首页    期刊浏览 2024年11月08日 星期五
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Predictors of critical thinking skills of incoming business students.
  • 作者:Whitten, Donna ; Brahmasrene, Tantatape
  • 期刊名称:Academy of Educational Leadership Journal
  • 印刷版ISSN:1095-6328
  • 出版年度:2011
  • 期号:January
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:The DreamCatchers Group, LLC
  • 摘要:The promotion of critical thinking ranks among the primary goals for educators today (Elder, 2004). As reported in a review of literature by the Office of Outcomes Assessment of the University of Maryland in 2006, critical thinking as an outcome of postsecondary education was made explicit by several recent national reports (Association of American Colleges and Universities, 1985; National Education Goals Panel, 1991; National Institute of Education Study Group, 1984). As such, the topic of critical thinking is of interest to educators. Various definitions of critical thinking have been offered. They all share a common set of meanings. Critical thinking refers to the use of cognitive skills or strategies and involves solving problems, formulating inferences, calculating likelihoods, and making decisions. According to the manual for the California Critical Thinking Skills Test (CCTST) developed by Peter and Noreen Facione, an important consensus with regard to the concept of critical thinking was announced in 1990 by a panel of theoreticians drawn from throughout the United States and Canada representing several academic fields. These experts characterized critical thinking as the process of purposeful, self-regulatory judgment (Facione, 1990). Critical thinking, so defined, is the cognitive engine which drives problem-solving and decision-making. At the core of critical thinking are the cognitive skills of reasoning, evaluation, analysis and inference.
  • 关键词:Business students;College admissions;Critical thinking;Engineering schools;Students;Teachers

Predictors of critical thinking skills of incoming business students.


Whitten, Donna ; Brahmasrene, Tantatape


INTRODUCTION

The promotion of critical thinking ranks among the primary goals for educators today (Elder, 2004). As reported in a review of literature by the Office of Outcomes Assessment of the University of Maryland in 2006, critical thinking as an outcome of postsecondary education was made explicit by several recent national reports (Association of American Colleges and Universities, 1985; National Education Goals Panel, 1991; National Institute of Education Study Group, 1984). As such, the topic of critical thinking is of interest to educators. Various definitions of critical thinking have been offered. They all share a common set of meanings. Critical thinking refers to the use of cognitive skills or strategies and involves solving problems, formulating inferences, calculating likelihoods, and making decisions. According to the manual for the California Critical Thinking Skills Test (CCTST) developed by Peter and Noreen Facione, an important consensus with regard to the concept of critical thinking was announced in 1990 by a panel of theoreticians drawn from throughout the United States and Canada representing several academic fields. These experts characterized critical thinking as the process of purposeful, self-regulatory judgment (Facione, 1990). Critical thinking, so defined, is the cognitive engine which drives problem-solving and decision-making. At the core of critical thinking are the cognitive skills of reasoning, evaluation, analysis and inference.

RELATED LITERATURE

Previous studies have used the CCTST to measure critical thinking (Williams, 2003; Zettergren, 2004; Colucciello, 2005; Yang, 2008). Scores are included on the following skills: inductive and deductive reasoning, evaluation, analysis and inference. Inductive reasoning and deductive reasoning were scored on the CCTST. Induction is usually described as moving from the specific to the general while deduction begins with the general and ends with the specific. In the case of a strong inductive argument it is unlikely or improbable that the conclusion would actually be false and all the premises true, but it is logically possible that it might. Arguments based on experience or observations are expressed inductively since inductive reasoning is based on making a conclusion based on a set of empirical data. If it is observed that something is true many times, concluding that it will be true in all instances is a use of inductive reasoning. Deductive reasoning allows proof the hypothesis is true. For valid deductive arguments, it is not logically possible for the conclusion to be false and all the premises true. For example, deductive reasoning begins with a general rule, which is known to be true. From that general rule a conclusion is made about something specific.

The evaluation score on the CCTST measures the results of an individual's reasoning. The justification of that reasoning in terms of the evidential, conceptual, methodological, criteriological and contextual considerations is also measured. Evaluation involves examining, appraising and judging something carefully. It is the process of examining a system or system component to determine the extent to which specified properties are present. Evaluation is the systematic determination of merit, worth, and significance of something or someone.

