting quality,
maternal co-residence with grandmothers appears to be negatively
associated with positive parenting. However, this relationship may be
mediated by the quality of the co-caregiving relationship and by
maternal maturity. Inversely, maternal autonomy and psychological
maturity appear to be associated with competent parenting. We examined
the quality of mother--grandmother relationships, maternal age, and
co-residence on parenting stress and practices in the current study.
Moreover, previous research has found that instrumental support and
practical assistance by grandmothers is associated with high levels of
parental control and the use of physical punishment, and that the number
of adults in a home influence perceptions of punishment. Thus, we
examined maternal attitudes toward the use of corporal punishment in
relation to family structure in the present study.
Methods
This study was part of a larger longitudinal study of maternal
parenting practices with newborns (The Volunteer Infant Parent
Study--VIPS) (Combs-Orme, Cain, & Wilson, 2004; Combs-Orme et al.,
2003). A total of 246 European-American and African-American mothers
were recruited from the population of delivering mothers at a
University-affiliated, publicly-funded hospital in a mid-size
southeastern city between February and November, 1999. Approximately
3200 babies are delivered annually at this urban hospital surrounded by
suburban areas and remote mountainous counties without delivery
facilities. Interviews were conducted in mothers' hospital rooms
within 36 hours after birth. The sub-sample of predominantly urban,
African-American mothers is used in this study (n = 103, 42%).
We provided mothers with $10 gift certificates for a large discount
department store chain for participation. Due to the sensitive nature of
some of the questions in the recruitment interview, mothers were only
interviewed in private. The recruitment interview took approximately 30
minutes to complete.
An opportunity sample was used because circumstances did not permit
probability sampling, as the hospital was not willing to provide an
enumeration of its delivering patients, and resources would not permit
an interviewer to be on the Unit at all times.
The recruitment interview included an Informed Consent Form, the
Adult-Adolescent Parenting Inventory-2 (AAPI-2) (Bavolek, 1984), the
Parental Bonding Inventory (PBI) (Parker, Tupling & Brown, 1979),
and extensive tracking data to facilitate follow-up.
We maintained early and continuous tracking of respondents between
recruitment and follow-up using personal letters and with the help of
collaterals and resources such as internet websites, local utility
companies and public housing boards. In total, 93% (n = 96) of the
African-American mothers were interviewed at follow-up between August,
1999, and July, 2000.
Mothers were given gift certificates valued at between $10 and $60
for a large discount department store chain to participate in the second
interview, with mothers who were hard to schedule being encouraged to
participate with increased incentives. Of the follow-up interviews, 97%
(n = 93) were conducted in mothers' homes, the homes of family or
friends, or at various community locations including coffee shops,
offices or jail, and 3% (n = 3) were conducted over the telephone when
this was the only way mothers would participate.
There were no differences in maternal age, education, marital
status, employment, total family income or previous parenting between
African-American mothers who stayed in the sample for follow-up (n = 96)
and those who did not participate at follow-up (n = 7) ([X.sup.2](6) =
7.59, p = .270).
The follow-up interview included measures of: parenting stress,
behavioral and emotional adjustment, parenting practices and the home
environment.
Extensive demographic data were gathered at recruitment and
follow-up, including household composition or family structure. In eight
cases mothers reported living with great-grandmothers,
foster-grandmothers, or aunts and were coded as living with
"grandmothers." Five mothers reported living with grandmothers
and unmarried partners and were coded as living with
"grandmothers." We also recorded mothers' date of birth,
previous parenting experience (binary), and maternal education. Data
were collected at follow-up about changes since delivery in household
composition or family structure (see above for coding), maternal
employment (binary), and family income.
Family structure, as described above, is used as the main
independent variable in the study, with possible confounders of the
relationship between co-caregiving and parenting entered as control
variables. Control variables include: maternal age (due to the presumed
influence of maturity on parenting practices); maternal employment (due
to the possible influence of work- and time management-related stress on
parenting); prior parenting (due to the presumed learning curve
associated with parenting); and total family income (due to the
relationship between adequacy of resources and parenting). It should be
noted that infant co-caregiving is inferred based on co-residence.
Research has indicated correlations among the nurturing individuals
receive in childhood, relationship quality, and subsequent parenting
(Brody, Flor & Neubaum, 1998; George & Solomon, 1999; Solomon
& George, 1996). The quality of the co-caregiving relationship was
estimated using two measures of mother-grandmother relationship. The
Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) (Parker et al., 1979), administered at
delivery, is a 25-item instrument that measures an adult's
perceptions of his/her primary caregivers on dimensions of caring and
protection. Of the 83 mothers in the current study who reported the
identities of their primary caregivers, 95% reported maternal
caregivers.
