Predicting overall ethical climate, student retention, cheating, satisfaction with university, and perceived stress with student perceptions of faculty unethical behavior.
Schnake, Mel ; Dumler, Michael P. ; Fredenberger, William 等
ABSTRACT
A survey of undergraduate business students at two state
universities, one in the Midwest and one in the South, revealed a
significant relationship between unethical faculty behavior and student
cheating behavior. Faculty unethical behavior related to course
requirements and classroom behavior were significantly related to
student cheating behavior but not to student satisfaction with their
university experience, or student retention. Faculty unethical social
behavior (e.g., dating students) was significantly related to student
retention. Finally, the results suggest that student cheating behavior
may be a more important predictor of student perceptions of the overall
ethical climate of their university than faculty unethical behavior.
INTRODUCTION
Student retention and cheating, as well as related issues such as
student satisfaction and perceived stress, are receiving renewed
attention in colleges and universities. There is some evidence which
suggests student cheating behavior continues to be a significant problem
in colleges and universities. In 1963, Bowers (1963) published the
results of a survey of over 5,000 students on 99 campuses across the
U.S. Student respondents were asked to describe their cheating behavior
on exams and major written assignments. Seventy-five percent of the
respondents admitted they had engaged in at least some form of cheating
behavior (e.g., copying off another student during an exam, using
"cheat sheets"). During the 1990-91 academic year, McCabe and
Trevino (1993) surveyed over 6,000 students at 31 small to medium sized
colleges and universities having highly selective admissions policies
across the U.S. They found that sixty-seven percent of the respondents
admitted engaging in at least one form of cheating. Both studies suggest
that the "ethical climate" of the college or university may be
an important determinant of student cheating.
In 1993, McCabe and Trevino (1996) surveyed 1,800 students at nine
medium-sized to large state universities which had participated in the
Bowers (1963) study. Sixty-three percent of the respondents admitted
engaging in at least one form of cheating behavior. They also found that
specific forms of cheating behavior, including copying from another
student during an exam, helping another student to cheat, and using
"crib notes" or "cheat sheets," had all increased
substantially. They concluded that while the numbers of students who are
cheating may not have increased over the years, the students who do
cheat are cheating more often and in a wider variety of ways (McCabe
& Bowers, 1994).
A number of factors have been related to college student
retention/attrition including demographics, attitudes, opinions,
experiences, values and faculty attitudes and behaviors (Porter,
2003-2004; Lundquist, Spalding & Landrum, 2002-2003; Glynn, Sauer
& Miller, 2003; Reason, 2003). There is also some evidence that the
ethical climate of a university impacts student retention. Schulte
(2001) examined graduate student perceptions of ethical climate at a
Midwestern metropolitan university and found that a positive ethical
climate was important in the retention of graduate students. In a study
of undergraduate students at a Midwestern metropolitan university,
Schulte, Thompson, Hayes, Noble and Jacobs (2001) similarly found
undergraduate perceptions of ethical climate to be related to student
retention. Recently, Schnake, Fredenberger and Dumler (2004) found
student perceptions of faculty unethical behavior were related to
student satisfaction with their university experience which was, in
turn, related to student retention. Further evidence of the link between
the ethical climate of organizations and the ethical behavior of
organizational members (e.g., lying, disobedience, and being an
accomplice) is provided by Wimbush, Shepherd & Markham, (1997) and
Peterson, (2002).
There has been surprisingly little research on the outcomes of
student perceptions of faculty unethical behavior (Tabachnick,
Keith-Spiegel & Pope, 1991; Keith-Spiegel, Tabachnick & Allen,
1993). Most previous research on student perceptions of faculty
unethical behavior has focused on such topics as sexual harassment and
has ignored the range of ethical dilemmas which occur in daily
faculty-student interactions (Tabachnick, Keith-Spiegel & Pope,
1991).
One important influence on a college or university's ethical
climate is the ethical/unethical behavior of faculty. Previous measures
of university ethical climate have included several items pertaining to
faculty behavior (Schulte, 2001). Thirty-five of the sixty items in the
Undergraduate Ethical Climate Index employed by Schulte, et al., (2001)
deal with faculty unethical behavior. The ethical climate of a college
or university may influence several important outcome variables
including student unethical behavior such as cheating, student
satisfaction with their university experience, student perceived stress,
and student retention.
