What effective schools do.
West, Martin R.
Arguably, the most important development in K-12 education over the
past decade has been the emergence of a growing number of urban schools
that have been convincingly shown to have dramatic positive effects on
the achievement of disadvantaged students. Those with the strongest
evidence of success are oversubscribed charter schools. These schools
hold admissions lotteries, which enable researchers to compare the
subsequent test-score performance of students who enroll to that of
similar students not given the same opportunity. Through careful study
of the most effective of these charter schools, researchers have
identified common practices--a longer school day and year, regular
coaching to improve teacher performance, routine use of data to inform
instruction, a culture of high expectations--that have yielded promising
results when replicated in district schools.
We have only a limited understanding of how these practices
translate into higher academic achievement, however. It may be that
attending a school that employs them enhances those basic cognitive
skills--such as processing speed, working memory, and reasoning--that
research in psychological science has shown contribute to success in the
classroom and later in life. Do schools that succeed in raising test
scores do so by improving their students' underlying cognitive
capacities? Or do effective schools help their students achieve at
higher levels than would be predicted based on measures of cognitive
ability alone?
To address this question, we thaw on unique data from a sample of
more than 1,300 8th graders attending 32 public schools in Boston,
including traditional public schools, exam schools that admit only the
city's most academically talented students, and charter schools. In
addition to the state test scores typically used by education
researchers, we also gathered several measures of the cognitive
abilities psychologists refer to as fluid cognitive skills. Our data
confirm that the latter are powerful predictors of students'
academic performance as measured by standardized tests.
Yet while the schools in our sample vary widely in their success in
raising test scores, with oversubscribed charter schools in particular
demonstrating clear positive results, we find that attending a school
that produces strong test-score gains does not improve students'
fluid cognitive skills. Put differently, our evidence indicates that
effective schools help their students achieve at higher levels than
expected based on their fluid cognitive skills. It also suggests that
developing school-based strategies to raise those skills could be an
important next step in helping schools to provide even greater benefits
for their students.
Crystallized Knowledge and Fluid Cognitive Skills Despite decades
of relying on standardized test scores to assess and guide education
policy and practice, surprisingly little work has been done to connect
these measures of learning with the measures developed over a century of
research by cognitive psychologists studying individual differences in
cognition. Psychologists now consider cognitive ability (few dare say
"intelligence" anymore) to have two primary components:
crystallized knowledge and fluid cognitive skills. Crystallized
knowledge comprises acquired knowledge such as vocabulary and
arithmetic, while fluid skills are the abstract-reasoning capabilities
needed to solve novel problems (such as the ability to identify patterns
and make extrapolations) independent of how much factual knowledge has
been acquired. The terms were coined by the late psychologist Raymond
Cattell, who first distinguished two types of intelligence. Cattell
noted that one "has the 'fluid' quality of being
directable at almost any problem," while the other "is
invested in particular areas of crystallized skills which can be upset
individually without affecting others."
Hundreds of studies show that, at any point in time, the two are
highly correlated: people with strong fluid cognitive skills are at an
advantage when it comes to accumulating the kinds of crystallized
knowledge assessed by most standardized tests.
That these capabilities are nonetheless distinct is best
illustrated by the fact that fluid cognitive skills decline with age
starting even in one's twenties, while crystallized knowledge tends
to rise over the decades, in some cases peaking as late as one's
seventies. In an influential 2002 study involving people ages 20 to 92,
University of Texas at Dallas psychologist Denise Park and colleagues
found that the fluid cognitive skills of participants in their twenties
exceeded those of participants in their seventies by as much as 1.5
standard deviations. In other words, more than 90 percent of
participants in their twenties had higher fluid cognitive skills than
did typical participants in their seventies. Those in their seventies
nonetheless scored higher than participants in any other age range on
tests of vocabulary, a key component of crystallized knowledge.
At a more fine-grained level, cognitive psychologists have
identified multiple aspects of fluid cognition, including processing
speed (how efficiently information can be processed), working memory
(how much information can be simultaneously processed and maintained in
mind), and fluid reasoning (how well novel problems can be solved).
Longitudinal studies tracking individuals from late childhood through
young adulthood indicate that gains in processing speed support gains in
working memory capacity that, in turn, support fluid reasoning. Each of
these abilities has been shown to be associated with academic
performance, suggesting that they promote or constrain learning in
school.
The strength of the relationship between fluid cognitive skills and
academic performance also suggests that schools that are particularly
effective in improving standardized test scores may do so by improving
fluid cognition along one or more of these dimensions. This is what our
research sought to explore.
Data and Sample We gathered the data for our study during the
spring of 2011 from 32 of the 49 public schools in Boston that serve
8th-grade students. The schools that agreed to participate in the study
included 22 open-enrollment district schools, five oversubscribed
charter schools, two exam schools to which students are admitted based
on their grades and standardized test scores, and three charter schools
that were not oversubscribed at the time the 8th-grade students in our
study were admitted. Boston's oversubscribed charter schools are of
particular interest, as multiple studies have exploited the lottery
admissions process to document the schools' effectiveness in
raising student test scores (see "Boston and the Charter School
Cap," features, Winter 2014).
