Dimensions of student perceptions of faculty ethical behavior: refining a measure and relationships with selected outcome variables.
Schnake, Mel ; Fredenberger, William ; Dumler, Michael P. 等
ABSTRACT
Scale items used in previous studies of student perceptions of
faculty ethical behavior were administered to a sample of undergraduate
business students in order to assess the dimensionality of these
perceptions. Results suggest five distinct dimensions of student
perceptions of faculty ethical behavior. These empirically derived
dimensions were then related to three outcome variables, satisfaction
with the university, intent to remain in the current degree program, and
intent to transfer to another university. Results of this analysis
suggest that one dimension of faculty ethical behavior is related to
student satisfaction with the university, and that satisfaction, in
turn, was related to student intent to transfer to a different
university.
INTRODUCTION
Recent developments in business (e.g., Enron, Adelphia, Tyco,
WorldCom, and Andersen Consulting) and politics (e.g., the expulsion of
James Traficant from the House of Representatives, and the recent Letter
of Admonition from the U.S. Senate Ethics Committee to Senator Robert G.
Torricelli) has brought ethics to the forefront of public debate and
media attention. However, while several scholars have suggested that
teaching is rife with ethical dilemmas (Svinicki, 1994), relatively
little empirical research has been conducted on ethics in academia
(Tabachnick, Keith-Spiegel & Pope, 1991; Keith-Spiegel, Tabachnick,
and Allen, 1993). The little research on faculty ethical behavior which
has been conducted has typically focused on such topics as sexual
harassment and has largely ignored the many daily ethical dilemmas which
occur in faculty-student interactions (Tabachnick, Keith-Spiegel &
Pope, 1991). Surprisingly, there is very little research on student
perceptions of faculty ethical behavior, and the outcomes of those
perceptions (Morgan & Korschgen, 2001). The few studies which have
addressed student perceptions of faculty ethical behavior have ignored
significant methodological issues, such as the dimensionality of the
rating instrument used to assess student perceptions, and the validity
and reliability of the measurement. The primary purpose of this study is
to improve the measurement of student perceptions of faculty ethical
behavior. Items from previous questionnaires related to a narrow range
of faculty behaviors dealing with the professor-student interaction as
well as additional items to more fully capture this domain will be
examined for content validity, dimensionality, and reliability.
Identifying dimensions of student perceived faculty ethical behavior
will enable researchers to create a valid and reliable scale which can
then be used to examine outcomes of student perceptions, such as student
retention. Relationships between these empirically-derived dimensions of
student perceived faculty ethical behavior and selected outcome
variables (student course satisfaction and intent to remain in their
current degree program or retention) will then be examined.
ETHICAL BEHAVIOR
Ethical behavior is rather difficult to define precisely given its
origin in personal, organizational and societal values (Ball, 2001). Not
that long ago, ethics was defined largely in terms of complying with the
law. While, expectations have risen in recent years, it is difficult to
draw a precise line between what is generally considered ethical
behavior, and what is not. In a general sense, what is considered
ethical behavior is what "most people" would consider right
and wrong. Clearly, different groups may define ethical behavior
differently. Therefore, one determinant of ethical behavior is the
context within which it occurs. Ethical behavior in one industry or
occupation may be quite different from another. Ultimately, an
individual's ethics are his or her beliefs about what is right or
wrong, good or bad (Garrett & Klonoski, 1992). Thus, an
individual's actual ethical behavior is influenced by his or her
personal beliefs and values, the climate of the organization, as well as
society's values.
FACULTY ETHICAL BEHAVIOR
Some authors have expressed skepticism about the general ethical
integrity of university and college professors (Anderson, 1992;
Callahan, 1982; Sykes, 1989). Others point out how difficult it is to
develop standardized expectations for behavior in university settings
given the diversity, professional training, and independence of faculty
(Whicker & Kronenfeld, 1994). A recent survey found that 74% of
college seniors in a Zogby International poll reported that their
professors, when teaching ethics, tell students that there is no clear
right or wrong; rather, what is "ethical" depends on
one's individual values and culture (Leo, 2002).
Yet other authors argue that ethical behavior has a key role in
higher education (Smith, 1996; Little, 1989; Markie, 1994; Strike &
Soltis, 1992). Evidence of the growing concern of ethics in academia is
the number of professional organizations which have developed codes of
ethics (e.g., the Academy of Management, American Association of
University Professors, American Psychological Association). In addition,
many colleges and universities have developed codes of conduct, although
it has been suggested that many of these take a legalistic or minimalist approach, defining codes of conduct largely in terms of legal compliance
(Kibler, 1994; Zabihollah, Elmore & Szendi, 2001).
