Creativity in the entrepreneurship classroom.
Schmidt, Jacqueline J. ; Soper, John C. ; Facca, Tina M. 等
INTRODUCTION
A recent study identified the consistent decline since 1990 in
creativity scores of U.S. students on the Torrance test of creativity
(Bronson & Merryman, 2010). At the same time, a 2010 American
Management Association study identified creativity and innovation as one
of the four critical skills needed for business success today and in the
future. A study of CEOs identified creativity as the number one
leadership competency of the future (Bronson & Merryman, 2010).
Is this decline in creativity a problem for the entrepreneurship
classroom? Is it important to include creativity exercises in an already
full entrepreneurship curriculum? In this study the authors review the
relationship between creativity and entrepreneurship, and study the
effectiveness of including divergent thinking exercises in the
entrepreneurship classroom.
The Relationship Between Creativity and Entrepreneurship
Creativity has been identified by several researchers as related to
entrepreneurship. Yar Hamidi, Wennberg and Berglund (2008) found that
high scores on creativity tests and prior entrepreneurial experiences
were both positively associated with entrepreneurial intentions, and
contended that creativity should be considered in models of
entrepreneurial intentions. Golshekoh, Gholamreza, Mirsaladin, Askary,
and Alireza (2010) also found a positive relationship between scores on
creativity tests and entrepreneurship. Fillis and Rentschler (2010)
found a link between creativity and motivation, actualization and
innovation. Several other studies have identified motivational traits
and creativity as important factors in entrepreneurial activity and
success (Baum, Locke, & Smith, 2000; Stewart & Roth, 2001).
Sternberg and Lubart (1999) defined creativity as the ability to
produce work that is both novel (original, unexpected) and appropriate
(useful, adaptive concerning task constraints). Using this definition
Youl-Lee, Florida, and Acs (2004) contended that "entrepreneurship
is a form of creativity and can be labeled as business or
entrepreneurial creativity because new businesses are original and
useful (p.882)."
Sternberg and Lubart's definition includes the elements
generally associated with creativity: the development of divergent
thinking (generating lots of unique ideas) followed by convergent
thinking (combining these ideas into the best result). Divergent
thinking (problem finding) is often associated with the arts and
humanities and tested by creativity tests, while convergent thinking
(problem solving) is often tested in intelligence tests and identified
as more associated with science and technology (Atherton, 2010).
Creativity tests, unlike IQ tests, require a multitude of responses
rather than a single response (Hocevar, 1981).
Creativity and Entrepreneurship Education
In 1994, Timmons argued that creativity should be central to
entrepreneurship education. Morrison and Johnston (2003) argued that
creativity was so important it should be introduced into the
entrepreneurship curriculum widely and not confined to a specific
course. In a survey of the chairs/directors of the top twenty-five U.S.
undergraduate programs in entrepreneurship as identified by Entrepreneur
magazine for the years 2009 and 2010, 79% of chairs agreed that courses
in creativity/innovation were very important in an entrepreneurship
program (Schmidt, Soper & Bernaciak, 2011). Enhancing creativity and
innovation was listed as one of the five skills by entrepreneurs in
Malaysia in which more training was needed (Josoh, Ziyae, Asimiran,
Kadir, 2011), indicating that the U.S. is not alone in its creativity
deficit.
Most of the assessment of creativity in the entrepreneurship
classroom has been to assess the convergent creativity of a project
produced by an individual or group through scales or ratings of peers,
instructors, or judges. This data can be collected through cases,
simulations or real life presentations such as business contests and
presentations. In the study of the Top 25 Undergraduate Entrepreneurial
Programs, all programs used this approach in teaching creativity
(Schmidt, et. al. 2011).
