Group versus individual learning of quantitative accounting topics: effects on test performance in the first-year accounting course.
Cagwin, Douglass ; Barker, Katherine J.
ABSTRACT
Educators continue to search for ways to improve both accounting
and methods of teaching. Increased use of cooperative learning is often
a feature of curriculum revision. Although previous research has shown
that cooperative learning techniques can sometimes lead to improved
student learning, there has been no research that has examined the
effects of specific cooperative techniques (e.g., group homework
assignments) on learning specific quantitative business topics.
This is a field study of first-year accounting students at a large
Southeastern university. Multiple regression analysis is used to
determine whether there is a difference in test performance on
quantitative accounting topics between students completing graded
homework in groups versus students completing the same assignments
individually.
The results of this field experiment indicate that test performance
of two specifically targeted quantitative topics was not influenced by
using the cooperative learning technique of graded group assignments.
Therefore business instructors may feel free to use this cooperative
learning technique without fear that it may jeopardize learning
quantitative topics. This research did find a positive relationship
between quantitative test performance and a higher number of university
credit hours completed prior to exposure of the tested quantitative
topics. This finding may help to guide those charged with revising
business curriculum to introduce quantitative accounting topics later
rather than earlier in the sequencing of required business courses.
INTRODUCTION
For nearly two decades there have been many appeals from both
accounting professionals (e.g., American Accounting Association [AAA],
1986); Arthur Andersen et al., 1989; and Accounting Education Change
Commission [AECC], 1990, 1992) and academics to improve undergraduate
accounting education; yet the debate continues as to how the accounting
curriculum or methods of teaching should be revised. Efforts by business
schools and individual business disciplines to improve the manner in
which courses are delivered have included an assortment of educational
methods (e.g., case studies, group projects, in-class projects,
cooperative learning assignments, and community service learning
projects). The use of cooperative learning techniques has often been a
feature of curriculum revision, particularly since many employers have
embraced a more cooperative focus in the workplace.
Cooperative learning has been defined by Cooper, et al. (1990) as:
"An instructional technique which requires students to work
together in small fixed groups on a structured learning task."
Previous research has shown that the use of cooperative learning
techniques generally, but not always, leads to increased learning by
students. An underlying assumption is that by working together, students
will help teach each other (Gilbert-MacMillan, 1983; Parker, 1984).
However, little is known about the effects of cooperative techniques in
specific learning situations (e.g., group vs. individual homework
assignments) or with regard to learning specific quantitative accounting
material. Such research is important to all educators who teach subjects
that are quantitative. If experiments involving cooperative techniques
show promise, then further research may prove fruitful.
This paper presents the results of test performances of two groups
of first-year accounting students at a major Southeastern state
university. All students received the same in-class lecture on two
quantitative accounting topics by the same instructor. Approximately
half the students were given a graded homework assignment to be
completed by their group, while the second half had identical graded
homework to be completed individually. Five to seven students were in
each group, and all members of the group received identical grades.
Later in the semester, the same students switched places. Those that had
been given a group assignment received an individual homework
assignment, and vice versa.
PRIOR STUDIES
A cooperative learning strategy allows students to work together on
a graded assignment with the hope that group members will share
knowledge within their group, thereby accomplishing a shared goal and
increasing overall individual performance. An individual learning
strategy requires students to work by themselves to accomplish their own
goals (Johnson & Johnson, 1989). Encouraging students to work
together has evolved from a grassroots effort by a few professors to an
established method of education and learning. The goals of cooperative
learning are diverse and include enhanced academic achievement and
cognitive growth, increased student motivation, improved attitudes
toward learning, social development and interpersonal relations (Natasi
& Clements, 1991). Although all of these goals are important, the
focus of this paper is restricted to the effect of cooperative learning
techniques on individual academic achievement as measured by a common
exam.
Some researchers have commented that merely placing students into
groups and asking them to cooperate on a project will not be successful
(Johnson & Johnson, 1990). These efforts often fail because student
groups are afflicted with problems descriptively labeled as "free
rider," "hitchhiker," "sucker," and
"rich-get-richer" effects (Johnson & Johnson, 1990).
Johnson & Johnson (1990) make the comment that " ... groups can
also flounder through self-induced helplessness, diffusion of
responsibility, social loafing, dysfunctional labor divisions, and
destructive conflict."