There is some evidence that student perceptions of ethical climate
may be related to important outcome variables such as student retention
(Lipshutz, 1993; Parker, 1997). The 103-item Ethical Climate Index was
administered to a sample of graduate students and faculty in the college
of education in a Midwestern state university (Schulte, 2001). Schulte
(2001) concluded that the data indicated that both faculty and students
believed that a positive ethical climate is important in student
retention in the degree program. Day-to-day faculty behavior and
interactions with students would clearly influence student perceptions
of the ethical climate, and thereby their likelihood of continuing in
their current program of study.
MEASURES OF STUDENT PERCEPTIONS
Given the paucity of research addressing student perceptions of
faculty ethical behavior, there are few research instruments available.
Tabachnick, Keith-Spiegel, and Pope (1991) developed a 63-item measure
which they used to compare the perceptions of teaching psychologists
with clinical psychologists. The items were based on a previous study of
teaching psychologists, and represent what the psychologists, not
necessarily the students, consider to be examples of unethical behavior.
The psychologists indicated the extent to which they engaged in these
behaviors as well as rating how ethical they viewed each behavior. They
also administered this instrument to college students and professors,
asking them to rate the acceptability of behaviors in which professors
might engage. They found great similarity between student and professor
perceptions, male and female student perceptions, freshmen and
upper-class student perceptions, and between Midwest and West Coast
student perceptions.
In a follow-up study, Keith-Spiegel, Tabachnick and Allen (1993)
developed a 107-item scale, taking 59 items from their earlier
questionnaire (Tabachnick, Keith-Spiegel & Allen, 1991), and an
additional 48 items from incidents contributed by students responding to
a request to describe ethical problems with professors. Thus,
approximately half of these items were identified by students as
examples of faculty unethical behavior. Students rated these items in
terms of how ethical they considered the behavior. They found few
differences between freshmen and advanced students, between Midwest and
West Coast students, and significant differences on only 6 of the 107
behaviors between male and female students. The types of faculty
behaviors perceived as most unethical by students are those which impact
fairness, and acts which violate a perceived class contract (e.g.,
"using a grading procedure that does not adequately measure what a
student has learned," "giving every student an A regardless of
the quality of their work," and "changing the criteria for
successful completion of the class in the middle of the semester").
Birch, Elliott and Trankel (1999) developed a 64-item measure which
asked respondents to rate the ethical appropriateness of 64 behaviors.
Twenty-seven items in this scale were adapted from the Tabachnick et al.
(1991) and Keith-Spiegel (1993) scale, and thirty-two items were
developed from a review of other ethics surveys, from discussions with
faculty and students, and from participant (faculty) discussions during
a workshop on ethics. This instrument was administered to the faculty at
a Western state university. They found that there was strong agreement
among the respondents about fairness in grading, relationships between
faculty and students, and university responsibilities. However, there
were "gray areas" about which there was much less agreement
concerning other aspects of student/faculty relationships. Similar to
previous research, they found no differences between male and female
respondents.
Morgan and Korschgen (2001) asked a sample of faculty and
undergraduate students to rate the ethicalness of 16 faculty behaviors
on a five-point scale with endpoints of "unquestionably not
ethical" to "unquestionably ethical." They selected items
from the Tabachnick, et al. (1991) scale which dealt primarily with
student-faculty relationships. They found significant differences
between faculty and student perceptions on four of the sixteen items.
Faculty saw increasing popularity by giving easy tests, accepting a
textbook rebate, sexual involvement with a student, and using profanity in lectures as more unethical than did students. Students saw using old
lecture notes, and the breaking of students' confidences as more
unethical than did faculty.
These instruments all measured a relatively broad array of faculty
behaviors which were judged to have ethical implications. In addition to
the day-to-day professor-student interactions (e.g., behavior in the
classroom, assignments, grading), these questionnaires measured such
faculty behaviors as dealings with colleagues, inappropriate use of the
institution's resources, and avoiding college/university or
departmental responsibilities.
One common methodological feature, and significant limitation, of
these studies is the use of multiple item measures of perceived faculty
ethical behavior, yet analyzing each item individually. All of these
researchers conducted statistical tests of each individual item included
in their scale. There was no assessment of discriminant or convergent
validity or even reliability estimation. Nor was there any attempt to
validate dimensions of perceived faculty ethical behavior across
different groups of respondents (e.g., students and faculty). The
problem with this, of course, is that some of these items may be highly
correlated and therefore redundant. If many subjects interpret two items
in a scale to mean the same thing, then we no longer have two separate,
independent measures. We are not measuring two separate constructs, and
they should not be interpreted nor analyzed as such. They should be
combined into a single measure. This is particularly likely to occur
when several similar items are presented to subjects at the same time.