Based on the previous research we expect that student perceptions
of unethical faculty behavior will be positively related to student
perceived stress and student cheating, and inversely related to student
satisfaction with their university experience, perceptions of the
overall ethical climate of the college/university, and student
retention. The following hypotheses are proposed.
H1: Student perceptions of unethical faculty behavior will be
significantly related to student responses to a measure of
overall ethical climate (faces scale) of their university.
H2: Student perceptions of unethical faculty behavior will be
significantly and positively related to student perceptions of
stress after controlling for student age gender and GPA.
H3: Student perceptions of unethical faculty behavior will be
significantly and positively related to student cheating after
controlling for student age gender and GPA.
H4: Student perceptions of unethical faculty behavior will be
significantly and inversely related to student retention after
controlling for student age gender and GPA.
H5: Student perceptions of unethical faculty behavior will be
significantly and inversely related to student satisfaction with
their university experience after controlling for student age
gender and GPA.
H6: Student perceptions of the overall ethical climate of their
university will be significantly and negatively related to
student perceptions of faculty unethical behavior and student
perceptions of the frequency of student cheating after
controlling for student age gender and GPA.
METHOD
Questionnaires were administered in undergraduate management
courses at a large state university in the Midwest and a medium-sized
state university in the South. The faculty unethical behavior
questionnaire was administered during the first week of class, while the
outcomes questionnaire (student cheating, satisfaction, stress, and
retention) were administered approximately two months later. Student
identification numbers were used to match students' questionnaires.
The questionnaires were administered via a web page. The web page
address was given to students and they were allowed approximately two
weeks to complete the questionnaires after it was announced that the
questionnaire was available online. Students were given bonus points
(less than 1% of the course grade) for their participation in the
research.
Measures
Student perceptions of faculty unethical behavior were measured
with a 20-item scale developed by Schnake, Fredenberger and Dumler
(2004). Student retention (intent to transfer to another college or
university) and student satisfaction with their college/university were
measured with 13 items (e.g., "I plan to finish my current degree
program at this university," "I plan to start looking at other
colleges/universities to transfer to," "I am very satisfied
with my educational experience at this university," and
"I'm very unhappy with my educational experience at this
university"). Perceived stress in the class in which they were
completing the questionnaire was measured with five items (e.g., "I
work under a great deal of stress in this class," and "This
class causes me to feel a lot of stress"). Perceptions of student
cheating were measured with a four-item scale (e.g., "I frequently
notice students cheating on exams," and "I frequently notice
students cheating on individual term papers/projects"). Based on
the "faces scale" employed in job satisfaction research
(Kunin, 1955), we also asked respondents to "consider all aspects
of your university, and select the image which best represents your
feelings about the overall ethical climate of your university or
college." Five faces ranging from a broad smile to a very sad face
were the alternative responses available. Data were coded so that the
higher the score, the more positive the perception of the ethical
climate. A similar faces scale has been used to measure the level of
pain in pediatric patients (Keck, Gerkensmeyer, Joyce & Schade,
1996).
Standard demographic data was also collected including gender, age,
year in school, major and GPA. Average age of students was 22.16 years,
average GPA was 2.83, and sixty percent were male. Sixty-one percent
were juniors, and thirty-eight percent were seniors. Thirty-five percent
were majoring in management, nineteen percent in marketing, five percent
in accounting, one percent in economics, five percent in management
information systems, nine percent in finance and the remaining
twenty-seven percent were enrolled in majors outside the college of
business (organizational communications, computer information systems,
and other).
RESULTS
The faculty unethical behavior scale was submitted to a principal
components factor analysis with varimax rotation. This analysis produced
three distinct factors which were interpreted as: (1) lowering course
standards (to achieve higher student evaluations and/or be liked by
students), (2) unethical classroom behavior, and (3) unethical
socializing. The student satisfaction and retention items were also
submitted to a principal components analysis with varimax rotation. The
results supported the a priori two factors. Principal components
analysis with varimax rotation of the perceptions of student cheating
behavior scale resulted in all items loading cleanly on a single factor.