While cooperative learning has positively influenced student
performance and attitude in classroom settings (Sharon, 1980; Johnson
& Johnson, 1989; Slavin, 1990), it has not always influenced
performance when used with strategies originally designed for individual
learning (e.g., graded homework assignments) (Carrier & Sales, 1987;
Klein & Pridemore, 1992; Klein, et al., 1994). The above research
suggests that a cooperative strategy may not affect educational outcomes
in all settings. Therefore, the success of cooperative learning
strategies is not assured, and its use may be more appropriate in some
settings than others.
Related research suggests that an advantage of cooperative learning
groups is that they give students an opportunity to talk aloud,
challenge and defend a point of view, and focus on the problem-solving
process rather than the answer (Gilbert-MacMillan, 1983). Parker (1984)
found that small-group cooperative learning aids in developing thinking
and problem-solving skills, and that this approach reduces student
anxiety and competition by creating a friendly atmosphere, which allows
students the freedom to learn from their mistakes. Another study of
eighth-grade pre-algebra students found that students who worked
cooperatively were better able to remember and apply problem-solving
strategies than those students from independent practice classes (Duren
& Cherrington, 1992).
The above findings lend credibility to the belief that cooperative
learning techniques may increase individual learning when applied to
quantitative accounting topics. In addition, this prior research
suggests that cooperative learning can improve student attitudes toward
the field of accounting.
HYPOTHESIS DEVELOPMENT
As described above, previous research has shown that cooperative
learning can be effective in facilitating learning, particularly when
dealing with quantitative topics in the field of mathematics. Therefore,
it is reasonable to expect that cooperative learning techniques could
enhance learning quantitative accounting topics as compared to using
only the traditional lecture-recitation model and other methods that
rely solely on individual efforts. However, it has not been established
that cooperative learning techniques, specifically group work on graded
homework assignments, are more effective than lecture-recitation and
individually graded homework assignments in assisting students to learn
quantitative rule-based accounting topics, such as inventory valuation
and cost allocations. Effects previously identified (e.g., "free
rider," "sucker," and "rich-get richer") may
mitigate any gains from collaboration in a specific setting. In
addition, although previous research has shown that cooperative learning
does have positive recall and transfer effect, cooperative learning when
applied to specific quantitative concepts may not transfer well to
individual performance, which leads to the following hypothesis:
H1: There is a difference in individual test performance on
quantitative questions between students completing graded homework
assignments in cooperative groups, and students completing graded
homework assignments individually.
Two specific quantitative accounting topics are investigated: cost
allocations and inventory valuation. The hypothesis is non-directional
since it has not been established whether the positive effects of
cooperative learning are offset by negative effects in a specific
accounting setting.
EXPERIMENT AND RESEARCH DESIGN
The subjects in this field experiment were sixty-nine students in
two sections of an accounting principles course at a major Southeastern
university. The format of the two sections was as similar as possible.
Each section of approximately 35 students met with the same instructor
each Tuesday and Thursday for 80 minutes throughout the semester.
Section 1 met from 9:30--10:50 a.m., and Section 2 met from 11:00
a.m.--12:20 p.m. Students self-selected into groups of from five to
seven students during the first week of the semester. The groups
remained intact during the entire semester.
Switching the groups on the two graded assignments limited
potential problems related to equivalency of subjects. Although the
research design minimized the risk of problems, equivalency of the
subjects was assessed because of its possible impact on interpretation
of results. Data was collected on eight demographic variables: SEX, AGE,
GPA, RACE, JOBHOURS per week, SEMHOURS (credit hours) enrolled during
the current semester, declared MAJOR, and CREDITS (semester credit
hours) earned prior to enrolling in the course. In addition, information
was gathered regarding prior accounting coursework (COURSE), and any
prior bookkeeping experience (EXP) of each student. Descriptive
statistics and univariate tests for differences between the sections are
shown in Tables 1 and 2, respectively. No significant differences
between the groups were found (p-value = .05), although RACE was weakly
significant (p-value < .10).
Of the overall sample of 69 students, 44 were male, 12 were
planning to major in accounting, 11 were non-white, six had previous
bookkeeping work experience, and 19 had previously taken accounting or
bookkeeping coursework (generally in high school). The mean student was
21.7 years of age, had accumulated 57.7 previous credit hours, was
currently enrolled in 14.9 credit hours, had a cumulative GPA of 2.97,
and was working 11.2 hours per week.