Respondents may honestly interpret several items similarly, or they may
respond to several items in a manner similar to phenomenon known as
common method variance. Respondents may, for example, attempt to be
consistent with their responses, producing spurious, inflated
correlations among the items. (Podsakoff & Organ, 1986). Regardless
of the specific reason, the result is, in the respondents' minds,
several items are the same. Analyzing or interpreting the results of
these items individually is no longer appropriate.
A perhaps more significant problem with a single item measure is
the difficulty in assessing its reliability and validity. In other
words, it is difficult to establish precisely what is in fact being
measured with single item measures.
Branstetter and Handelsman (2000) used a modified version of the
Tabachnick et al. (1991) scale to measure graduate student perceptions
of psychology faculty ethical behavior. In the first part of their
study, they asked graduate students to evaluate how ethical they viewed
50 faculty behaviors. In the second part of the study, they asked
respondents to report how frequently they engaged in the same 50
behaviors. They point out that the Tabachnick, et al. (1991) scale
relates to six distinct areas of faculty behavior: (1) in-class issues,
(2) lessons and evaluations, (3) outside-of-classroom issues, (4)
relationships in academia, (5) responsibilities to students and
colleagues, and (6) issues unique to teaching of psychology. This
implies that the 63 items in the Tabachnick, et al. (1991) scale should
collapse into these 6 dimensions. In fact, Branstetter and Handelsman
(2000) did perform an exploratory factor analysis on the data from the
first part of their study, which resulted in 5 factors (based on the
eigenvalues = 1.00 rule of thumb and a scree test). Thus, the analysis
did not support the a priori six dimensions of ethical faculty behavior.
They interpreted these factors as follows: (1) systemic issues in
academia; (2) dual relationship issues between graduate teaching
assistants and students; (3) sexual relationships between faculty and
students; (4) fidelity, justice and general competence in teaching; and
(5) veracity, confidentiality and professional issues. A subsequent
analysis examined graduate teaching assistant perceptions on these
factors across age, gender, year in program, and specialty area. They
found that older teaching assistants were less likely to rate dating and
sexual relationships with students as ethical, and that, compared to
women, men were more likely to rate the dual relationships factor as
unethical but more likely to rate dating and sexual issues as ethical.
Using data from the second part of their study, where they asked the
same graduate teaching assistants to rate the frequency in which they
themselves engaged in each of the 50 behaviors, the 50 items were
compared individually across various subgroups (e.g., gender, age). One
of their conclusions was that male graduate teaching assistants (GTAs)
engaged in certain behaviors more frequently than did female GTAs.
However, two of the three behaviors (scale items) loaded on the same
factor (Factor 3: Sexual Relationships; item 9: "Becoming sexually
involved with a student", and item 18: "Engaging in sexual
fantasies about a student"). Since these loaded on the same factor
in the principal components analysis, it makes little sense to further
analyze them individually. In the respondents minds, these items measure
largely the same construct. The third item included in this analysis did
not load significantly on any of the factors in the exploratory factor
analysis. Therefore, it is difficult to interpret what exactly is being
measured with this item, or assess its validity and reliability.
SINGLE ITEM MEASURES
It has long been accepted that multiple-item measures are
preferable to single-item measures (Nunnally, 1978). This preference is
based on the general notion that a measurement instrument consists of a
random sample of items from the domain of all items which measure the
construct of interest. There are several other, more specific reasons
for the preference for multiple-item measures. First, it is not possible
to test the internal consistency reliability of a single-item measure.
In addition, reliability can often be improved by adding items to a
scale. However, recently it has been suggested that there are
circumstances where single-item measures may be desirable ( Scarpello
& Campbell, 1983; Wanous, Riechers, & Hudy, 1997; Gardner,
Cummings, Dunham & Pierce, 1998; Nagy, 2002). The relative
advantages and disadvantages of single-item versus multiple-item
measures is not the focus of this research. Rather, this research
examines the possible misuse of previous multiple item measures of
student perceptions of faculty ethical behavior as single-item measures,
and creating a valid and reliable scale which taps a limited part of the
domain of faculty ethical behavior, specifically, those dealing with
professor-student interactions. More completely specifying this domain
by developing additional scale items and assessing the dimensionality,
content validity and reliability of this instrument will enable
researchers to more fully test the notion that student perceptions of
faculty ethical behavior are related to important outcomes such as
student retention.