The CCTST score on Analysis measures the ability to identify the intended and actual inferential relationships intended to express beliefs, judgments, experiences, reasons, information or opinions. This includes the sub-skills of examining ideas, detecting arguments, and analyzing arguments into their component elements. Analysis is the process of breaking a complex topic or substance into smaller parts to gain a better understanding of it. Perhaps, in its broadest sense, it might be defined as a process of isolating or working back to what is more fundamental by means of which something, initially taken as given, can be explained or reconstructed. This process is a method of studying the nature of something or of determining its essential features and their relations. Analysis involves detailed examination of the elements to understand them, separation of those elements to examine the individual parts and assessment based on careful consideration of those elements.

Inference is the process of arriving at some conclusion that, though it is not logically derivable from the assumed premises, possesses some degree of probability relative to the premises. The CCTST score on Inference measures the ability to draw a conclusion or making a logical judgment based on circumstantial evidence and prior conclusions rather than on the basis of direct observation. In other words, inference is the act or process of deriving a conclusion based solely on what one already knows. It is the act of deriving one idea from another. Inferences can be valid or invalid and can proceed through either deductive reasoning or inductive reasoning.

Much research has been conducted on predictors of success in academia. Burton and Ramist published a report in 2001 on some of the studies predicting the success of students in college. The conclusion was that scholastic aptitude test (SAT) scores and high school records of grade point average (GPA) and high school rank in class were the most common predictors (Burton & Ramist, 2001).

Previous studies included Class/Year in School (Kealey, Holland & Watson, 2005; Lampert, 2007). Kealey, Holland and Watson (2005) observed that class/year in school was not significant in predicting performance in a course while in Lampert (2007)'s research this variable was significant in predicting critical thinking scores.

Bridgeman, Burton and Pollack found in 2008 that High School GPA was significant as a predictor of college GPA. Ventura (2005) determined that High School Rank was significant as an academic predictor and Baron determined in 1992 that it was a significant predictor of college grades. Troutman (1978) indicated that high school rank was a predictor of performance in freshman mathematics. Math is logic based and therefore draws on various types of critical thinking.

SAT Scores were significant as academic predictors in studies by Ventura (2005), Bridgeman, Burton and Pollack (2008) and Baron (1992). Osana, Lacroix, Tucker, Idan and Jabbour (2007) indicated that verbal ability is strongly related to syllogistic reasoning, which is evaluating whether a conclusion necessarily follows from two premises. This is consistent with findings by Quinlin (1989) which discussed the relationship between mathematics and reflective thinking and inference. In addition, in 2007 Cavanagh discovered that students who scored high on the math portion of the SAT had greater career accomplishments in fields related to science, technology, engineering and mathematics. Finally, Stylianides and Stylianides (2008) discuss the link between deductive reasoning and mathematics. Fields like engineering rely heavily on the type of processes involved in critical thinking (Niewoehner, 2008; Ceylon & Lang, 2003).

Gender was included in studies by Ventura in 2005 and Kealey, Holland and Watson in 2005 and was not significant as an academic predictor. Race was included as a variable to contribute information not collected in previous studies. Major was significant as a predictor of performance in a course by Kealey, Holland and Watson (2005) however, Lampert ascertained it was not significant in predicting critical thinking scores.

The idea that critical thinking can and should be taught was not embraced by all initially, including Glaser (1984) and other skeptics, because many believed it was a misguided effort. They argued that thinking skills were context-bound and do not transfer across academic domains. However, a paper published by Halpern in 1999 noted studies of successful instruction in critical thinking that included work conducted by Rubinstein and Firstenberg (1987), Lochhead and Whimby (1987), and Wood (1987). Successful methods of teaching critical thinking include practice and teaching critical thinking for transfer from one situation to another (Van Gelder, 2005; Brahmasrene & Osisek, 2003; Willingham, 2007).