Both sections were taught by one of the authors using a common
syllabus. Class discussion and in-class exercises were the same for both
sections. However the assignment of graded group versus graded
individual homework assignments was reversed between the sections for
two topics: cost allocations and inventory valuation. Each graded
homework assignment was worth 5% of the total grade, and each member of
a group received a common grade for the group assignment. The subjects
then took a common multiple-choice examination, administered at a common
time and place. The examination consisted of 50 multiple-choice
questions, 13 of which were quantitative in nature. Of the 13
quantitative questions, three related to cost allocations, and three to
inventory valuations. The remaining seven were general quantitative
questions.
Univariate tests were used to assess the relative performance of
the two subject sections on the test questions against the seven
non-experimental sections relating to the specific quantitative topics
and to assess the equivalency of the two subject sections. Data from all
nine sections showed that students correctly completed 39.5%, 57.7% and
65.7% of the allocation, inventory valuation, and remaining questions,
respectively.
Test performance of the two subject sections was not substantially
different on the questions of interest from the other seven accounting
sections not included in the experiment (allocations was .1% lower than
the composite total, inventory valuation was 2.9% higher). There was no
statistically significant difference in raw score test performance for
the questions of interest between the two sections; however, section 2
marginally outperformed section 1 on the remaining test questions
(p-value = 0.0757), and the test as a whole (p-value = 0.0995). Test
performance was then regressed against the homework method used, test
scores on other questions, and control variables to determine the
significance and direction of the homework-method variable.
REGRESSION MODEL
The model as initially tested is as follows:
SCORE = + [[beta].sub.1]METHOD + [[beta].sub.2]MAJOR +
[[beta].sub.3]EXP + [[beta].sub.4]SEX + [[beta].sub.5]RACE +
[[beta].sub.6]COURSE + [[beta].sub.7]CREDITS + [[beta].sub.8]GPA +
[[beta].sub.9]SEMHRS + [[beta].sub.10]AGE + [[beta].sub.11]JOBHR +
[[beta].sub.12]QUEST
The variable of interest, METHOD, is a dichotomous indicator
variable coded "0" for a group homework assignment, and
"1" for an individual assignment. An additional variable,
QUEST, is included and represents the non-quantitative test questions.
It is included to allow modeling of the comparability of performance on
the questions of interest and the remainder of the test. A description
of all independent variables is included below as Table 3.
Correlation of the independent variables was examined. Most were
not significantly correlated at the alpha = 0.05 level. Exceptions
included the expected positive correlation of AGE with accumulated
CREDITS, and negative correlation of JOBHRS with SEMHRS. In addition,
MAJOR was significantly correlated with SEX (accounting majors tended to
be female); CREDITS was significantly correlated with SEX (males tended
to have accumulated more university credits by the time they took this
course, possibly because as non-accounting majors they avoided
accounting courses as long as possible); and AGE negatively correlated
with SEMHRS (the few part-time students were older.) The highest
correlation was 0.51 (SEMHRS with JOBHRS). Tests for multicollinearity
for all regressions were performed. All variance inflation factors and
condition numbers were well below the suggested values of 10 and 100,
respectively, indicating that multicollinearity among these variables is
not a problem. In addition, tests for heteroscedasticity, and analysis
of residuals and autocorrelation, the Durbin-Watson D statistic revealed
no violations of these assumptions. Analysis of the studentized
residuals revealed no outliers that needed attention.
REGRESSION RESULTS
Test performance of the two subject sections was not substantially
different on the questions of interest from the other seven accounting
sections not included in the experiment (allocations was .1% lower than
the composite total, inventory valuation was 2.9% higher). Data from all
nine sections showed that students correctly completed 39.5%, 57.7% and
65.7% of the allocation, inventory valuation, and remaining questions,
respectively. There was no statistically significant difference in raw
score test performance for the questions of interest between the two
subject sections; however, section 2 marginally outperformed section 1
on the remaining test questions (p-value = 0.0757) and the test as a
whole (p-value = 0.0995).