RESEARCH GOAL
The purpose of this research is to examine a multiple-item measure
of student perceived faculty ethical behavior to assess the
dimensionality, or content validity, of this measure. The ultimate goal
of this research is to develop a valid, yet more parsimonious, measure
of this construct. This measure is based on items taken from the
Tabachnick, Keith-Spiegel, and Pope (1991); Keith-Spiegel, Tabachnick
and Allen (1993), and Morgan and Korschgen (2001) instruments. Items
were selected which focused on the faculty-student relationship both
within and outside of the classroom. There are clearly other important
types of faculty behaviors which have ethical implications, however,
students are not qualified judges of these behaviors (e.g., assisting
junior colleagues). We focused on the limited domain of ethical faculty
behaviors which students could readily observe and thus could accurately
judge.
Empirically-derived dimensions of faculty ethical behavior will
then be regressed on important outcome variables including student
course satisfaction and student retention (intent to remain in their
current degree program and intent to transfer to a different
university).
MEASURES
A 95-item questionnaire was developed, taking items from previous
measures (Tabachnick, Keith-Spiegel & Pope, 1991; Keith-Spiegel,
Tabachnick & Allen, 1993; Morgan & Korschgen, 2001; Branstetter
& Handelsman, 2000). Items were selected which dealt specifically
with faculty behaviors relating to student-faculty interaction both
within and outside of the classroom. Additional items were added to the
scale to more completely measure the ethical principles related to
student affairs suggested by Kitchener (1985), and the five areas of
teaching behavior with distinct ethical implications suggested by Smith
(1996). Kitchener (1985) suggested five ethical principles in dealing
with students: (1) respect for autonomy, respect for the
individual's right to make his/her own decisions; (2)
nonmaleficence or doing no harm to others; (3) beneficence or benefiting
others; (4) justice and treating others fairly; and (5) fidelity, being
loyal and trustworthy. Smith (1996) suggested that certain faculty
behaviors are directly related to ethical issues: (1) total student
empowerment (e.g., turning over responsibility for the course to
students, structuring the course to achieve high student ratings); (2)
instructional materials and student expectations (e.g., choosing a
textbook without reading it); (3) instructor's presence (e.g.,
frequently missing class, not being available to students); (4)
examinations and grading (e.g., giving test questions not related to
course material); (5) professional relationships (e.g., giving a high
recommendation for a student whose performance does not warrant it). At
this early stage in the research process, it was important to be
inclusive in order to fully measure the domain of faculty ethical
behaviors relating to student-faculty interaction and identify
dimensions of this domain. This domain is illustrated in Figure 1.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Items included were developed based on both faculty and student
inputs. The original Tabachnick et al. (1991) scale was generated based
on input from teaching psychologists. However, the follow-up study
(Keith-Spiegel, Tabachnick and Allen, 1993) included approximately the
same number of items based on input from students. The Birch, Elliott,
and Trankel (1999) scale was based in part on the Tabachnick et al.
(1991) scale, but additional items (32 items) were generated based on
discussions with both faculty and students. Thus, the scale employed in
the present study is based approximately equally on input from faculty
and students. In addition, there is recent evidence that students and
faculty have relatively similar views regarding faculty ethical behavior
(Morgan & Korschgen (2001).
Instructions on this scale defined ethical behavior in terms of
right and wrong, and distinguished it from desirable and undesirable
behavior (Appleby, 1990; Murray, 2000), as recommended by Morgan and
Korschgen (2001). Respondents were instructed to focus on whether a
behavior was right or wrong, and not whether they liked or disliked it.
A seven-point Likert-type scale was employed with scale anchors of
Definitely Ethical (1) and Definitely Unethical (7), with a midpoint of
Neutral (4).
METHOD
The 95 item Faculty Behavior questionnaire was administered to
several undergraduate business classes during the third week of class in
the Fall Semester 2002 at a medium-sized state university in the south,
and in a large lecture-hall management class at a large state university
in the Midwest. A total of 132 students responded at the Midwestern
university. The average age of these students is 21.03, 50.4% are male
(.8% did not report their gender), and the average grade point average
is 2.97. At the southern university,
The Faculty Behavior scale was submitted to a principal components
analysis resulting in nineteen factors with eigenvalues greater than
1.0, explaining 65.85% of the variance in the scale. However, not all of
these factors were interpretable, and a scree test suggested four to
five factors. A varimax rotation was performed and items which did not
load cleanly (factor loading >= .50) on a single factor were omitted.