Less focus has been on the assessment of the divergent creativity
in the entrepreneurship classroom or in expanding the creative
approaches used by students. This data can be collected through
creativity tests. Although the most common test of divergent thinking is
the Torrance test of creativity (Coren, 1995), other divergent thinking
tests are: picture-word tests in which subjects are asked to write as
many reactions to the picture as they can in one minute, and the
alternative-uses tests in which subjects are asked to think of alternate
uses for a variety of common objects such as a shoe, pencil, etc.. The
range of divergent thinking responses can be measured in several ways:
fluency (number of ideas generated); flexibility (variety of ideas,
categories or perceptions); elaboration (the ability to add to or build
off of the idea); originality (the ability to create fresh, new ideas);
complexity (the ability to conceptualize many layers or difficult
concepts); imagination (inventing new categories); curiosity (the use of
probing questions about the idea); risk taking (to be courageous,
willing to stand apart) (Wilson, 2004).
Why is divergent thinking critical?
The 2010 AMA study stressed that the increased importance of
creativity in the future comes from changes in the nature of work,
global competition, pace of change and organizational structure. As
different cultures (based on different values) approach problem solving
and problem finding differently (Choi, Koo, & Choi, 2007); the need
to think in new ways will be critical in the global economy. This
difference in approaching creativity and problem finding has already
been recognized between fields. Berglund and Wennberg (2006) found in
comparing engineering students and business school students (both groups
in entrepreneurship programs) that while they had similarities in
creativity test scores, they did not differ in approaches to creative
problem solving/finding when the fields (engineering, business)
emphasized different creative issues and methods. Divergent thinking
exercises help students develop multiple approaches to
problem-finding/solving.
Current Study
The current study examines whether students enrolled an
entrepreneurship class perform better in the number of ideas generated
(fluency) and the range/variety(flexibility) of ideas, are more
confident of their creativity, and use more creative approaches then
students not in entrepreneurship courses. The study also examines
whether practice in divergent thinking exercises increases
entrepreneurship students' abilities in generating the number of
ideas, range of ideas, and range of creative approaches used. The
following hypotheses are advanced.
Hypothesis One: Students in the entrepreneurship class will
generate more ideas and a greater range of ideas than students not
enrolled in the entrepreneurship class.
Hypothesis Two: Students in the entrepreneurship class will
perceive themselves as more creative and will report engaging in more
creative approaches than those not in the entrepreneurship class.
Hypothesis Three: Practice in divergent thinking activities will
increase entrepreneurship students' abilities to generate ideas and
to expand the range of ideas.
METHOD
Eighty-nine students enrolled in the first course of an
entrepreneurship minor and forty-two students enrolled in basic
communication public speaking courses were given two picture-word tests
and two creativity surveys. The entrepreneurship course is an elective
course in an entrepreneurship minor and the public speaking course is a
required university course for all students. While both courses value
creativity; one in generating ideas for presentations, the other in
generating ideas for projects; exercises in divergent thinking were done
in the entrepreneurship class and not in the public speaking class.
Student populations in both courses were of freshman and sophomore
rank and there were no substantial gender differences in class
composition. All of the students attended a private Midwestern
university in the U.S. and both classes were semester long courses
running from January to May 2011. Students were given the divergent
thinking exercise and the creativity survey during the first week of
class in January and again during the last week of class in May.
The exercises to test divergent thinking were picture-word tests.
The first picture-word test had a picture of a man in a building with
the word "DO" on it. The second picture-word test had a
picture of a bobsledder with the word "BE" (Stamp, 2010). The
students were shown the picture and asked to generate and write down as
many words and ideas they could associate with the picture in one
minute. After the papers were collected, students completed a survey
about how comfortable they were doing the assignment using a scale from
1 (not comfortable) to 10 (extremely comfortable); how much creativity
they think they possessed using a scale from 1(not creative) to 10
(extremely creative); and to indicate from a list of several activities
the frequency of the times (never, seldom, frequently, always) they use
the activity when they encounter a problem or need to develop a new
idea.