HYPOTHESIS

The above literature review leads to the hypothesis that the total critical thinking score and its components such as inductive reasoning, deductive reasoning, evaluation, analysis, and inference are affected by college classification (class/year in school), high school GPA, high school rank, SAT verbal scores, SAT mathematical scores (math), gender, race and major. These scores are directly proportional to all independent variables except gender and major. Gender is a dummy variable where 0 and 1 represent male and female, respectively. This means the likelihood of being male increases the critical thinking scores, and vice versa. Major is a dummy variable where 0 represents business major and 1 for non-business major.

For empirical analysis, the models have been constructed as shown below:

CT = CONSTANT + [b.sub.1] CLASS + [b.sub.2] HSGPA + [b.sub.3] HSRANK + [b.sub.4] VERBAL + [b.sub.5] MATH + [b.sub.6] GENDER + [b.sub.7] RACE + [b.sub.8] MAJOR + [u.sub.i]

CT represents the total critical thinking, evaluation, analysis, inference, inductive reasoning and deductive reasoning scores. Description of the variables is summarized in Table 1. [u.sub.i] is a stochastic error term or disturbance term.

DATA

The California Critical Thinking Skills Test (CCTST) developed by Insight Assessment was administered to students during 2004-2006 academic years in an introductory accounting course. The dispersed target group helps eliminate selection bias. The test completion rate was about 77.5 percent or 483 forms completed out of 623 students, resulting in 300 usable forms after collecting high school and SAT data. Table 2 provides descriptive statistics of the total critical thinking, evaluation, analysis, inference, inductive reasoning and deductive reasoning scores. These are scale variables where differences between values are comparable. A mean total critical thinking of 15.24 out of 34 possible points or 44.82 percent suggests the respondents in this study are performing at a similar level as the group means of 15.89 provided by Insight Assessment Technical Report number 4, which makes the CCTST available (Facione, 1990). In addition, a previous study by Williams in 2003 reported means of 15.38 and 16.62. The mean scores of inductive and deductive reasoning are 8.42 or 49.53 percent and 6.82 or 40.12 percent, respectively. Evaluation, analysis and inference scores show the averages of 3.93 or 28.07 percent, 3.92 or 43.55 percent, and 7.39 or 67.18 percent, respectively. Most students in this study are freshmen and sophomore (averaged 1.8 out of 4 classifications). Their average high school GPA was 2.96 with 0.584 of high school ranked. This means participants were right above the upper half of their class. The SAT verbal scores were 468.96 compared with the 2006 Indiana average of 498 and national average of 503. The SAT mathematical scores were 492.96 compared with the 2006 Indiana average of 509 and national average of 518. Gender, race and major are nominal variables where the variable values do not have a natural ranking. Their frequencies are reported in Table 3. About 52.7 percent or 158 out of 300 participants are female while 47.3 percent (142/300) are male. When asked how they identify themselves, 82 percent or 246 out of 300 valid cases indicated Anglo American or Caucasian while 18 percent (54/300) are others. Regarding major, 56 percent or 168 out of 300 are business majors. The rest, 44 percent (132/300) are non-business majors.

METHODOLOGY

The ordinary least square (OLS) method was employed to test the above hypotheses. One of the tasks in performing regression analysis with several independent variables was to calculate a correlation matrix for all variables. Table 4 reports the Pearson Correlations for all about herein dependent variables. There were no particularly large intercorrelations among independent variables except for high school GPA and high school rank. High school GPA was eliminated to avoid multicollinearity problem. However, a measure of multicollinearity among independent variables would be performed.

RESULTS

The assumption of linear multiple regression and the fitness of the model was tested. According to the computed values of a multiple regression model, the null hypothesis was rejected at a significant level of less than 0.01 (F test) in all models as shown in Table 5 and 6. This means that among these estimated equations, there existed a relationship between critical thinking scores (total, inductive reasoning, deductive reasoning, evaluation, analysis and inference) and the explanatory variables: years in college (classification), high school GPA, high school rank, SAT verbal scores, SAT mathematical scores, gender, race and major. The coefficient of multiple determination (R Square) of models in Table 5 varied from 0.22 to 0.41 which are comparable to similar studies. Kealey, Holland and Watson studied the association between critical thinking and performance in principles of accounting and reported an adjusted R Square of .31 (Kealey, Holland & Watson, 2005). Lampert used analysis of variance in a study of critical thinking dispositions as na outcome of undergraduate education and reported an adjusted R Squared of .05 (Lampert, 2007).