A second factor analysis was performed on the reduced-item scale (10
items were omitted), again producing nineteen factors with eigenvalues
>= 1.0. A scree test again suggested between four and five factors.
Once again, several factors were not interpretable, and several items
did not load cleanly on a single factor. The data were submitted to a
varimax rotation, and each scale item was examined to determine those
items which exhibited very high (>= .50) loadings on a single factor.
Thirty such items were identified.
These thirty items were submitted to a principal components
analysis, resulting in five factors with eigenvalues >= 1.0,
explaining 60.53 % of the variance. A scree test also suggested five
factors. The results of this analysis appear in Table 1.
Nine items loaded cleanly on Factor 1. These items deal with a
professor's behavior within the classroom, including grading,
examinations, and fairness in dealing with students. This factor was
interpreted as Classroom Ethical Behavior. Five items loaded on Factor
2. These items pertain to loaning money to students, borrowing money
from students, accepting gifts from students, and asking favors of
students. This factor was interpreted as Inappropriate Exchange. Six
items loaded on Factor 3. These items deal with friendships and
non-romantic relationships with students. This factor was interpreted as
Relationships Outside the Classroom. Four items loaded on Factor 4.
These items focus on dating and having sexual relationships with
students. This factor was interpreted as Dating and Relationships. Six
items loaded on Factor 5. These items have to do with effort and the use
of time. More specifically, these items addressed the professor not
being adequately prepared for class, not maintaining scheduled office
hours, frequently arriving late for class, frequently missing class
without notice, and taking a long time to grade and return exams. This
factor was interpreted as Inappropriate Use of Time. Coefficient alpha
reliability estimates ranged from .79 to .89 for these five factors.
Reliabilities and correlations among the five empirically derived
dimensions of faculty ethical behavior appear in Figure 4. Reliability
estimates appear on the diagonal. As Table 2 shows, there is a moderate
degree of correlation among the five factors. Factors 2 and 3 (favors
and loans, platonic relationships outside of the classroom), factors 2
and 4 (favors and loans, dating), factors 3 and 4 (platonic
relationships outside the classroom, dating), and factors 1 are 5
(classroom behaviors, inappropriate use of time) are most strongly
related.
OUTCOME VARIABLES
Questionnaires containing outcome measures were administered to
students in these introductory management and information systems
classes. The outcome measures were administered approximately two months
after the administration of the ethical behavior scale. Student ID
numbers were used to match respondent questionnaires. A thirteen-item
scale measured retention (intent to finish the current degree program at
the current university) (9 items, e.g., "I plan to finish my
current degree program at this university."), and satisfaction with
the current university (4 items, e.g., "I really do not like this
college/university"). Principal components analysis of these 13
items revealed three factors. Three retention items ("There is a
very good chance that I will transfer to another college/university to
finish my degree," "I plan to start looking at other
colleges/universities to transfer to," and "I plan to change
colleges/universities as soon as possible.") loaded on a single
factor. This factor was interpreted as "intent to transfer."
Three additional retention items ("I plan to finish my current
degree program at this college/university," "I made the right
choice when I enrolled in this college/university," "I'm
very unhappy with my educational experience at this
college/university.") loaded on a second factor. This factor was
interpreted as "intent to remain." The four satisfaction items
(e.g., "I am very satisfied with my educational experience at this
college/university," "I really do not like this
college/university.") loaded together along with the remaining
three retention items.
Coefficient alpha reliability analysis was performed on the items
making up these three factors. Reliability estimate of the intent to
remain scale is .87. A coefficient alpha reliability estimate of .91 for
the intent to transfer scale was achieved by dropping the "I plan
to change colleges/universities as soon as possible" item. Finally,
a coefficient alpha reliability coefficient of .81 was achieved by
dropping the "I really do not like this college/university"
item.
RELATIONSHIPS AMONG PERCEIVED FACULTY ETHICAL BEHAVIOR AND OUTCOME
VARIABLES
Variables for the five faculty ethical behavior subscales and the
three outcome variables were created by averaging respondents'
scores on these items. The three outcome variables were regressed on the
five faculty ethical behavior scales. The results indicate that only one
of the faculty ethical behavior scales was significantly related to any
of the outcome variables. Inappropriate Exchange was significantly and
negatively related to satisfaction with the university (R2 = .06, p <
.05; b = -.20, p < .01).