The care subscale of the PBI has 12 items allowing for a maximum
score of 36 (higher scores indicate greater care), and the protection
subscale has 13 items allowing for a maximum score of 39 (higher scores
indicate greater overprotection and control). The overall measure and
the subscales (care and protection) show acceptable published
test-retest, split-half, and inter-rater reliability and concurrent
validity (Parker et al., 1979).
At follow-up, mothers also rated their relationships with their own
mothers using a three-point scale (worse, average or better when
compared to others) on the Young Adult Self Report (YASR) (Achenbach,
1997), which measures adaptive functioning in social relationships.
Achenbach (1997) reports acceptable one-week test-retest reliability.
According to Abidin (1995), parenting stress is the tension parents
feel in fulfilling their parenting functions. This tension or stress may
be associated with mothers' self-perceived competence in the
parenting role, feelings of social isolation and emotional closeness to
their infants, physical health, feelings of restriction within the
parenting roles, and depression. Also associated with parenting stress
are infant qualities that make it difficult for parents to fulfill their
parenting roles, such as hyperactivity or demandingness. Parenting
stress is important in the study of parenting because stress can lead to
child abuse.
The Parenting Stress Index-Short Form (PSI-SF) (Abidin, 1995), a
36-item, standardized instrument used to measure stress related to
parenting and parent-child interactions, is widely used in
investigations of parenting stress and intervention research. The
measure was standardized for use with parents of children from I month
to 12 years of age.
The PSI-SF has three subscales (parental distress, difficult child,
and parent-child dysfunctional interaction), each with a range from 12
to 60, with higher scores indicating greater parenting stress. For
comparison, raw scores are converted to percentile scores, and scores
above the 90th percentile represent clinical levels of stress that
should be referred for professional assistance.
Published coefficient alphas for the PSI-SF subscales vary from .70
to .84. Construct validity is supported by theoretically meaningful
correlations between the PSI-SF scores and constructs such as child
adjustment. In addition, studies show higher (more stressed) PSI-SF
scores among neglectful, drug-addicted, maladjusted, and abusive parents
(Abidin, 1995). For low-income African-American mothers, Hutcheson and
Black (1996) found the PSI-SF to to have acceptable levels of internal
consistency and stability over six months and high concurrent validity.
The Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME)
Inventory for Families of Infants and Toddlers (Caldwell & Bradley,
1984), the most widely used measure for observational data on the
quality of the home environment, is a naturalistic observational
technique used to measure parenting practices. Scoring for the HOME is
based on a minimum of an hour of observation, along with information
gleaned during the interview.
Four subscales in the HOME that measure various aspects of
parenting behaviors were used as dependent variables: emotional and
verbal responsivity, acceptance, provision of appropriate play
materials, and parental involvement. Scores are based on dicotomous
responses (present or not present) for each question, and summated
scores are compared to norms, with respondents in the lowest quartile considered "at risk" for poor child development related to
poor parenting.
Published internal consistency estimates summarized by Bradley
(1994) are consistently over .80 for total scores, with subscale
coefficients from .30 to .80. Inter-rater reliability has been
consistently reported to be .80 or greater. A review of the concurrent
and predictive validity of the HOME showed significant relationships to
children's intellectual level and cognitive development (Benasich
& Brooks-Gunn, 1996). Although Berlin, Brooks-Gunn, Spiker, and
Zaslow (1995) contend that the HOME Learning Materials Subscale may
overlook some ways in which poor mothers provide general learning
experiences without economic resources, Bradley, Caldwell, Rock,
Barnard, Gray, Hammond, Mitchell, Siegel, Ramey, Gottfried, and Johnson
(1989) and Bradley, Mundfrom, Whiteside, Casey, and Barrett (1994)
assert that the HOME is valid for use with economically disadvantaged and African-American families.
Maternal attitudes toward the use of corporal punishment are
measured using the Strong Belief in the Use and Value of Corporal
Punishment Subscale of the Adult-Adolescent Parenting Inventory (AAPI-2)
(note that maternal attitudes toward the use of corporal punishment are
measured, not the actual use of corporal punishment). The AAPI-2 is a
validated and reliable 40-item inventory designed to measure parenting
attitudes (Bavolek & Keene, 1999). Bavolek (1984) reports that
sampling considerations in the establishment of the AAPI norms included
"geographic region, urban and rural settings, ethnic group, sex,
socioeconomic status and age" (p. 45). The Corporal Punishment
subscale has eleven items. Age-specific (adolescent and adult) scores
are compared to norms (sten scores). Low sten scores (1 to 4) indicate a
risk for practicing known abusive parenting practices (i.e. hitting,
intimidation, pain and belittlement); high sten scores (7 to 10)
indicate parenting attitudes that reflect a nurturing, non-abusive
parenting philosophy (i.e. the use of alternative strategies to corporal
punishment); and mid-range sten scores (4 to 7) represent the parenting
attitudes of the general population.