Note that R Square is a measure of goodness of fit. R Square of zero does not mean that there is no association among the variables. Two variables, gender and major, had no significant influence on all six critical thinking models. Therefore, gender and major were omitted. Only significant variables were included in the final models. The final results are shown in Table 6. The F test shows significant level of less than 0.01 in all six models. The variance inflation factor (VIF) is also presented to detect multicollinearity among independent variables. A value of VIF less than 10 generally indicates no presence of multicollinearity. It appears that the observed dependencies did not affect their coefficients.

Furthermore, significant test (t-test) for all critical thinking models show the coefficients of independent variables with varying degrees of significant t-value ([alpha] < 0.01, 0.05 and 0.10), all with expected positive signs. Therefore, the null hypothesis of years in college (class), high school rank, SAT verbal, mathematical scores, and race was rejected as shown in Table 6. Class variable was significant ([alpha] < 0.05) for total scores, deductive reasoning and inference, but marginally significant ([alpha] < 0.10) for inductive reasoning. Class had no significant impact on evaluation and analysis scores. High school rank highly and significantly ([alpha] < 0.01) affected total and inference scores while significantly ([alpha] < 0.05) affected inductive, deductive reasoning, evaluation and analysis scores. SAT verbal scores had a highly significant t-value ([alpha] < 0.01) on all models except deductive reasoning where it was significant with t-value ([alpha] < 0.05). However, SAT mathematical scores had highly significant t-value ([alpha] < 0.01) on the total critical thinking score, inductive reasoning, deductive reasoning while having significant t-value ([alpha] < 0.05) on analysis and marginally significant t-value ([alpha] < 0.10) on evaluation. It had no effect on inference scores. With respect to the race variable, race had a significant t-value ([alpha] < 0.05) only on the total critical thinking score while a marginally significant t-value ([alpha] < 0.10) on inductive reasoning.

CONTRIBUTIONS

This paper makes three important contributions and supports previous studies. First, since Class/Year in School was significant for all measures of critical thinking except Evaluative and Analytical, different measures of critical thinking may develop over a student's academic career. It may be that these measures of critical thinking develop later in students academic experiences. Therefore studies of students further in their academic careers may be of interest. High school rank was significant for all measures of critical thinking. This is consistent with previous studies by Ventura (2005) and Baron (1992). They included high school rank and determined that it is positively significant as an academic predictor.

The second important contribution was a result of examining the SAT math and verbal scores separately. In doing so it revealed that inferential thinking was predicted by verbal scores but not math scores. Verbal score on the SAT were significant for all measures of critical thinking. Students that have high verbal skills may be able to perform better on critical thinking skills test due to their high verbal ability. The math score on the SAT was significant for total score, inductive, deductive, evaluative and analytical reasoning. It would be interesting to do further research on why math skills are significant for only these measures of critical thinking and not inferential thinking.

The third important contribution was the inclusion of race which was significant for total score and inductive thinking but not for other measure of critical thinking. It may be due to Caucasian students have greater access to academic resources such as home computers and the internet. This may influence performance in terms of total score. However, further research may need to be conducted to determine reasons for the significance in terms of inductive reasoning versus other measures of critical thinking.

PRACTICAL IMPLICATIONS

Measures of critical thinking skills are used for assessment purposes, including self-assessment for accreditation. In addition, information on critical thinking skills is useful to designers of curriculum. While critical thinking is difficult to teach, there is a need to teach thinking skills at all levels of education. As Carr (1990) and Willingham (2007) state in their articles on teaching critical thinking, it should not be taught on its own or by relying on special courses and text. Instead, every teacher should create an atmosphere where students are encouraged to read deeply, question, engage in divergent thinking, look for relationships among ideas, and grapple with real life issues (Carr, 1990).