Two-way and three-way interactions among the outcome variables were
tested and found to be non-significant. However, bivariate correlations
of -.20 (p < .01) and .17 (p < .05) was observed between
satisfaction and intent to transfer, and satisfaction and intent to
remain, respectively. This raises the issue of whether student
perceptions of faculty ethical behavior might have an indirect effect on
retention through satisfaction with the university. To examine this
question. additional regressions were performed. First, Inappropriate
Exchange has already displayed a relationship with satisfaction with the
university. Next, satisfaction was regression on intent to remain and
intent to transfer. Satisfaction with the university was significantly
related to intent to transfer (R2 = .05, p < .01; b = -.24, p <
.01). Thus, while not providing a complete test of direct and indirect
relationships, the results are suggestive that student perceptions of
faculty ethical behavior may impact satisfaction with the university,
which in turn affects students' intent to transfer.
DISCUSSION
Previous versions of measures of faculty ethical behavior have
generally been rather lengthy (e.g., 63 items). However, this research
provides evidence that several dimensions of student perceived faculty
ethical behavior can be measured with a much shorter scale.
The only previous factor analysis of student perceptions of faculty
ethical behavior (Branstetter & Handelsman, 2000) also identified 5
dimensions. However, this previous research employed a 50-item measure
of specific teaching and teaching-related behaviors in a sample of
graduate teaching assistants. There are some similarities between this
previous research and the present study. Both studies identified a
factor involving lending money to students, asking favors of students,
selling goods to students, and accepting gifts from students. Both
studies also identified a factor dealing with dating and romantic
relationships between professors and students. In addition, both studies
identified a factor related to fairness and general competence in
teaching (e.g., using alcohol or drugs while teaching, not maintaining
scheduled office hours, missing class).
This study has identified five distinct dimensions of student
perceptions of faculty ethical behavior, and conducted initial work
toward developing a more parsimonious measure of these dimensions.
Further psychometric work on scale development is warranted to further
validate this measure. Once the scale is finalized, it can easily be
re-written to assess the "ethical climate" of the institution
by asking respondents to rate the frequency of each behavior in general
among the faculty. However, a reliable and valid measure of this
construct should provide the impetus for examining how student
perceptions of faculty ethical behavior, or the "ethical
climate" of the institution, affect important outcome variables
such as student retention, class attendance, perceived stress, and even
student performance.
This study also provides some preliminary evidence that student
perceptions of faculty ethical behavior may be related directly and/or
indirectly to some important outcome variables, including satisfaction
with the university, intent to remain, and intent to transfer. While
faculty ethical behavior in the present study was significantly related
to only satisfaction with the university, satisfaction with the
university and intent to transfer were significantly and inversely
related. It may be that perceptions with faculty behavior are directly
related to satisfaction with the university and indirectly related to
intent to remain and intent to transfer through satisfaction.
One limitation of the study is that the same sample was used for
scale validation and testing relationships with outcome variables.
However, tempering this limitation is that predictor and outcome
variables were measured at different times, thus minimizing to some
extent common method variance problems.
REFERENCES
Anderson, M. (1992). Imposters in the temple. New York: Simon &
Schuster.
Appleby, D.C. (1990). Faculty and student perceptions of irritating
behavior in the college classroom. The Journal of Staff, Program &
Organization Development. 8, 41-46.
Ball, R. (2001). Practical difficulties with ethics. Public
Management, 83(3), 2-5.
Branstetter, S.A. & M.M Handelsman. (2000). Graduate teaching
assistants: Ethical training, beliefs, and practices. Ethics &
Behavior, 10(1), 27-53.
Callahan, D. (1982). Should there be an academic code of ethics?
Journal of Higher Education, 53, 335-344.
Gardner, D.G., L..L. Cummings, R.B. Dunham & J.L. Pierce.
(1998). Single-Item versus multiple-item measurement scales: An
empirical comparison. Educational & Psychological Measurement,
58(6), 898-915.
Garrett, T.M. & R.J. Klonoski. (1992). Business Ethics, 3rd
Edition, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Keith-Spiegel, P., G. Tabachnick & M. Allen. (1993). Ethics in
academia: Students' views of professors' actions. Ethics &
Behavior, 3, 149-162.
Kitchener, K.S. (1985). Ethical principles and ethical decisions in
student affairs. In H.J. Canon & R.D. Brown (Eds.) New directions
for student services: Applied ethics in student services. San Francisco,
CA: Josey-Bass, 17-29.
Leo, J. (2002). Professors who see no evil. U. S. News & World
Report, 133(3), 14.
Lipschutz, S.S. (1993). Enhancing success in doctoral education:
From policy to practice. New Directions for Institutional Research, 80:
69-80.
Little, D. (1989). Are academicians by their very nature ethical,
or should they be? Paper presented at the Annual Convention of the
Virginia Community Colleges Association, Roanoke, VA.