Results
The objective of these analyses was to identify how family
structure/co-caregiving affects maternal stress, parenting, and
attitudes toward the use of corporal punishment. In order to investigate
these research questions, multiple linear regression was used due to the
continuous nature of the dependent variables. To test the effect of
co-caregiving, the control variables were entered into the regression
equation first; family structure was entered into the regression
equation after entry of the control variables. Two-tailed tests (Alpha
[less than or equal to] .05) were used because results in either
direction were of importance and there is inadequate prior research for
definitive predictions about the directions of the relationships.
Few scale items had missing data; items with missing data had only
a small percentage missing data; and few respondents had any missing
data. Missing item values were imputed with expectation maximization
using non-missing values for the other items in the particular scale
(Acock, 1997) (SPSS Version 10).
Coefficient alpha was computed for each measure. All measures used
in the study had good to excellent internal reliability, ranging from
.64 to .89. Statistical power is the probability that the null
hypothesis will be correctly rejected, and it is the complement of a
Type II error. The sample sizes used in the regression analyses reported
here are adequate to detect medium to large effect sizes, but not small
effect sizes (Cohen, 1988).
Table 1 shows that of the 96 mothers and infants in the study, 36%
were living alone, 23.9% were living with grandmothers, 25% with
unmarried partners, and 14.5% with married partners. Family structure
was significantly related at the .000 level to both age of mother and
total family income. The youngest mothers were living with grandmothers
and the oldest with married partners. Those living alone were the
poorest: nearly three-quarters had incomes under $5,000. Next
disadvantaged were those living with grandmothers, followed by those
with unmarried partners. Married mothers had the greatest economic
resources.
Table 1 also reveals family structure was significantly related at
the .005 level to both previous parenting and maternal education, but
not related to maternal employment. Over three-quarters of married
mothers reported prior parenting experience (85.7%), whereas only about
one-quarter of mothers living with grandmothers did. Mothers living
alone, and mothers living with unmarried partners, were about as likely
to have parented prior to the index children as not.
Only mothers living with unmarried partners were more likely to be
employed (54.2%). Between 57.1% and 69.6% of the other mothers were
unemployed. Of the mothers who were working (42%), 30% were doing so
full-time.
The majority of all mothers, regardless of family structure, had at
least a high school education (range from 87% of mothers living with
their mothers, to 100% of mothers living with unmarried partners and
married partners). Over half of mothers living with married partners had
some college (57.1%).
Tables 2 and 3 show the results for the multiple regression analyses. Co-caregiving did not affect maternal stress after controlling
for maternal age, education, employment, total family income, previous
parenting, maternal care, maternal overprotection, and maternal
relationship quality with her caregiver. However, as high as 16% of our
sample reported parental distress levels in the clinical range, and 17%
of the variance in parental distress could be explained using maternal
demographic variables. In particular, bivariate correlations reveal that
parental distress significantly increased as total family income
decreased. Distress also significantly increased for mothers who
reported that as children they received less optimal care from their
primary caregivers.
Co-caregiving also was not related to maternal parenting after
controlling for the possible cofounders. However, 16% of the mothers
exhibited "at risk" levels of maternal acceptance and
involvement, potentially placing their infants at risk developmentally.
In addition, co-caregiving did not affect maternal attitudes toward
the use of corporal punishment after taking account of the control
variables. However, the mean score for the corporal punishment subscale
was 34.29 (SD = 5.46), indicating that these mothers (67%) may be at
risk for abuse (i.e. hitting, intimidation, pain and belittlement).
Maternal demographic variables did not affect maternal attitudes toward
the use of corporal punishment.
Discussion
The current study reveals that with respect to parenting stress and
practices, the multigenerational and two-parent family is not
necessarily an improvement over single motherhood. Poverty and the
quality of the parenting experienced by mothers influenced parenting
more than marital status or family structure. Moreover, the relative
lack of severe parenting dysfunction among the poor and single-parent
mothers in our study suggests great hidden assets, resilience, and
strengths in the African-American family. Our study, then, has important
implications for the way we think about single motherhood in general and
African-American parenting in particular on levels ranging from the
clinical to the political. In other words, our study challenges the
accepted wisdom in our political and popular culture that has insisted
upon the centrality of the nuclear family to all aspects of familial and
even national health. Instead, we have shown that a true commitment to
strong families and healthy children begins with a focus on the
debilitating effects of poverty in the African-American community.