A study by Yazici (2004) indicated that collaborative learning enhances critical thinking skills. Therefore studies that include group work as a teaching strategy for critical thinking may be valuable. Ishiyama, McClure, Hart and Amico in 1999 found no significant difference in critical thinking disposition and evaluation of teaching strategy lending support to utilizing methods of instruction the enhance critical thinking skills. Williams (2003) indicated that critical thinking was the strongest indicator of multiple-choice examination performance. This has implications for educators as they develop curriculum and measurement instruments, especially for assessment purposes. As Peach, Mukherjee, and Hornyak (2007) noted, critical thinking is recognized as important but difficult to assess. However, it is an essential component for assessment and institutions must participate in assessing and developing it.

CONCLUSION

This article provides identification of several important directions for future research. A focus on different evaluation methods may reveal of interest. In addition, measuring infusion of critical thinking would be valuable. Demonstrating critical thinking skills in the classroom and then observing the effectiveness would be of interest to educators since it is understood that teaching critical thinking it is difficult. Finally, research that focuses on conveying the importance and power of critical thinking to students may determine whether it generates interest. This may provide information educators can use to improve their ability to teach critical thinking skills.

REFERENCES

Baron, J. (1992). SAT's, Achievement tests and High-school rank as Predictors of College Performance. Educational & Psychological Measurement, 52 (4) 1047-1055.

Brahmasrene , T. & Osisek, P. (2003). Infusing Marginal Thinking into a Principles of Economics Course. Midwestern Business and Economic Review, 31, 22-23.

Bridgeman , B., Burton, N. & Pollack, J. (2008). Predicting Grades in College Courses. Journal of College Admission, Spring, 20-25.

Burton , N. & Ramist, L. (2001). Predicting Success in College: SAT Studies of Classes Graduating Since 1980. The College Board, Research Report No. 2001-2, 1-2.

Carr, K. (1990). How can we Teach Critical Thinking? ERIC Digest adapted from Childhood Education, Winter 1988, 69-73.

Cavabagh, S. (2007). SAT Scores Linked to Adult Success. Education Week, 27 (4).

Ceylon , T. & Lang, W.L. (2003). Critical Thinking and Engineering Education. Conference Paper Presented at American Society for Engineering Education at Valparaiso University, IN. April 4-5, 2003, 41-43.

Colucciello, M. (2005). Relationships Between Critical Thinking Dispositions and Learning Styles. Journal of Professional Nursing, 15 (5) 294-301.

Elder, L. (2004). The State of Critical Thinking Today: The need for a Substantive Concept of Critical Thinking. http:// www.criticalthinking.org/resources/articles/the-state-ct-today.shtml.

Facione, P. (1990). Critical Thinking: A Statement of Expert Consensus for Purposes of Educational Assessment and Instruction. The Complete American Philosophical Association Delphi Report, The California Academic Press, ERIC Doc. No.: ED 315 423, 1-3.

Facione, P. (1990). The California Critical Thinking Skills Test: College Level Technical Report #4--Interpreting the CCTST, Group Norms and Sub-Scores. The California Academic Press, ERIC Doc. No.: ED 327-566, 4-11.

Glaser, R. (1984). Education and Thinking: The Role of Knowledge. American Psychologist, 3, 93-104.

Halpern, D. (1999). Teaching for Critical thinking: Helping College Students Develop the Skills and Dispositions of a Critical Thinker. New Directions for Teaching and Learning, 80 Winter 1999, 69-70.

Ishiyama, J., McClure, M., Hart, H., & Amico, J. (1999). Critical Thinking Disposition and Locus of Control as Predictors of Evaluations of Teaching Strategies. College Student Journal, 33 (2) 272-273.

Kealey, B., Holland, J., & Watson, M. (2005). Preliminary Evidence on the Association Between Critical Thinking and Performance in Principles of Accounting. Issues in Accounting Education, 20 (1) 39-44.

Lampert, N. (2007). Critical Thinking Dispositions as an Outcome of Undergraduate Education. Journal of General Education, 56 (1) 21-28.