Markie, P.J. (1994). A professor's duties: Ethical issues for
college teaching. Lanhan, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.
Morgan, B.L. & A.J. Korschgen. (2001). The ethics of faculty
behavior: Students' and professors' views. College Student
Journal, 35(3), 418-432.
Murray, B. (2000). Professors' most grating behaviors. Monitor
on Psychology, January, 56-67.
Nagy, M.S. (2002). Using a single-item approach to measure facet job satisfaction. Journal of Occupational & Organizational
Psychology, 75(1), 77-87.
Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Parker, C.E. (1997). Making retention work. Black Issues in Higher
Education, 13(26), 120-128.
Podsakoff, P.M., & Organ, D. W. (1986). Self-reports in
organizational research: Problems and prospects. Journal of Management,
12, 531-544.
Scarpello, V. & Campbell, J. P. (1983). Job satisfaction: Are
all the parts there? Personnel Psychology, 36, 577-600.
Smith, R. (1996). Essential ethical considerations in education.
Education, 117(1), 17-21.
Strike, K.A. & J.F. Soltis. (1992). The ethics of college
teaching. New York: Teachers College Press.
Svinicki, M. (1994). Ethics in college teaching. In W.G. McKeachie
(Ed.), Teaching tips: Strategies, research, and theory for college and
university teachers. 9th Edition, Lexington, MA: D.C. Heath, 269-277.
Sykes, C. (1989). ProfScam: Professors and the demise of higher
education. Washington, DC: Regnery Gateway, Inc.
Tabachnick, B.G., P.C. Keith-Spiegel & K.S. Pope. (1991).
Ethics of teaching: Beliefs and behaviors of psychologies as educators.
American Psychologist, 46, 506-515.
Wanous, J. P., Reichers, A. E. & Hudy, M. J. (1997). Overall
job satisfaction: How good are single-item measures? Journal of Applied
Psychology, 82, 247-252.
Whicker, M. & Kronenfeld, J. (1994). Dealing with ethical
dilemmas on campus. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Zabihollah, R., R.C. Elmore & J.Z Szendi. (2001). Ethical
behavior in higher educational institutions: The role of the code of
conduct. Journal of Business Ethics, 30(2), 171-183.
Mel Schnake, Valdosta State University
William Fredenberger, Valdosta State University
Michael P. Dumler, Illinois State University
Table 1: Results of the Principal Components Analysis of Faculty
Ethical Behavior
Factor 1 Factor 2
Classroom Inappropriate
Ethical Exchange
Behavior
Giving lower grades to students .77 .00
who disagree with him/her (the
instructor).
Teaching class under the influence .75 .00
of alcohol or recreational drugs.
Not providing alternative teaching .65 .00
and testing procedures for students
who have learning disabilities.
Giving exams which do not reflect .73 .00
the material covered/discussed
Allowing how much he/she likes or .72 .23
dislikes a student to influence the
student's grade.
Including false or misleading .79 .00
information that may hurt the
student's chances when writing a
letter of recommendation for the
student.
Including material on a test that was .75 .00
not covered in the lectures or
assigned reading.
Requiring students to disclose .74 .23
highly personal information in a
group discussion or exercise (e.g.,
students who remain silent and don't
discuss the information are graded
down).
Asking small favors (such as a ride .00 .57
home) from students.
Accepting expensive gifts from .28 .70
students.
Borrowing money from students. .19 .82
Lending money to a student. .00 .78
Accepting inexpensive gifts from .00 .70
students.
Teaching a class without being .17 .00
adequately prepared that day.
Failure to maintain regularly .36 .17
scheduled office hours.
Frequently arriving several minutes .15 .00
late for class.
Frequently missing class without .43 .00
advance notice.
Not getting exams graded and .32 .00
returned until 4 weeks after the
exam was given.
Never learning any of the student's .00 .20
names in a relatively small class
(e.g., 20 to 30).
Being sexually attracted to a .00 .16
student.
Dating a student majoring in a field .00 .27
outside the professor's teaching
assignment and unlikely to ever
enroll in the professor's class.
Dating a student not currently .00 .31
enrolled in the professor's class.
Becoming sexually involved with a .00 .00
student after the course is
completed and grades are filed.
Hugging a student. .00 .00
Accepting a student's invitation to a .13 .28
party.
Selling goods (e.g., books, a car) to .00 .13
a student.
Hiring a student to work for him/her -.12 .20
(painting a house, baby-sit, etc.).
Beginning an on-going friendship .00 .20
with a student who is enrolled in the
professor's class.
Going to a bar with students after .33 .36
class.