Table 5 sets forth the coefficients, t-statistics and p-values of
the ordinary least squares regressions on the full set of independent
variables. The model is significant (F = 4.067, p-value = 0.001), and
adjusted [R.sup.2] is 0.2220. The only significant independent variables
are CREDITS and QUES, indicating that the number of semester credit
hours accumulated prior to this course are positively associated with
total test score, and that the non-quantitative questions on the test do
have some correlation with the questions of interest. (Since the QUEST
variable could be disguising common variance with other variables, a
regression was run without QUEST. The variable GPA is then the only
significant variable.) The variable of interest, METHOD, shows no
indication of statistical significance (p-value = 0.92).
To derive a more parsimonious model, stepwise regression was
performed with selection of the "best" model based on Mallows
C(p) to minimize bias, and adjusted R2 to maximize explanatory power.
The model developed after reduction by stepwise procedures is included
as Table 6. The most appropriate parsimonious model for the dependent
variable SCORE included SEX, CREDITS, AGE, RACE, and QUEST in the
variable set, with only CREDITS and QUEST significant at the alpha =
0.05 level. The addition of METHOD provides virtually no change in the
coefficients other than a 1% change in the value of the intercept. All
the tests show clearly that METHOD has no statistically significant
effect. Therefore the hypothesis is rejected.
To further understand the factors influencing test performance, a
further regression was run to determine whether test scores as a whole
were predictable. This regression of total SCORE on the full set of
independent variables, excluding the METHOD variable, is also reported
on Table 5. The model is significant (F = 4.547, p-value = 0.0001), and
adjusted R2 is 0.3787. Although the predictive value has increased
substantially for the test as a whole, only the GPA variable is
significant.
CONCLUSIONS
The results of this field experiment indicate that test performance
is not positively or negatively influenced by using the cooperative
learning technique of graded group homework assignments versus graded
individual homework assignments. This result is similar to previous
mathematics studies where mathematics students exposed to cooperative
learning situations learned as well as students in more traditional and
individual-dependent learning strategies.
The most important finding of this study is that using the
cooperative learning technique of graded group homework assignments
versus graded individual assignments made no difference in individual
test performance. Therefore accounting instructors may feel free to use
this cooperative learning technique without fear that it may jeopardize
learning quantitative accounting topics.
It is possible in the present study that there are positive effects
of group learning but that they were mitigated by previously described
negative effects (e.g., "free rider," "sucker," and
"rich-get-richer"). If these effects could be controlled in a
real-world setting, group assignments could lead to improved
performance.
Performance on the quantitative questions of interest was not
highly correlated with the non-quantitative questions. This lack of
correlation may have been caused by a particular study strategy of the
students, where students tend to study those topics that are easier to
learn rather than the more difficult topics, such as cost allocations
and inventory valuation. (Anecdotal evidence confirming this strategy
was gathered during class discussions following the test.) It is not
surprising that students found quantitative questions to be the most
difficult to answer correctly.
The significance of the CREDITS variable in predicting quantitative
test scores is somewhat unclear. However, it is very possible that
students with more accumulated university credits have had more exposure
to various quantitative topics. Therefore these students can more easily
assimilate quantitative accounting topics than older students or those
students with higher GPAs. Because of the significance of the CREDITS
variable in predicting quantitative test scores, those who are involved
in revising accounting and business curriculum may want to rethink where
accounting principles courses are introduced to students. Students may
benefit from being exposed to other quantitative courses before they are
required to take accounting principles courses.
Although it was not tested here, the cooperative learning strategy
employed by this study may have improved the overall attitudes of
students towards fellow students, accounting, and business in general,
as was found in several mathematics studies (Davidson, 1971; Olsen,
1973; Brechting & Hirsch, 1977; Chang, 1977; Shaughnessy, 1977;
Treadway, 1983). Any positive change in students' attitude towards
the field of accounting would be most welcomed by most business
instructors, and is worthy of future study.
As with all research studies, there are many limitations. Care
should be exercised in generalizing the results to other environments.
The test subjects were all enrolled in a required introductory
accounting course. While the sample represented a representative
cross-section of predominately sophomore and junior business students at
a large Southeastern university, they may not be representative of
non-business students or students at other universities. Secondly, only
those subjects enrolled in two sections instructed by one of the authors
were included in the study. While all sections used a common textbook
and methodology, and overall test scores appeared to be comparable
between all other accounting sections, it is possible that results are
not generalizable to other instructors. Thirdly, the specialized topics
of cost allocations and inventory valuation were the topics of study.
The effects of group versus individual study may vary for other topics.