Lockhead , J. & Whimby, A. (1987). Teaching Analytic Reasoning Through Think-Aloud Pair Problem Solving. New Directions for Teaching and Learning, no. 30. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Niewoehner, R.J. (2008). Applying a Critical Thinking Model for Engineering Education. The Thinker's Guide to Engineering Reasoning, United States Naval Academy, 341-344.

Office of Outcomes Assessment University of Maryland University College. (2006). Critical Thinking as a Core Academic Skill: A Review of Literature, Spring 1-11.

Osana, H., Lacroix. G., Tucker, B., Idan, E. & Jabbour, G. (2007), Development of Interplay Between Automatic Processes and Cognitive Resources in Reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99 (4) 898-899.

Peach, B., Mukherjee. A., & Hornyak, M. (2007), Assessing Critical Thinking: A College's Journey and Lessons Learned. Journal of Education for Business, 22, (July/Aug) 313-318.

Quinlin, C. (1989). Inferential Thinking for Mathematics Teachers. The Australian Mathematics Teacher, 45 (4) 2022.

Rubinstein, M. F., & Firstenberg, I. R. (1987), Tools for Thinking. New Directions for Teaching and Learning, no. 30. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Stylianides, G., & Stylianides. A (2008), Proof in School Mathematics: Insights from Psychological Research into Students' Ability for Deductive Reasoning. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 10 (2) 103-105.

Troutman, J. (1978). Cognitive Predictors of Final Grades in Finite Mathematics. Educational & Psychological Measurement, 38 (2) 401-404.

Van Gelder, T. (2005). Teaching Critical Thinking: Some lessons from Cognitive Science. College Teaching, 53 (1) 42-43.

Ventura, P. (2005). Identifying Predictors of Success for an Objects-First CS1. Computer Science Education, 15 (3) 227-240.

Willingham, D. (2007). Critical Thinking: Why is it so Hard to Teach. American Educator, Summer 2007 10-11.

Williams, R. (2003). Critical thinking as a Predictor and Outcome Measure in a Large Undergraduate Educational Psychology Course. Conference Presentation, The American Education Research Association, Chicago Il 7-19.

Williams, R. (2003). Thinking Skills and Work Habits: Contributors to course Performance. Journal of General Education, 51 (3) 200-201.

Yang, Y. (2008). A Catalyst for Teaching Critical Thinking in a Large University Class in Taiwan: Asynchronous Online Discussions with the Facilitation of Teaching Assistants. Educational Technology Research & Development, 56 (3) 241-264.

Yazici, H. (2004). Student Perceptions of Collaborative Learning in Operations Management Classes. Journal of Education for Business, November/December 111-117.

Zettergren, K. (2004). Changes in Critical Thinking Scores: An Examination of One Group of Physical Therapist Students. Journal of Physical Therapy Education, Fall 1-2.

Donna Whitten, Purdue University North Central

Tantatape Brahmasrene, Purdue University North Central
Table 1: Description of Variables

Dependent variables

TOTALCT    Total score on all 34 questions. Total score on inductive
           and deductive reasoning questions. Total score on
           evaluation, analysis and inference questions.

INDUCT     Inductive Reasoning--Starting with a specific hypothesis
           and moving to a general rule by making a conclusion based
           on a set of empirical data. An example of inductive
           reasoning is scientific confirmation and experimental
           disconfirmation.

DEDUCT     Deductive Reasoning--Begins with a general rule, which we
           know to be true, and ends with the specific conclusion. An
           example of deductive reasoning is geometric proofs in
           mathematics.

EVAL       Evaluation--The systematic determination of merit, worth,
           and significance of something or someone.

ANALY      Analysis--Analysis involves detailed examination of the
           elements to understand them, separation of those elements
           to examine the individual parts and assessment based on
           careful consideration of those elements.

INFER      Inference--The process of deriving a conclusion based
           solely on what one already knows.