Means, standard deviations, reliability estimates and a correlation
matrix appear in Table 1. As can be seen from this table, all
coefficient alpha reliabilities are within acceptable ranges, ranging
from .78 to .93. Student cheating is positively related to two
dimensions of faculty unethical behavior, low course standards and
unethical classroom behavior. Student cheating was not significantly
related to unethical faculty social behavior. Student retention and
student satisfaction were not significantly correlated to any of the
three dimensions of unethical faculty behavior. Perceived stress was
positively related to the faculty unethical behavior dimension low
course standards. Finally, student age, GPA and gender were not related
to any of the dimensions of unethical faculty behavior.
Results of the regression analysis of faculty unethical behavior on
outcome variables appears in Table 2. First, student demographic
variables were entered into the regression equation to control for the
effects of student age, gender and GPA. Then the three dimensions of
unethical faculty behavior were entered into the regression equation to
assess the extent to which faculty unethical behavior contributions to
explained variance in the outcome variables beyond the effects of the
demographic variables. After controlling for these variables, we found
some types faculty unethical behavior did influence student cheating
behavior.
As Table 2 shows, faculty unethical behavior explained significant
incremental variance beyond the student demographic variables on student
cheating, providing support for Hypothesis H3. The student demographic
variables were not significantly related to student cheating. Faculty
unethical behavior did not explain significantly more variance beyond
the student demographic variables on student retention and student
satisfaction, although the amount of explained variance increased
slightly in both cases. Neither Hypothesis H4 nor H5 was supported.
Neither the student demographic variables nor the faculty unethical
behavior variables were significantly related to student perceived
stress. Thus, Hypothesis H2 was not supported.
To assess which dimensions of faculty unethical behavior
contributed to explained variance in each of these outcome variables,
beta coefficients, which appear in Table 3, were examined. Two
dimensions of faculty unethical behavior, low course standards and
unethical classroom behavior, significantly contributed to explained
variance in student cheating, explaining a total of 13% of the variance.
Student age and the faculty unethical behavior dimension of unethical
socializing both contributed to explained variance in student retention
(inverse relationships), explaining a total of approximately 9% of the
variance. Only student age contributed to explained variance in student
satisfaction with their university experience, explaining a total of 9%
of the variance.
To assess the influences on perceptions of the overall ethical
climate of the university a stepwise "usefulness analysis"
regression analysis was performed (Darlington, 1988). Student age,
gender and GPA were entered on step one as control variables. Then the
three faculty unethical behavior scales were entered on step two,
followed by the student cheating measure on step three. This was
followed by a similar analysis alternating the order of the faculty
ethical behavior scales and the student cheating measure. The faculty
unethical behavior scales explained significant increment variance
beyond the control variables (model [R.sup.2] = .11, p < .01). The
student cheating measure explained significant incremental variance
(change in [R.sup.2] = .08, p < .01) beyond the faculty unethical
behavior scales (model [R.sup.2] = .19, p < .01). Conversely, faculty
unethical behavior did not explain significant incremental variance
(change in [R.sup.2] = .03, p = .08) beyond the student cheating
measure. Thus, Hypothesis H1 is not supported.
DISCUSSION
This research provides evidence that faculty unethical behavior is
significantly related to student cheating behavior. Specifically,
unethical course-related behavior, but not unethical social behaviors,
contributed to explained variance in student cheating, after controlling
for student age, gender, and GPA. Apparently, faculty unethical
course-related behaviors establish a climate or culture which may
encourage or at least tolerate student unethical behavior in the form of
cheating. Faculty may serve as models for students. As they observe
faculty engaging in unethical course-related behaviors, it may
facilitate similar behavior in students (Bandura, 1977; Manz & Sims,
1981). Models may have a "disinhibitory effect" on observers
(Mantz & Sims, 1981). That is, students observe faculty engaging in
unethical course-related behavior and not receiving a punishment, or
perhaps even receiving some type of reward (e.g., better student
evaluations for lowering course requirements). Students are then more
likely to engage in unethical course-related behaviors such as cheating.