A brief review of our sample reveals that the majority of
African-American mothers were parenting their infants with the help of
co-caregivers, however, the largest single proportion of mothers were
parenting their infants alone. Mothers parenting alone were the poorest;
however, they experienced comparable levels of parenting stress, and
were as responsive, accepting, and involved with their infants as
mothers parenting with the help of married partners, unmarried partners
and their infants' grandmothers. In addition, single
African-American mothers had comparable learning materials in their
homes as mothers parenting with the help of co-caregivers, and promoted
the use of corporal punishment in a comparable manner. These findings
suggest overarching parenting resilience among poor and single mothers.
While marital status and family structure did not affect parenting
stress, 16% of our sample reported parental distress levels that
warranted referrals for professional assistance. Lower incomes and
mothers' reports that they themselves were not nurtured adequately
increased parenting role stress. Not surprisingly, the financial burdens
of parenthood contributed to parenting role stress regardless of family
structure. Of interest is the finding that mothers' perceptions of
the parenting they received significantly affected their distress,
indicating a potential pathway for the transmission of parenting
practices across generations.
Moreover, while our study found that marital status and family
structure did not affect maternal parenting practices, 16% of our
mothers exhibited "at risk" levels of maternal acceptance and
involvement, potentially placing their infants at risk developmentally.
Again, the vast majority of mothers (upwards of 84%) scored in the
normative range for acceptance and involvement, indicating that the
majority of mothers are faring well in their parenting capacities.
The mothers who appear to be significantly less accepting and
involved with their infants may be exhibiting culturally acceptable and
traditional parenting practices. Maternal acceptance appears to be
closely related to discipline style. Bradley (1998a,b,c) contends that
firm, hands-on discipline in the African-American culture is
traditional, functional, and appropriate. However, this sentiment is
widely contested. Maternal involvement, measures how consistently the
mother talks to the infant, provides toys and structural play for the
infant, and keeps the infant in visual range. Young (1970) found that
African-American children are not commonly encouraged to converse with
adults but are encouraged to listen and follow orders and interact with
other children. The adaptive strengths suggested in African-American
families (i.e. extended family support and shared family roles)
(Franklin & Boyd-Franklin, 1985; Garcia-Coll et al., 1995; McAdoo,
1978; Young 1970) would also suggest that African-American mothers'
involvement with children is not as developmentally salient as it would
be under European standards due to additional caregivers'
involvement. These culturally acceptable practices could conceivably decrease the level of involvement between the infants and mothers, but
whether they are detrimental to the infant developmentally should be
determined empirically. Even if low maternal involvement is detrimental,
involvement by other family members may act to protect African-American
infants from negative effects.
Similarly, while marital status and family structure did not affect
maternal attitudes toward the use of corporal punishment, 67% of mothers
reported agreement with very strict, rigid, and authoritarian discipline
practices (i.e. hitting, intimidation, pain and belittlement). This
finding urges a review of a point of contention in the African-American
parenting literature regarding the use of harsh, physical discipline.
Bradley (1998a,b,c) takes one side of the debate. She summarizes
"seminal studies" on African-American parenting to conclude
that African-American parents embrace a firm, hands-on approach to child
discipline that is functional, appropriate and administered by caring
supportive parents. She discusses this firm discipline approach as an
unconscious influence transmitted from slavery where African-American
parents maintained harsh controls over children to protect them from
suffering and death inflicted by the slave owners. And, she extends this
model to a contemporary context, arguing that firm discipline is needed
in today's society to prepare African-American children to live,
work and function in a racist society. Raymond, Jones and Cooke (1998)
take the opposing position and find Bradley's sentiments regarding
the functional and appropriate use of corporal punishment unfounded.
They state that although "slave-parenting" techniques may have
been transmitted to this generation, they are no longer functional or
adaptive and may be transmitting underachievement and a violent
propensity.
The problem with this debate is that African-American parenting
practices and child outcome studies have not been explored sufficiently
to make any conclusions. Our study suggests that the majority of
African-American mothers embrace very strict, rigid, and authoritarian
attitudes toward discipline. However, actual practices and outcomes for
children were not measured. This is an important area that requires
further exploration.
The aforementioned results should be understood in light of study
limitations. Probability sampling was not achieved, possibly threatening
external validity. Additionally, the sample sizes used to test
co-caregiving and parenting stress and practices were sufficient only to
detect large effect sizes. Thus it is conceivable that co-caregiving
does affect parenting stress and practices in practically significant
ways that could not be detected in the current study.