Independent variables

CLASS      College classification, 1-4

HSGPA      High school GPA

HSRANK     High school rank

VERBAL     SAT verbal score

MATH       SAT math score

GENDER     0 = male
           1 = female

RACE       0 = other
           1 = Anglo American, Caucasian

MAJOR      0 = business majors
           1 = non-business majors

Table 2: Descriptive Statistics

                                                                Std.
                      N     Minimum    Maximum      Mean     Deviation

TOTALCT              300       5          28       15.24       4.399
INDUCT               300       2          15        8.42       2.457
DEDUCT               300       1          14        6.82       2.570
EVAL                 300       0          10        3.93       1.957
ANALY                300       1          6         3.92       1.200
INFER                300       1          13        7.39       2.510
CLASS                300       0          4         1.80        .804
HSGPA                300      1.14       4.31       2.96        .800
HSRANK               292      .032      1.000       .584        .234
VERBAL               259      230        760       468.96      83.181
MATH                 259      270        760       492.96      86.461
Valid (listwise)     252

Table 3: Frequency

                                        Valid    Cumulative
GENDER           Frequency   Percent   Percent    Percent

Valid      0        142       47.3      47.3        47.3
           1        158       52.7      52.7       100.0
         Total      300       100.0     100.0

RACE

Valid     .00       54        18.0      18.0        18.0
         1.00       246       82.0      82.0       100.0
         Total      300       100.0     100.0

MAJOR

Valid     .00       168       56.0      56.0        56.0
         1.00       132       44.0      44.0       100.0
         Total      300       100.0     100.0

Table 4: Correlations

                                  CLASS        HSGPA        HSRANK

CLASS     Pearson Correlation       1          -.089        -.086
          Sig. (2-tailed)                       .123         .144
          N                        300          300          292

HSGPA     Pearson Correlation     -.089          1         .590(**)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .123                      .000
          N                        300          300          292

HSRANK    Pearson Correlation     -.086       .590(**)        1
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .144         .000
          N                        292          292          292

VERBAL    Pearson Correlation      .015       .293(**)     .390(**)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .806         .000         .000
          N                        259          259          252

MATH      Pearson Correlation     -.021       .253(**)     .434(**)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .741         .000         .000
          N                        259          259          252

GENDER    Pearson Correlation      .067      -.167(**)    -.192(**)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .246         .004         .001
          N                        300          300          292

RACE      Pearson Correlation      .047         .092       .132(*)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .414         .113         .024
          N                        300          300          292

MAJOR     Pearson Correlation   -.168(**)     .116(*)      .271(**)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .004         .045         .000
          N                        300          300          292

                                  VERBAL        MATH        GENDER

CLASS     Pearson Correlation      .015        -.021         .067
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .806         .741         .246
          N                        259          259          300

HSGPA     Pearson Correlation    .293(**)     .253(**)    -.167(**)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .000         .000         .004
          N                        259          259          300

HSRANK    Pearson Correlation    .390(**)     .434(**)    -.192(**)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .000         .000         .001
          N                        252          252          292

VERBAL    Pearson Correlation       1         .655(**)       .051
          Sig. (2-tailed)                       .000         .413
          N                        259          259          259

MATH      Pearson Correlation    .655(**)        1           .087
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .000                      .162
          N                        259          259          259

GENDER    Pearson Correlation      .051         .087          1
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .413         .162
          N                        259          259          300

RACE      Pearson Correlation      .088         .012         .008
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .157         .842         .895
          N                        259          259          300

MAJOR     Pearson Correlation    .167(**)     .135(*)     -.195(**)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .007         .029         .001
          N                        259          259          300

                                   RACE        MAJOR

CLASS     Pearson Correlation      .047      -.168(**)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .414         .004
          N                        300          300

HSGPA     Pearson Correlation      .092       .116(*)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .113         .045
          N                        300          300

HSRANK    Pearson Correlation    .132(*)      .271(**)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .024         .000
          N                        292          292

VERBAL    Pearson Correlation      .088       .167(**)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .157         .007
          N                        259          259

MATH      Pearson Correlation      .012       .135(*)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .842         .029
          N                        259          259

GENDER    Pearson Correlation      .008      -.195(**)
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .895         .001
          N                        300          300