The picture-word tests were evaluated by counting the number of
responses generated (fluency) and the range/variety (flexibility) of the
responses. To assess range/variety, responses were coded and recorded in
the following eight categories: simply repeating the images on the
picture such as man, briefcase, sled, etc.; identifying action in the
picture such as walking, running, etc.; creating a story as to what was
happening in the picture such as going to meeting or interview,
competing for gold, etc.; creating a broader meaning or metaphor for the
picture such as work, IBM, Olympics, etc.; identifying a feeling such as
sad, happy, fearful, etc.; identifying colors; identifying a time frame
(past, night, etc.); expressing a sensory experience such as see
(blurry), hear (loud), taste (sour) or touch (rough).
The responses to the surveys on comfort level, confidence in
creativity, and activities were also tabulated. The number of approaches
used to solve a problem was measured by counting the number of times a
student selected "always" as a response.
RESULTS
T-tests were used to determine significant differences between the
means of various groups (e.g., entrepreneurship students vs.
non-entrepreneurship students). H1, which predicted that students in the
entrepreneurship class would perform better at divergent thinking
activities in generating ideas and in having a wider range of ideas than
those not enrolled in entrepreneurship classes, was supported. Students
in the entrepreneurship class did perform significantly better than
non-entrepreneurship students in both rounds 1 and 2, both in the number
of ideas generated and the range (variety) (Table 1).
H2, which predicted that students in entrepreneurship classes would
perceive themselves more creative than those not in entrepreneurship
classes, had mixed results. There were no significant differences in
perceived creativity between the two groups of students in the round
one. However, in the second round, entrepreneurship students perceived
themselves as significantly more creative than non-entrepreneurship
students perceived themselves to be (7.80 vs. 6.57 on the ten-point
scale; t = 2.81,p = .007, df = 54).
The second part of H2 which predicted that students in the
entrepreneurship class would engage in more creative approaches than
those not in the class was not supported. There was no significant
difference in the number of creative approaches used by entrepreneurship
vs. non-entrepreneurship students in round 1. In comparing round 1 with
round 2, entrepreneurship students evidenced no significant difference
in the number of creative approaches used, but non-entrepreneurship
students increased their average number of creative approaches
significantly, from 2.64 to 4.91 (t = 2.09, p = .063, df = 10).
The most common creative approaches for entrepreneurship students
in round one were talking, internet and brainstorming. In round two,
internet was the most common approach, followed by talking and taking a
break. (Table 2)
While not increasing the number of creative approaches used,
entrepreneurship students reported significant increases in their use of
the internet as an approach to creativity between rounds 1 and 2, from a
mean of 3.32 to 3.66 (t = 3.72, df = 43, p =.001). Non-entrepreneurship
students evidenced significant increases in the use of mind mapping and
word association (Table 3).
H3, which predicted practice in divergent thinking exercises would
increase entrepreneurship students' ability to generate ideas
(fluency) and the range (flexibility) of ideas received mixed results.
The first part that entrepreneur students would generate more ideas was
supported. Entrepreneurship students evidenced a significant increase in
the number of ideas generated between the two rounds (11.68 to 13.79),
as shown in Table 4, whereas non-entrepreneurship students showed no
significant difference between rounds.
The second part of the hypothesis that entrepreneurship students
would increase the range (flexibility) of ideas was not supported.
Although entrepreneurship students did not increase the range of their
ideas, there was a significant increase in the number of entries within
categories in their range. Among entrepreneurship students, in round 1,
the most common types of ideas generated were metaphors, or what it is
like, followed by ideas related to what one sees something is, what one
feels, and then the actions one identifies in the picture. There were
significant increases between rounds 1 and 2 on the number of ideas
related to feelings, actions and metaphors (Table 5).