Table 1
Demographic Characteristics
Percent Percent
Percent Mother, Mother,
Percent Mother, Baby & Baby &
Mother Baby & Unmarried Married
& Baby Grandmother Partner Partner
(n = 35) (n = 23) (n = 24) (n = 14)
Characteristic
Maternal Age *
14-18 years 17.1 65.2 20.8 7.1
19-23 years 54.3 21.7 37.5 28.6
24-28 years 20.0 8.7 25.0 35.7
29-33 years 2.9 4.3 12.5 21.4
34-37 years 5.7 0.0 4.2 7.1
Income **
<5,000 71.4 39.1 29.2 7.1
5,000-19,999 28.6 34.8 37.5 50.0
20,000-34,999 0.0 21.7 33.3 14.3
35,000-49,999 0.0 0.0 0.0 14.3
50,000+ 0.0 4.3 0.0 14.3
Previous Parenting ***
Yes 54.3 26.1 54.2 85.7
No 45.7 73.9 45.8 14.3
Maternal Employment ****
Not employed 60.0 69.6 45.8 57.1
Part-time 11.4 13.0 12.5 7.1
Full-time 28.6 17.4 41.7 35.7
Maternal Education *****
8th grade or less 0.0 8.7 0.0 0.0
GED 11.4 4.3 0.0 0.0
High School 54.3 69.6 58.3 35.7
Voc. Training 22.9 8.7 25.0 7.1
College 11.4 8.7 16.7 57.1
Note. The percentage of missing data ranged from 0 to 2%.
* F = 7.8, p = .000
** F = 11.5, p = .000
*** [chi square] = 12.7, p = .005
**** [chi square] = 3.9, p = .69
***** [chi square] = 13.0, p = .005
Table 2
Co-Caregiving Effects on Maternal Stress (N = 92)
Total Stress
Variables B t Beta
Age -.65 -1.35 -.25
Education 1.40 .62 .10
Employment -1.67 -.45 -.06
Income -1.10 -.52 -.08
Previous Parenting 1.88 1.12 .17
Care -.28 -1.35 -.15
Overprotection .06 .29 .03
Relationship Quality -1.10 -.76 -.08
Step 1 [R.sup.2] = .13 ([R.sup.2] adj = .04)
F (8,83) = 1.51,
p = .165
Married Partner -9.34 -1.70 *
Unmarried Partner -5.21 -1.33 *
Grandmother -6.35 -1.45 *
Step 2 [R.sup.2] change = .04,
F change (3,80) = 1.27,
p = .289
Overall Model [R.sup.2] = .17 ([R.sup.2] adj = .05)
F (3,80) = 1.46,
p = .164
Parental Distress
Variables B t Beta
Age .03 .11 .02
Education -1.01 -.07 -.11
Employment 1.28 .54 .07
Income -1.72 -1.25 -.19
Previous Parenting .21 .20 .03
Care -.22 -1.65 -.19
Overprotection .14 1.03 .11
Relationship Quality -.80 -.86 -.09
Step 1 [R.sup.2] = .17 ([R.sup.2] adj = .09)
F (8,83) = 2.07,
p = .048
Married Partner -2.02 -.57 *
Unmarried Partner -2.21 -.87 *
Grandmother -1.70 -.60 *
Step 2 [R.sup.2] change = .01,
F change (3,80) = .28,
p = .838
Overall Model [R.sup.2] = .18 ([R.sup.2] adj = .06)
F (3,80) = 1.54,
p = .133
Dysfuctional Interaction
Variables B t Beta
Age -.39 -1.85 -.34
Education 1.25 1.26 .20
Employment -1.29 -.80 -.11
Income .27 .29 .05
Previous Parenting 1.08 1.46 .22
Care -.02 -.24 -.03
Overprotection -.03 -.29 -.03
Relationship Quality -.15 -.23 -.03
Step 1 [R.sup.2] = .07 ([R.sup.2] adj = -.02)
F (8,83) = .81,
p = .599
Married Partner -5.85 -2.42 *
Unmarried Partner -3.20 -1.85 *
Grandmother -4.07 -2.11 *
Step 2 [R.sup.2] change = .08,
F change (3,80) = 2.59,
p = .058
Overall Model [R.sup.2] = .15 ([R.sup.2] adj = .04)
F (3,80) = 1.33,
p = .225
Difficult Child
Variables B t Beta
Age -.38 -1.63 -.30
Education .61 .55 .09
Employment -.51 -.28 -.04
Income -.64 -.61 -.09
Previous Parenting 1.01 1.23 .18
Care -.19 -1.82 -.21
Overprotection .07 .71 .08
Relationship Quality -.58 -.82 -.09
Step 1 [R.sup.2] = .14 ([R.sup.2] adj = .06)
F (8,83) = 1.70,
p = .111
Married Partner -2.66 -.99 *
Unmarried Partner -.48 -.25 *
Grandmother -1.25 -.59 *