Models may also have a "facilitation effect" (Mantz &
Sims, 1981). In this case, the observed behavior of the model serves as
a cue for observers to engage in similar types of behaviors.. It would
appear that when students observe faculty engaging in unethical
behaviors, they may be willing to overlook their own ethical beliefs and
increase their risk taking by cheating. The message is that faculty are
role modeling undesirable behavior when they exhibit unethical personal
and classroom conduct. Their inapposite behavior encourages students to
do likewise.
Unethical faculty socializing behavior was significantly and
inversely related to student retention. It is interesting to note that
unethical faculty classroom behavior was not significantly related to
student retention, but unethical socializing such as dating students and
attending student parties, did explain significant amounts of variance
in student retention beyond the effects of student age, gender, and GPA.
It is not clear why this difference was found. However, it does suggest
that students' retention decisions are influence by faculty
unethical social behavior, while students' cheating decisions are
influenced by faculty unethical course-related behaviors.
The results suggest that faculty unethical behavior does not have a
uniform effect on outcome variables. Certain types of faculty unethical
behavior are related to student unethical behavior (i.e., cheating),
while other types of faculty unethical behavior are related to student
retention. One avenue for future research is to further examine which
specific types of faculty unethical behavior are related to various
outcome variables, such as overall ethical climate, student
satisfaction, and retention.
This research has also provided some evidence that student
perceptions of the prevalence of student cheating have a significantly
stronger impact on perceptions of the ethical climate of the university
than do perceptions of faculty unethical behavior. This contradicts
somewhat with previous research which has used faculty unethical
behavior as a major component in measures of ethical climate. Future
research should address the specific factors which impact perceptions of
ethical climate and not just assume that faculty unethical behavior is a
major determinant.
REFERENCES
Bandura, A. (1977) Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Bowers, W.J. (1964) Student dishonesty and its control in college.
New York: Bureau of Applied Social Research, Columbia University.
Darlington, R.B. (1988) Multiple regression in psychological
research. Psychological Bulletin, 79: 161-182.
Keck J, Gerkensmeyer J, Joyce B, and Schade J. (1996) Reliability
and validity of the FACES and Word Descriptor scales to measure pain in
verbal children, Journal of Pediatric Nursing 11(6):368-374.
Glynn, J.D., Sauer, P.L. & Miller, T.E. (2003) Signaling
student retention with prematriculation data. NASPA Journal, 41:
Keith-Spiegel, P., G. Tabachnick & M. Allen (1993) Ethics in
academia: Student's views of professors' actions. Ethics &
Behavior, 3: 149-162.
Kunin, T. (1955). The construction of a new type of attitude
measure. Personnel Psychology, 8:65-67.
Lundquist, C, R.J. Spalding & E.R. Landrum (2002-2003) College
student's thoughts about leaving the university: The impact of
faculty attitudes and behaviors. Journal of College Student Retention,
4: 123-133.
Mantz, C.C. & H.P. Sims (1981) Vicarious learning: The
influence of modeling on organizational behavior. Academy of Management
Review, 6: 105-113.
McCabe, D.L. and W.J. Bowers (1994) Academic dishonesty among male
college students: A thirty-year perspective. Journal of College Student
Development, 35: 3-10.
McCabe, D.L. & L.K. Trevino (1993) Academic dishonesty: Honor
codes and other contextual influences. Journal of Higher Education, 64:
522-538.
McCabe, D.L. & L.K. Trevino (1996) What we know about cheating
in college. Change, 1996, 28: 28-33.
Peterson, D.K. (2002) Deviant workplace behavior and the
organization's ethical climate. Journal of Business and Psychology,
17: 47-61.
Porter, S.T. (2003-2004) Understanding retention outcomes: Using
multiple data sources to distinguish between dropouts, stopouts, and
transfer-outs. Journal of College Student Retention, 5: 53-70.
Reason, R.D. (2003) Student variables that predict retention:
Recent research and new developments. NASPA Journal, 40:
Schnake, M.E., W.F. Fredenberger & M.P. Dumler (2004)
Dimensions of student perceptions of faculty ethical behavior: Refining
a measure and relationships with selected outcome variables. Academy of
Educational Leadership Journal, 8: 1-16.