Eigenvalue 7.96 4.85
% of Variance Explained 27.43 16.74
Factor 3 Factor 4
Relationships Dating
Outside the
Classroom
Giving lower grades to students .00 .00
who disagree with him/her (the
instructor).
Teaching class under the influence .00 .18
of alcohol or recreational drugs.
Not providing alternative teaching .00 .21
and testing procedures for students
who have learning disabilities.
Giving exams which do not reflect .00 .00
the material covered/discussed
Allowing how much he/she likes or .00 .00
dislikes a student to influence the
student's grade.
Including false or misleading .00 .00
information that may hurt the
student's chances when writing a
letter of recommendation for the
student.
Including material on a test that was .00 .00
not covered in the lectures or
assigned reading.
Requiring students to disclose .00 .00
highly personal information in a
group discussion or exercise (e.g.,
students who remain silent and don't
discuss the information are graded
down).
Asking small favors (such as a ride .38 .29
home) from students.
Accepting expensive gifts from .00 .21
students.
Borrowing money from students. .24 .11
Lending money to a student. .33 .14
Accepting inexpensive gifts from .23 .21
students.
Teaching a class without being .21 .00
adequately prepared that day.
Failure to maintain regularly .00 .00
scheduled office hours.
Frequently arriving several minutes .00 .00
late for class.
Frequently missing class without .00 .13
advance notice.
Not getting exams graded and .00 .14
returned until 4 weeks after the
exam was given.
Never learning any of the student's .00 .00
names in a relatively small class
(e.g., 20 to 30).
Being sexually attracted to a .21 .68
student.
Dating a student majoring in a field .16 .83
outside the professor's teaching
assignment and unlikely to ever
enroll in the professor's class.
Dating a student not currently .20 .82
enrolled in the professor's class.
Becoming sexually involved with a .26 .79
student after the course is
completed and grades are filed.
Hugging a student. .57 .33
Accepting a student's invitation to a .61 .16
party.
Selling goods (e.g., books, a car) to .74 .00
a student.
Hiring a student to work for him/her .73 .16
(painting a house, baby-sit, etc.).
Beginning an on-going friendship .66 .16
with a student who is enrolled in the
professor's class.
Going to a bar with students after .49 .26
class.
Eigenvalue 1.90 1.59
% of Variance Explained 6.56 5.46
Factor 5
Inappropriate
Use of Time
Giving lower grades to students .12
who disagree with him/her (the
instructor).
Teaching class under the influence .18
of alcohol or recreational drugs.
Not providing alternative teaching .23
and testing procedures for students
who have learning disabilities.
Giving exams which do not reflect .23
the material covered/discussed
Allowing how much he/she likes or .00
dislikes a student to influence the
student's grade.
Including false or misleading .13
information that may hurt the
student's chances when writing a
letter of recommendation for the
student.
Including material on a test that was .18
not covered in the lectures or
assigned reading.
Requiring students to disclose .19
highly personal information in a
group discussion or exercise (e.g.,
students who remain silent and don't
discuss the information are graded
down).
Asking small favors (such as a ride .00
home) from students.
Accepting expensive gifts from .14
students.
Borrowing money from students. .00
Lending money to a student. .00
Accepting inexpensive gifts from .11
students.
Teaching a class without being .64
adequately prepared that day.
Failure to maintain regularly .56
scheduled office hours.
Frequently arriving several minutes .77
late for class.
Frequently missing class without .62
advance notice.
Not getting exams graded and .61
returned until 4 weeks after the
exam was given.
Never learning any of the student's .73
names in a relatively small class
(e.g., 20 to 30).
Being sexually attracted to a .12
student.
Dating a student majoring in a field .00
outside the professor's teaching
assignment and unlikely to ever
enroll in the professor's class.
Dating a student not currently .00
enrolled in the professor's class.
Becoming sexually involved with a .00
student after the course is
completed and grades are filed.
Hugging a student. .11
Accepting a student's invitation to a .00
party.
Selling goods (e.g., books, a car) to .17
a student.
Hiring a student to work for him/her .00
(painting a house, baby-sit, etc.).
Beginning an on-going friendship .13
with a student who is enrolled in the
professor's class.
Going to a bar with students after .00
class.
Eigenvalue 1.23
% of Variance Explained 4.25
Table 2: Correlations Among Faculty Ethical Behavior Dimensions
Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 5
Factor 1 .90 .40 ** .12 * .12 * .54 **
Factor 2 .86 .60 ** .49 ** .31 **
Factor 3 .79 .53 ** .21 **
Factor 4 .87 .19 **
Factor 5 .80