It is also possible that the cooperative learning technique chosen for
this study did not have sufficient strength on its own to obtain either
positive or negative results as measured by test performance.
There are ample opportunities to expand upon this research.
Suggestions for future research include the following:
(1) Testing whether a student's opinion of the relative amount
of learning group and individual assignments is positively correlated
with actual performance.
(2) Testing whether a student's relative enjoyment of group
versus individual assignments is correlated with relative learning.
(3) Testing whether a student's preference for the type of
homework assignment is affected by either or both the student's
belief regarding the relative amount of learning and the relative
enjoyment.
(4) Testing whether a student's attitude toward the field of
accounting is improved by the cooperative learning technique employed.
REFERENCES
Accounting Education Change Commission. (1990). Objectives for
Accountants, Position Statement Number One, Bainbridge Island, WA: AECC
Accounting Education Change Commission. (1992). The First Course in
Accounting, Position Statement Number Two, Bainbridge Island, WA: AECC
American Accounting Association. (AAA), Committee on the Future
Structure, Content, and Scope of Accounting Education (The Bedford
Committee). (Spring, 1986). Future accounting education: Preparing for
the expanding profession. Issues in Accounting Education, 169-190.
Arthur Andersen & Co.; Arthur Young; Coopers & Lybrand;
Deloitte, Haskins & Sells; Ernst & Whinney; Peat, Marwick, Main
& Co.; Price Waterhouse; & Touche Ross. (1989). Perspectives on
Education: Capabilities for Success in the Accounting Profession. New
York, NY: Andersen et al.
Brechting, M.C. & C.R. Hirsch. (1977). The effects of
small-group discovery learning on achievement and attitudes in calculus.
American Mathematics Association of Two-Year Colleges Journal, 2, 77-82.
Carrier, C.A. & G.C. Sales. (1987). Pair versus individual work
on the acquisition of concepts in a computer-based instructional lesson.
Journal of Computer-Based Instruction. 14, 11-17.
Chang, P.T. (1977). On Relationships Among Academic Performance,
Sex Difference and Attitude Persistence of Small Groups in Development
College Level Mathematics Courses. Doctoral dissertation, Georgia State
University, Dissertation Abstracts International, 38(7), 4002A.
Cooper, S., L. Cook, L. Smith, R. Mueck & J. Cuseo. (1990).
Cooperative Learning and College Instruction: Effective Use of Student
Learning Teams. Long Beach, CA: Institute of Teaching and Learning.
Davidson, N. (1971). The Small-Group Discovery Method of
Mathematics Instruction as Applied to Calculus. Doctoral dissertation,
University of Wisconsin, Dissertation Abstracts International, 31(11),
5927A.
Duren, P.E., & A. Cherrington. (February, 1992). The effects of
cooperative group work versus independent practice on the learning of
some problem-solving strategies. School Science and Mathematics, 80-83.
Gilbert-Macmillan, K.M. (1983). Mathematical Problem-Solving in
Cooperative Small Groups and Whole Class Instruction. Doctoral
dissertation, Stanford University. Dissertation Abstracts International,
4409A.
Johnson, D.W. & R.T. Johnson. (1989). Cooperation and
Competition: Theory and Research. Edina, MN: Interaction Book Company.
Johnson, D.W. & R.T. Johnson. (1990). Cooperative learning and
achievement. In S. Sharon (Ed.), Cooperative Learning: Theory and
Research. New York, NY: Praeger, Inc.
Klein, J.D. & D.R. Pridemore. (1992). Effects of cooperative
learning and need for affiliation on performance, time on task, and
satisfaction. Educational Technology, Research and Development, 40.
Klein, J.D., J.A. Erchul & D.R. Pridemore. (1994). Effects of
individual versus cooperative learning and type of reward on performance
and continuing motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19,
24-32.
Natasi, B.K. & D.H. Clements. (1991). Research on cooperative
learning: Implications for practice. School Psychology Review, 20(1),
120-131.
QOlsen, J.C. (1973). A Comparison of Two Methods of Teaching a
Remedial Mathematics Course in Community College. Doctoral dissertation,
Utah State University. Dissertation Abstracts International, 34(12),
7522A.
Parker, R. (1984). Small group cooperative learning in the
classroom. OSSC Bulletin, 27(7), 56-60.
Sharon, S. (1980). Cooperative learning in small groups: Recent
methods and effects on achievement, attitudes, and ethic relations.