Step 2 [R.sup.2] change = .01,
F change (3,80) = .37,
p = .777
Overall Model [R.sup.2] = .15 ([R.sup.2] adj = .04)
F (3,80) = 1.31,
p = .236
* Standardized Beta Coefficients are not used with dummy coded
variables
Table 3
Co-Caregiving Effects on Maternal Parenting (N= 81)
Responsivity
Variables B t Beta
Age .01 .12 .03
Education .19 .60 .10
Employment .11 .22 .03
Income .04 .13 .02
Previous Parenting .03 .11 .02
Care -.01 -.20 -.03
Overprotection .00 .14 .02
Relationship Quality -.24 -1.22 -.15
Step 1 [R.sup.2] = .05 ([R.sup.2] adj = -.06)
F = (8,72) = .43,
p = .899
Married Partner .24 .32 *
Unmarried Partner .65 1.19 *
Grandmother .65 1.14 *
Step 2 [R.sup.2]change = .03,
F change (3,69) = .65,
p = .588
Overall Model [R.sup.2] = .07 ([R.sup.2] adj = -.08)
F = (3,69) = .48,
p = .907
Acceptance
Variables B t Beta
Age -.05 -.74 .17
Education .34 1.13 .19
Employment .18 .38 .06
Income .13 .48 .08
Previous Parenting .24 1.03 .19
Care -.03 -1.03 -.14
Overprotection .03 1.07 .13
Relationship Quality -.04 -.21 -.03
Step 1 [R.sup.2] = .05 [R.sup.2] adj = -.05)
F = (8,72) = .49,
p = .859
Married Partner -.48 -.69 *
Unmarried Partner .50 .94 *
Grandmother .64 1.16 *
Step 2 [R.sup.2]change = .04,
F change (3,69) = 1.10,
p = .356
Overall Model [R.sup.2] = .10 ([R.sup.2] adj = -.05)
F = (3,69) = .66,
p = .773
Involvement
Variables B t Beta
Age -.01 -.09 -.02
Education .18 .61 .10
Employment -.28 -.57 -.08
Income .62 2.32 .35
Previous Parenting -.28 -1.16 -.20
Care .00 -.12 -.02
Overprotection .01 .34 .04
Relationship Quality -.21 -1.09 -.12
Step 1 [R.sup.2] = .16 ([R.sup.2] adj = -.06)
F = (8,72) = 1.67,
p = .121
Married Partner -.38 -.53 *
Unmarried Partner .65 1.22 *
Grandmother -.48 -.86 *
Step 2 [R.sup.2]change = .05,
F change (3,69) = 1.45,
p = .235
Overall Model [R.sup.2] = .21 ([R.sup.2] adj = .08)
F = (3,69) = 1.63,
p = .109
Learning Materials
Variables B t Beta
Age .01 .19 .04
Education .34 1.40 .18
Employment -.32 -.61 -.09
Income .43 1.51 .24
Previous Parenting -.12 -.49 -.09
Care .00 .04 .01
Overprotection .02 .63 .08
Relationship Quality -.01 -.07 -.01
Step 1 [R.sup.2] = .11 ([R.sup.2] adj = .01)
F = (8,72) = 1.09,
p = .384
Married Partner .07 .09 *
Unmarried Partner .19 .34 *
Grandmother -.01 -.01 *
Step 2 [R.sup.2]change = .00,
F change (3,69) = .05,
p = .985
Overall Model [R.sup.2] = .11 ([R.sup.2] adj = -.03)
F = (3,69) = .77,
p = .667
* Standardized Beta Coefficients are not used with dummy coded
variables
References
Abidin, R. R. (1995). Parenting Stress Index: Professional Manual
(3ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
Achenbach, T. M. (1997). Manual for the Young Adult Self-Report and
Young Adult Behavior Checklist. Department of Psychiatry, University of
Vermont.
Acock, A. C. (1997). Working with missing values. Family Science
Review, 10(1), 76-102.
Allen, W. R. (1978). The search for applicable theories in black
family life. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 40, 117-129.
Andersen, L. M. (2000). Thinking About Women: Sociological
Perspectives on Sex and Gender (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Baca Zinn, M. & Eitzen, D. S. (1999). Diversity in American
Families. New York: Addison Wesley Longman.