RACE      Pearson Correlation       1           .031
          Sig. (2-tailed)                       .596
          N                        300          300

MAJOR     Pearson Correlation      .031          1
          Sig. (2-tailed)          .596
          N                        300          300

Note

** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

* Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

Table 5: Coefficients of Preliminary Models

                    TOTALCT           INDUCT            DEDUCT

Constant          -2.978 ***           .198             -3.177
                   (-1.953)           (.207)           (-3.464)

CLASS               .594 **            .280             .315 *
                    (2.115)           (1.586)           (1.861)

HSRANK             3.278 ***         1.639 **          1.639 ***
                    (2.978)           (2.373)           (2.475)

VERBAL             .012 ***          .007 ***           .005 **
                    (3.360)           (3.204)           (2.242)

MATH               .018 ***          .006 ***          .012 ***
                    (5.050)           (2.800)           (5.475)

GENDER            .203 (.436)      -.253 (-.867)     .456 (1.629)

RACE                .981 *            .716 **            .265
                    (1.738)           (2.021)           (.781)

MAJOR                -.402             -.416             .014
                    (-.860)          (-1.420)           (.051)

R Square             0.409             0.271             0.372

F Statistics      24.140 ***        12.987 ***        20.637 ***

                     EVAL              ANALY             INFER

Constant            -1.856             .375             -1.498
                   (-2.351)           (.794)           (-1.585)

CLASS                .188              .044             .362 **
                    (1.289)           (.509)            (2.080)

HSRANK             1.325 **           .701 **           1.252 *
                    (2.324)           (2.055)           (1.836)

VERBAL             .006 ***           004 ***          .002 ***
                    (3.008)           (3.865)           (.975)

MATH                .004 **           .002 **            .012
                    (1.977)           (2.036)           (5.479)

GENDER           -.017 (-.071)     -.005 (-.033)      .225 (.779)

RACE                 .440              -.121            .662 *
                    (1.505)           (-.692)           (1.893)

MAJOR                -.192             .075              -.285
                    (-.795)           (.521)            (-.984)

R Square             0.215             0.250             0.301

F Statistics       9.528 ***        11.581 ***        14.976 ***

Notes

t statistics are in parentheses.

Significant level : * 0.10, ** 0.05, ***0.01

Table 6: Coefficients of Final Models

                   TOTALCT           INDUCT           DEDUCT

Constant          -2.978 **           .122          -2.904 ***
                   (-1.960)          (.127)          (-3.280)

CLASS              .639 **           .304 *          .338 **
                   (2.303)          (1.744)          (2.019)
                    1.012            1.012            1.012

HSRANK            2.949 ***         1.614 **         1.394 **
                   (2.829)          (2.461)          (2.232)
                    1.287            1.287            1.271

VERBAL             .012 ***         .007 ***         .005 **
                   (3.313)          (3.081)          (2.335)
                    1.867            1.867            1.860

MATH               .018 ***         .006 ***         .012 ***
                   (5.214)          (2.725)          (5.760)
                    1.948            1.948            1.939

RACE                .982 *          .700 **
                   (1.745)          (1.977)
                    1.020            1.020

R Square            0.406            0.264            0.363

F Statistics      33.688 ***       17.684 ***       35.198 ***

                     EVAL            ANALY            INFER

Constant            -1.177            .366             .922
                   (-1.652)          (.864)          (1.029)

CLASS                                                .372 **
                                                     (2.033)
                                                      1.010

HSRANK             1.267 **         .703 **         1 995 ***
                   (2.364)          (2.208)          (3.011)
                    1.260            1.260            1.192

VERBAL             .006 ***         004 ***          .010 ***
                   (3.130)          (3.943)          (5.184)
                    1.854            1.854            1.182

MATH                .003 *          .002 **
                   (1.859)          (2.096)
                    1.937            1.937

RACE

R Square            0.200            0.250            0.200

F Statistics      20.494 ***       27.043 ***       20.046 ***

Notes

t statistics are in parentheses.

Significant level : * 0.10, ** 0.05, ***0.01

Number underneath parentheses is variance inflation factor,
a measure of collinearity.
联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有