Review of Educational Research, 50, 241-272.
Shaughnessy, J.M. (1977). Misconceptions of probability: An
experiment with a small-group, activity based, model building approach
to introductory probability at the college level. Educational Strategies
in Mathematics, 8(3), 295-316.
Slavin, R.E. (1990). Cooperative Learning: Theory, Research, and
Practice. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Treadway, R.T. (1983). An Investigation of the Real Problem-Solving
Curriculum in the College General Education Mathematics Course. Doctoral
Dissertation, University of North Carolina at Greensboro. Dissertation
Abstracts International, 45(1), 108A.
Douglass Cagwin, Lander University
Katherine J. Barker, Lander University
Table 1: Descriptive Statistics
Panel A--Dichotomous Variables
Demographic Statistic N # = 1 # = 0 % = 1 % = 0
MAJOR (1 = Accounting) 69 12 57 17.4% 82.6%
EXPerience (1 = Past Experience) 69 6 63 8.7% 91.3%
SEX ( 1 = Male) 69 44 25 63.8% 36.2%
RACE (1 = Non-white) 69 11 58 15.9% 84.1%
COURSE (1 = Previous Coursework) 69 19 50 27.5% 72.5%
Panel B--Continuous and Discrete Variables
Demographic Statistic N Mean Maximum Minimum
CREDITS 69 57.7 164 33
GPA 67 * 2.97 4.0 1.9
SEMHRS 69 14.9 18 3
AGE 69 21.7 43 19
JOBHR (per week) 69 11.2 45 0
* Two subjects transferred from another institution at the
beginning of the semester and had no accumulated GPA.
Table 2: Univariate Tests of Group Equivalency (a)
Demographic Statistic Mean
Sec 1 Sec 2 Std Dev
Sec 1 Sec 2 Parametricb
Statistic P-value Non-parametric
Statistic P-value
MAJOR (1=Accounting) 0.18 0.17 0.39 0.38 0.05
EXPerience (1=Past Exp) 0.12 0.06 0.33 0.24 0.88 (c)
SEX (1=Male) 0.71 0.57 0.46 0.50 1.16
RACE (1=Non-white) 0.24 0.09 0.43 0.28 1.70 (c)
COURSE (1=Previous 0.32 0.23 0.47 0.43 0.88 (c)
coursework)
CREDITS 58.0 57.5 25.0 15.2 0.1
GPA 3.0 3.00 0.60 0.60 -0.25
SEMHRS 15.4 14.5 1.78 3.20 1.49 (c)
AGE 20.9 21.6 2.34 5.20 -0.74 (c)
JOBHR (per week) 9.9 12.4 12.4 14.3 -0.76
Demographic Statistic Mean
Sec 1 Sec 2 Std Dev
Sec 1 Sec 2 Parametricb
Statistic P-value Non-parametric
Statistic P-value
MAJOR (1=Accounting) 0.96 0.003 >0.25
EXPerience (1=Past Exp) 0.39 0.79 (d) >0.25
SEX (1=Male) 0.25 1.35 (d) >0.25
RACE (1=Non-white) 0.01 2.88 (d) 10>p>.05
COURSE (1=Previous 0.39 0.95 (d) >.25
coursework)
CREDITS 0.92 0.81 (e) 0.42
GPA 0.8 -0.09 (e) 0.93
SEMHRS 0.15 1.34 (e) 0.18
AGE 0.46 0.08 (e) 0.93
JOBHR (per week) 0.45 -0.49 (e) 0.63
(a) N = 34 for Section 1 and N = 35 for Section 2; except for GPA,
which has N = 33 and N = 34, respectively.
(b) T-tests of differences between means.
(c) Failed F-test that variances are equal; results computed with
Cochran Procedure.
(d) Test of equal proportions Chi-Square statistic.
(e) Wilcoxon Rank Sum Test Z-statistic.
Table 3: Independent Variable Descriptions
Variable Name Variable Description
METHOD Indicator variable where 1 = group homework
assignment, and 0 = individual homework assignment.
MAJOR Anticipated major field of study, indicator variable
where 1 = Accounting; 0 = Not
Accounting (other business major).
EXP Indicator variable where 1 = previous accounting or
bookkeeping work experience, 0 = no previous
experience.
SEX Indicator variable where 1 = Male, 0 = Female.