Barbarin, A. O. (1993). Coping and resilience: Exploring the inner
lives of african american children. Journal of Black Psychology, 19(4),
478-492.
Barbarin, A. O. & Soler, E. R. (1993). Behavioral, emotional,
and academic adjustment in a national probability sample of african
american children: Effects of age, gender, and family structure. Journal
of Black Psychology, 19(4), 423-446.
Bavolek, S. J. (1984). Handbook for the AAPI Adult-Adolescent
Parenting Inventory. Park City, UT: Family Development Resources, Inc.
Bavolek, S. J. & Keene, R. G. (1999). Adult-Adolescent
Parenting Inventory (AAPI-2) Administration and Development Handbook.
Asheville, NC: Family Development Resources Inc.
Benasich, A. A. & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1996). Maternal attitudes
and knowledge of child-rearing: Associations with family and child
outcomes. Child Development, 67(3), 1186-1205.
Berlin, L. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., Spiker, D., Zaslow, M J. (1995).
Examining observational measures of emotional support and cognitive
stimulation in black and white mothers of preschoolers. Journal of
Family Issues, 16(5), 664-686.
Bradley, R. C. (1998a). Child discipline in African American
families: Scholars must question the generalizability of poor research
[9pp]. Family Journal [Online serial], 6(4). Available: ProQuest Direct
[1999, Sept. 18].
Bradley, R. C. (1998b). Child rearing in african american families:
A study of the disciplinary practices of african american parents.
Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development, 26, 273-281.
Bradley, R. C. (1998c). Cultural interpretations of child
discipline: Voices of African American scholars [46pp]. Family Journal
[Online serial], 6(4). Available: ProQuest Direct [1999, Sept. 9].
Bradley, R. H. (1994). The Home Inventory: Review and reflections.
Advances in Child Development, 25, 241-288.
Bradley, R. H., Caldwell, B. M., Rock, S. L, Barnard, K, E., Gray,
C., Hammond, M. A., Mitchell, S., Siegel, L., Ramey, C. T., Gottfried,
A. W., & Johnson, D. L. (1989). Home environment and cognitive
development in the first 3 years of life: A collaborative study
involving six sites and three ethnic groups in North America.
Developmental Psychology, 25(2), 217-235.
Bradley, R. H., Mundfrom, D. J., Whiteside, L., Casey, P. H.,
Barrett, K. (1994). A factor analytic study of the infant-toddler and
early childhood versions of the HOME inventory administered to white,
black, and Hispanic American parents of children born preterm. Child
Development, 65, 880-888.
Brody, H. G., Flor, L. D. & Neubaum, E. (1998). Coparenting
processes and child competence among rural african-american families. In
M. Lewis & C. Feiring (Eds.), Families, Risk, and Competence (pp.
227-244). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Caldwell, B. & Bradley, R. H. (1984). Home Observation for
Measurement of the Environment. Little Rock: Center for Research on
Teaching and Learning, University of Arkansas.
Chase-Lansdale, L. P., Brooks-Gunn, J. & Zamsky, E. S. (1994).
Young african-american multigenerational families in poverty: Quality of
mothering and grandmothering. Child Development, 65, 373-393.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral
sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cohen, J. & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied Multiple Regression
Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed., pp. 172-176).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Association Publications.
Combs-Orme, T., Cain, D.S., Wilson, E. E. (2004). Do maternal
concerns at delivery predict parenting stress during infancy? Child
Abuse and Neglect, 28(4), 377-392.
Combs-Orme, T., Wilson, E. E., Cain, D. S., Page, T., & Kirby,
L. D. (2003). Context-based parenting in infancy: Background and
conceptual issues. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 20(6),
437-472.
Davis, D. W. (1997). The direction of race of interviewer effects
among African-Americans: donning the black mask. American Journal of
Political Science, 41(1), 309-323.
Franklin, A. J., & Boyd-Franklin, N. (1985). A
psychoeducational perspective on black parenting. In H. P. McAdoo &
J. L. McAdoo (Eds.), Black Children: Social, Educational & Parental
Environments. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.
Garcia-Coll, C., Lamberty, G., Jenkins, R., McAdoo, P. H., Crnic,
K., Wasik, H. B., & Garcia, V. H. (1996). An integrative model for
the study of developmental competencies in minority children. Child
Development, 67, 1891-1914.
Garcia-Coll, C. T., Meyer, E. C., & Brillon, L. (1995). Ethnic
and minority parenting. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of Parenting:
Biology and Ecology of Parenting (Vol. 2, pp. 189-209). Hillsdale, NJ:
Earlbaum.