Schulte, L.E. (2001-2002) Graduate education faculty and student
perceptions of ethical climate and its importance in the retention of
students. College Student Retention, 3: 119-136.
Schulte, L.E., F. Thompson, K. Hayes, J. Noble & E. Jacobs
(2001) Undergraduate faculty and student perceptions of the ethical
climate and its important in retention. 35: 565-576.
Tabachnick, B.G., P.C. Keith-Spiegel & K.S. Pope (1991) Ethics
of teaching: Beliefs and behaviors of psychologists as educators.
American Psychologist, 46: 506-515.
Wimbush, J.C., J.M. Shepard & S.E. Markham (1997) An empirical
examination of the relationship between ethical climate and ethical
behavior from multiple levels of analysis. Journal of Business Ethics,
16: 1705-1716.
Mel Schnake, Valdosta State University
Michael P. Dumler, Illinois State University
William Fredenberger, Valdosta State University
Table 1: Means, Standard Deviations, Reliability Estimates, and a
Correlation Matrix
Variable Mean SD 1 2 3
1. Low Course Standards 2.06 0.9 1 .59 ** .55 **
2. Unethical Classroom Behavior 2.34 1 0.83 .68 **
3. Unethical Social Behavior 1.4 0.7 0.93
4. Student Cheating 3.04 1.3
5. Student Retention 4.68 0.6
6. Student Stress 2.16 1
7. Student Satisfaction 4.01 0.7
8. Student Age
9. Student Gender na na
10. Student GPA 2.83 0.5
11. Faces 4.16 0.7
Variable 4 5 6 7
1. Low Course Standards .30 ** 0 .15 * -0.12
2. Unethical Classroom Behavior .31 ** 0 0.07 0
3. Unethical Social Behavior 0.12 0 0.12 -0.12
4. Student Cheating 0.87 0 0.11 -21 *
5. Student Retention 0.9 -0.1 .43 **
6. Student Stress 0.93 0
7. Student Satisfaction 0.83
8. Student Age
9. Student Gender
10. Student GPA
11. Faces
Variable 8 9 10 11
1. Low Course Standards 0.09 -0.1 0.05 -.24 **
2. Unethical Classroom Behavior 0.04 0 0.08 -.25 **
3. Unethical Social Behavior 0.11 0.02 0.09 -.14 *
4. Student Cheating -0.1 0.01 0.06 -.33 **
5. Student Retention -.16 * 0.12 0.03 0.11
6. Student Stress 0.06 0.03 0.11 -0.1
7. Student Satisfaction .21 ** -0.1 0.11 .30 **
8. Student Age na 0 0.1 0.06
9. Student Gender na 0.07 0.03
10. Student GPA na 0.11
11. Faces na
N = 190 to 261 (missing data) Reliability estimates boldfaced
on the diagonal.
* p < .05 ** p < .01 na=not applicable
Table 2
Results of the Regression Analysis of Faculty Unethical Behavior on
Student Cheating, Satisfaction with University, Stress, and Retention
Student Cheating [R.sup.2] Change [R.sup.2]
Step 1: Age, Gender, GPA 0.012
Step 2: Faculty Unethical Behavior .129 ** .117 **
Student Retention
Step 1: Age, Gender, GPA .054 *
Step 2: Faculty Unethical Behavior .085 * 0.031
Student Satisfaction
Step 1: Age, Gender, GPA .054 *
Step 2: Faculty Unethical Behavior .092 * 0.038
Student Stress
Step 1: Age, Gender, GPA 0.016
Step 2: Faculty Unethical Behavior 0.053 0.037
* p < .05 ** p < .01
Table 3: Specific Variables Contributing to Explained Variance in
Dependent Variables
Student
Cheating Retention Satisfaction Stress
Student Age -0.09 -.17 * .21 ** 0.02
Student Gender .01 .13 -.09 .05
Student GPA .06 .03 .11 .11
Unethical Socializing -0.16 -.24 * -.13 .08
Low Course Standards .18 * .08 -.17 .18 *
Unethical Classroom 32 ** 0.16 0.14 -0.06
Behavior
* p < .05 ** p < .01 Data in table are standardized beta coefficients.