RACE Indicator variable where 1 = Non-white, 0 = White.
COURSE Indicator variable where 1 = previous accounting or
bookkeeping coursework, 0 = no previous coursework.
CREDITS Number of college credit hours completed prior to
current semester.
GPA Current grade-point average on a four-point scale.
SEMHRS Number of credit hours enrolled in for current
semester.
AGE Student age in years at time of examination.
JOBHR Number of hours per week of employment.
QUEST Percentage of correct questions on the examination
that were on topics other than the quantitative
questions of interest: cost allocations or inventory
valuation.
Table 4: Correlation Matrix of the Independent Variables
Variable MAJOR EXP SEX CREDITS GPA
MAJOR 1.00
EXP .13 1.00
SEX -.37 -.20 1.00
CREDITS -.22 .16 .27 1.00
GPA .16 .03 -.17 -.12 1.00
SEMHRS -.01 -.06 -.18 -.23 .22
AGE .05 .03 .08 .41 .07
RACE .22 .01 -.17 -.13 -.11
COURSE .23 .16 -.21 -.11 .03
JOBHR -.16 .08 .16 .25 -.23
Variable SEMHRS AGE RACE COURSE JOBHR
MAJOR
EXP
SEX
CREDITS
GPA
SEMHRS 1.00
AGE -.33 1.00
RACE .19 -.12 1.00
COURSE -.02 .08 .00 1.00
JOBHR -.51 .18 -.20 .17 1.00
Note: Bold type = significant at alpha = 0.05 level.
Table 5: Results from OLS Regressions of SCORE and TOTAL on
Independent Variables
SCORE (b)
F-statistic = 4.067 [R.sup.2] 0.2943
p-value = 0.0001 Adj [R.sup.2] 0.2220
Independent
Variable Coefficient t-statistic p-value
Intercept -55.76 -1.58 0.11
MET HOD -0.51 -0.10 0.92
MA JOR 4.01 0.51 0.61
EXP 1.09 0.11 0.91
SEX -6.01 -0.97 0.33
RACE -12.92 -1.70 0.09
COU RSE -2.18 -0.34 0.73
CRE DITS 0.33 2.12 0.04
GPA 4.67 0.78 0.44
SEM HRS -0.09 -0.06 0.95
AGE 0.86 1.11 0.27
JOB HR 0.02 0.07 0.10
QUEST 1.84 3.02 0.00
TOTAL (b)
F-statistic = 4.547 [R.sup.2] 0.4855
p-value = 0.0001 Adj [R.sup.2] 0.3787
Independent
Variable Coefficient t-statistic p-value
Intercept 22.16 1.28 0.21
MET HOD N/A N/A N/A
MA JOR 2.74 0.72 0.47
EXP -0.82 -0.17 0.86
SEX -2.30 -0.77 0.44
RACE -5.58 -1.52 0.13
COU RSE -4.16 -1.37 0.18
CRE DITS 0.11 1.39 0.17
GPA 12.98 4.64 0.00
SEM HRS -0.41 -0.58 0.56
AGE 0.61 1.51 0.14
JOB HR -0.08 -0.66 0.51
QUEST N/A N/A N/A
(a) where SCORE is the prediction of the quantitative questions.
(b) where TOTAL is the prediction of the total test scores.
Table 6: Results for OLS Regressions of SCORE on Reduced Set
of Independent Variables
Without METHOD
F-statistic = 10.203
p-value = 0.0001
[R.sup.2] 0.2850
Adj [R.sup.2] 0.2570
Independent Coefficient t-statistic p-value
Variable
Intercept -52.05 -2.78 0.01
SEX -7.05 -1.29 0.20
CREDITS 0.32 2.25 0.03
AGE 0.89 1.27 0.21
RACE -12.53 -1.77 0.08
QUEST 2.15 4.70 0.00
METHOD
With METHOD
F-statistic = 8.438
p-value = 0.0001
[R.sup.2] 0.2850
Adj [R.sup.2] 0.2513
Independent
Variable Coefficient t-statistic p-value
Intercept -51.81 -2.73 0.01
SEX -7.05 -1.28 0.20
CREDITS 0.32 2.24 0.03
AGE 0.88 1.26 0.21
RACE -12.53 -1.76 0.08
QUEST 2.15 4.68 0.00
METHOD -0.50 -0.10 0.92