DISCUSSION
The study supports extant research on the relationship between
creativity and entrepreneurship. The findings demonstrate that students
who are in entrepreneurship classes do perform better at divergent
thinking both in fluency (ideas generated) and in flexibility (range of
expressed ideas) both at the beginning and the conclusion of the class
compared to their non-entrepreneurship counterparts. The significant
increase in the performance of these entrepreneurship students between
rounds 1 and 2 also supports the claim that exercises in divergent
thinking are important in the entrepreneurship classroom (Schmidt, et.
al. 2011) and should be included in course work.
Besides including divergent thinking exercises in the
entrepreneurship curriculum the findings suggest several other
recommendations. First, the finding that entrepreneurship students had
significant changes in their allocation of ideas within categories of
their range, but did not expand their range of options for expressing
ideas, suggests that instructors should focus on exploring a variety of
ways in which divergent thinking can be expressed (originality,
elaboration, complexity, imagination) and directly practice these in the
classroom to improve the student's range of ideas generated.
Second, while students in the entrepreneurship class did generate
more ideas and a wider range, their approaches to creativity did not
increase as they did for the non- entrepreneurship students. This
finding suggests that the environment (classroom) could be an important
factor in developing approaches to creativity. In further analyzing this
type of data researchers should look closely at what approaches to
creativity are taught and emphasized in the class. Are entrepreneurship
classes focusing on very specific types of approaches (internet in this
case) rather than exploring a wider variety of approaches? In the global
economy, the capacity to recognize and adapt to a variety of creative
styles and perspectives will be essential in the future (AMA, 2010).
This could mean that classes in entrepreneurship need to incorporate and
stress more variety in approaches to creativity to increase student
understanding and use of various methods.
Third, while there were no differences in perceptions of creativity
between students at the start of the course, at the end of the course
entrepreneurship students increased their perception of their creativity
potential. This finding suggests that practice in divergent thinking
activities can affect perception of and confidence in creativity, and
supports Morrison and Johnston's (2003) argument that creativity
should be widespread throughout the entrepreneurship curriculum rather
than in a single course.
Limitations and Future Research
The limitations of the study relate primarily to the methodology.
First, the frequency scale used for creative approaches should be
expanded from a four choices to a ten point scale. This would allow for
more clarification of how often a student engages in different
approaches. Because of the narrow scale, the study was limited to using
"always" for assessment of choices. Second, the courses
examined were both freshman/sophomore level courses and the
entrepreneurship course was the beginning course in an entrepreneurship
minor. Even though the entrepreneurship course was an elective, it is
unclear whether students enrolled in it really considered themselves as
entrepreneurs or were simply curious about the minor. Conducting the
same study with students enrolled in the third or fourth course of the
minor would be helpful in validating this perception about
entrepreneurship and creativity. Finally, the study should be expanded
to have students complete 3 or 4 picture-word tests and surveys in each
of the classrooms instead of only the two given. This would provide more
information to assess whether the improvement in divergent thinking is
due more to the effect of practice in divergent thinking exercises or to
the classroom environment (structure) or both. Research has already
shown that different fields (Berglund & Wennberg, 2006) approach
problem finding and problem solving in different ways. Future
researchers may implement the study with student populations from other
countries and cultures. The results of the tests could be compared to
identify differences or similarities in types of divergent thinking and
approaches to creativity between cultures. This research could result in
identifying and teaching different approaches to creativity in the
classroom that could lead to better understanding and more effective
interaction with other cultures.
REFERENCES
American Management Association (2010). AMA 2010 Critical Skills
Survey. Retrived from http://www.p21.org/. Atherton, J. (2010). Learning
and Teaching: Convergent and Divergent Thinking. (Online UK) Retrieved
10 June 2011 from http.//www.learningandteaching/
info/learningconverge/htm.
Baum, R., Locke, E.A., and Smith, K. (2000). A multi-dimensional
model of venture growth. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 292-303.
Berglund, H. & Wennberg, K. (2006). Creativity among
entrepreneurship students: comparing engineering and business education.
International Journal of Continuing Engineering Education and Lifelong
Learning, 16, 366-379.