George, C., & Solomon, J. (1999). Attachment and caregiving:
The caregiving behavioral system. In J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (Eds.),
Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications (pp.
649-670). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
hooks, b. (1984). Feminist Theory: From Margin to Center. Boston,
MA: South End Press.
Hunter, A. G. (1997). Counting on grandmothers: Black mothers'
and fathers' reliance on grandmothers for parenting support.
Journal of Family Issues, 18(3), 251-269.
Hunter, A. G., Pearson, J. L., Ialongo, N. S., & Kellam, S. G.
(1998). Parenting alone to multiple caregivers: Child care and parenting
arrangements in black and white urban families. Family Relations, 47,
343-353.
Hutcheson, J. J. & Black, M. M. (1996). Psychometric properties
of the parenting stress index in a sample of low-income African-American
mothers of infants and toddlers. Early Education and Development, 7(4),
381-400.
Jackson, A. P. (1998). The role of social support in parenting for
low-income, single, black mothers. Social Service Review, 72, 365-379.
Jackson, F. J. (1993). Multiple caregiving among African americans
and infant attachment: The need for an emic approach. Human Development,
36, 87-102.
Jackson, A. P., Gyamfi, P., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Blake, M.
(1998). Employment status, psychological well-being, social support, and
physical discipline practices of single black mothers [41pp]. Journal of
Marriage and the Family [Online serial], 60(4). Available: ProQuest
Direct [1999, Sept. 18].
McAdoo, H. P. (1978). Factors related to stability in upwardly
mobile black families. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 40, 761-776.
McLoyd, C. V. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on black
families and children: Psychological distress, parenting, and
socioemotional development. Child Development, 61, 311-346.
McLoyd, C. V. & Randolph, M. S. (1984). The conduct and
publication of research on afro-american children: A content analysis.
Human Development, 27, 6575.
Murray, V. M., Bynum, M. S., Brody, G. H., Willert, A. &
Stephens, D. (2001). African American single mothers and children in
context: A review of studies on risk and resilience. Clinical Child and
Family Psychology Review, 4(2), 133-155.
Myers, H. F., Rana, P. G., & Harris, M. (1979). Black Child
Development in America, 1927-1977. Westport, CT: Greenwood.
Nunnally, J. C. & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric Theory
(3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Parker, G., Tupling, H. & Brown, B. L. (1979). A parental
bonding instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 32, 1-10.
Pearson, L. J., Hunter, G. A., Ensminger, E. M., & Kellam, G.
S. (1990). Black grandmothers in multigenerational households: Diversity
in family structure and parenting involvement in the woodlawn community.
Child Development, 61, 434-442.
Randolph, M. S. (1995). African american children in single-mother
families, in B. J. Dickerson (Ed.), African American Single Mothers:
Understanding Their Lives and Families (pp. 117-145). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage Publications.
Raymond, J. H., Jones, F. & Cooke, V. (1998). African american
scholars and parents cannot blame current harsh physical punishment of
black males on slavery: A response to "cultural interpretations of
childellipsis" [55pp]. Family Journal [Online serial], 6(4).
Available: ProQuest Direct [1999, Sept. 181.
Rhodes, P. J. (1994). Race-of-interviewer effects: a brief comment.
Sociology, 28(2), 547-559.
Solomon, J. & George, C. (1996). Defining the caregiving
system: Toward a theory of caregiving. Infant Mental Health Journal,
17(3), 183-197.
Spencer, M. B. (1990). Development of minority children: An
introduction. Child Development, 61, 267-269.
Tolson, J. F. T., & Wilson, N. M. (1990). The impact of two-
and three-generational black family structure on perceived family
climate. Child Development, 61, 416-428.
Wakschlag, S. L., Chase-Lansdale, L. P., & Brooks-Gunn, J.
(1996). Not just "ghosts in the nursery": Contemporaneous intergenerational relationships and parenting in young african-american
families. Child Development, 67, 2131-2147.
Washington, E., & McLoyd, V. C. (1982). The external validity
of research involving American minorities. Human Development, 25,
324-339.
Wilson, M. N. (1989). Child development in the context of the black
extended family. American Psychologist, 44(2), 380-385.
Wilson, M. N., Kohn, L. P., Curry-El, J. & Hinton, I. D.
(1995). The influence of family structure characteristics on the
child-rearing behaviors of African-American mothers. Journal of Black
Psychology, 21(4), 450-462.
Young, H. V. (1970). Family and childhood in a southern negro
community. American Anthropologist, 72, 269-288.
DAPHNE S. CAIN
Louisiana State University
TERRI COMBS-ORME
University of Tennessee