Bronson, P. and Merryman, A. (2010). The creativity crisis.
Newsweek July 19, 2010.
Choi, I., Koo, M., and Choi, J. (2007). Individual differences in
analytic versus holistic thinking. Personality And Social Psychology
Bulletin, 33, 691-705.
Coren, S. (1995). Differences in divergent thinking as a function
of handedness and sex. American Journal of Psychology, 108 (3), 311-325.
Fillis, I. and Rentscheler, R. (2010). The role of creativity in
entrepreneurship. Journal of Enterprising Culture, 18 (1), 49-81.
Golshekoh, F. Hassan, A., Gholamreza, P., Mirsaladin, E., Askary,
P. and Alireza H., (2010). Relationship between creativity, grade point
average, achievement motivations, age and entrepreneurship among
university students. Australian Journal of Basic and Applied Sciences, 4
(10), 5372-5328.
Hocevar, D. (1981). Measurement of creativity: Review and critique.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 4 (5), 450-464
Josoh, R., Ziyae, B., Asimiran, S., Kadir, S. (2011). Entrepreneur
training needs Analysis: Implication on the entrepreneurial skills
needed for successful entrepreneurs. International Business &
Economics Research, 10 (1), 143-147.
Morrison, A. Johnston, B. (2003). Personal Creativity for
Entrepreneurship. Active Learning in Higher Education. Vol. 4 (2),
145-158.
Schmidt, J. Soper, J. and Bernaciak, J. (2011) Assessing creativity
in the entrepreneurship classroom. Presented at Small Business Institute
Conference, Bonita Springs, Florida.
Stamp, J. (2010) Creativity Workshop. John Carroll University.
University Hts., Ohio.
Sternberg, J. and Lubart T. (1999). The concept of creativity:
prospects and paradigms, in Steinberg, R. (Ed.) Handbook of Creativity.
Cambridge University Press, New York.
Stewart, W. and Roth, P. (2001). Risk propensity differences
between entrepreneurs and managers: A meta-analysis review. Journal of
Applied Psychology. 86, 145-153.
Timmons J. (1994). New Venture Creation. Chicago, Irwin.
Wilson, L. (2004). Divergent Thinking Abilities. Retrieved from
http:// Yar Hamidi, D, Wennberg, K and Berglund, H. (2008). Creativity
in entrepreneurship education. Journal of Small Business and Enterprise
Development, 15(2) 304-320.
Youl-Lee, S, Florida, R and Acs, Z. (2004). Creativity and
entrepreneurship: A regional analysis of new firm formation. Regional
Studies, 38(8), 879-891.
Jacqueline J. Schmidt, John Carroll University
John C. Soper, John Carroll University
Tina M. Facca, John Carroll University
Table 1: Divergent Thinking Performance
Mean Mean Non- t df p
Entrepreneurship Entrepreneurship
Ideas 11.10 8.85 2.44 100 .016
Generated
Round 1
Range of 4.62 3.97 2.02 74 .047
ideas
Round 1
Ideas 13.79 9.24 3.89 60 .000
Generated
Round 2
Range of 4.55 3.90 2.09 39 .043
ideas
Round 2
Table 3: Significant Increases in Creative Approaches
Used by Non-entrepreneurship Students
Round 1 Mean Round 2 Mean
Non- Non-
entrepreneurship entrepreneurship
Mind mapping 2.00 2.50
Word association 1.78 2.61
t df p
Mind mapping 2.31 17 .035
Word association 3.59 17 .002
Table 4: Significant Increase in Ideas Generated
Round 1 Round 2 t df p
Ideas generated 11.68 13.79 2.33 55 .024
Table 5: Significant Increases in Types of Ideas Generated
Idea Type Round 1 Round 2 t df p
Feelings 1.73 3.50 4.03 29 .000
Actions 1.00 1.73 2.39 10 .038
Metaphors 3.54 4.56 2.15 47 .036