Within those schools, we collected data on all students for whom we
obtained parental consent for participation and who were in attendance
on the day we collected data. These 1,367 students represent 43 percent
of all 8th-grade students attending public schools in Boston and 64
percent of the students in participating schools. Seventy-seven percent
of the students in our sample are from low-income families, 38 percent
are African American, and 39 percent are Hispanic, in each case closely
matching the demographic composition of all 8th-grade students attending
public schools in the city and 8th graders attending the same schools.
The fluid cognitive skills we measured for each student included
processing speed, working memory, and fluid reasoning. For processing
speed, students were asked to translate numbers into corresponding
symbols using a number-symbol key, and to indicate as quickly as
possible under a time constraint whether either of two symbols on the
left side of a page matched any of five symbols on the right side. For
working memory, students viewed an array of blue circles, blue
triangles, and red circles, and were instructed to count the number of
blue circles within 4.5 seconds. After viewing between one and six
arrays, they were prompted to record the number of blue circles
contained in each. Finally, the fluid-reasoning task required students
to choose which of six pictures completed the missing piece of a series
of puzzles that became progressively more difficult. Because these three
measures are closely related in theory and were positively correlated
among the students in our sample, we also averaged them to create a
summary measure of students' fluid cognitive ability.
Fluid Cognitive Skills Predict Test Scores Our first step is to
examine the relationship between our measures of fluid cognitive skills
and scores from the state's standardized tests. We look at the
students' scores on the Massachusetts Comprehensive Assessment
System (MCAS) tests in math and reading (ELA) and improvements in those
test scores over time. We use simple correlation coefficients to measure
the strength of the relationship between fluid cognitive skills and test
scores. Correlation coefficients can range from -1 to 1, with a
correlation of 0 indicating that there is no linear relationship between
the two variables in question.
The correlations between our measures of fluid cognitive skills and
8th-grade math test scores are positive and statistically significant,
ranging from 0.27 for working memory to 0.53 for fluid reasoning. The
correlation between math test scores and our summary measure of fluid
cognitive ability is 0.58, which implies that differences in fluid
cognitive skills can account for more than one-third of the total
variation in math achievement. The relationships are somewhat weaker for
test scores in reading. Even so, variation in our summary measure of
fluid cognitive ability can explain as much as 16 percent of the total
variation in reading achievement.
Fluid cognitive skills are also related to the rate at which
students improve their test-score performance over time. To measure
gains in student achievement, we calculate the difference between
8th-grade performance in each subject and the performance level that
would have been expected based on performance in both subjects in 4th
grade. The correlations between our summary measure of fluid cognitive
ability and test-score gains in math and reading were 0.32 and 0.18,
respectively.
A high degree of correlation between measures of fluid cognitive
skills and test scores is not news. As noted above, fluid cognitive
ability has a long track record of predicting how much students know and
are able to do. Our findings do suggest, however, that the specific
measures of fluid cognitive skills we administered in classrooms as part
our research were able to capture academically relevant differences in
student cognition.
Schools Improve Test Scores but Not Fluid Skills We address our
central question of whether schools that raise student test scores also
improve fluid cognitive skills in two complementary ways. First, we use
our entire sample to analyze the extent to which the schools that
students attend can explain the overall variation in student test scores
and fluid cognitive skills, controlling for differences in prior
achievement and student demographic characteristics (including gender,
age, race/ethnicity, and whether the student is from a low-income
family, is an English language learner, or is enrolled in special
education). Second, we focus on the subset of students who entered the
admissions lottery at one of the five oversubscribed charter schools in
order to study how attending one of those schools affected test scores
and fluid cognitive skills.
Consistent with other research on school effects, we find that the
school a student attends can explain a substantial share of the overall
variation in test scores: that single factor explains 34 percent of the
variation in math scores and 24 percent of the variation for reading. In
contrast, after accounting for prior achievement and demographics, the
school attended explains just 2.3 percent of our summary measure of
fluid cognitive ability.
This pattern suggests that schools may influence students'
test scores but not by affecting their fluid cognitive skills. However,
this analysis does not account for the possible sorting of students into
particular schools based on characteristics not captured by their prior
achievement and demographic characteristics. Such "selection
effects" could in theory account for the apparent school impacts on
test scores, or even the apparent absence of impacts on fluid cognitive
skills.
Our second analysis aims to address this concern. Because the
oversubscribed charter schools in our sample admit students via random
lotteries, comparing the outcomes of lottery winners (most of whom
enrolled in a charter school) and lottery losers (most of whom did not)
is akin to a randomized-control trial of the kind often used in medical
research. Evaluations led by Harvard's Tom Kane and MIT's Josh
Angrist have used this lottery-based method to convince most skeptics
that the impressive test-score performance of the Boston charter sector
reflects real differences in school quality rather than the types of
students charter schools serve.
Due to the limited coverage of our sample, we cannot claim for our
analysis the same level of rigor as these previous lottery-based
evaluations. Of the roughly 700 applicants for the lotteries used to
admit students in the 8th-grade cohort in our study, only 200 of them
are in our evaluation sample. Focusing on lottery applicants is
nonetheless useful because it enables us to hold constant whatever
unmeasured differences lead some students to apply for a seat in a
charter school and others to remain within the district. When comparing
lottery winners and losers, we also control for prior achievement and
the same set of demographic characteristics used in our broader
analysis. We use standard methods to account for the fact that not all
lottery winners enrolled in a charter school and remained there
throughout middle school (and some lottery losers eventually obtained a
seat). This approach enables us to generate estimates of the effect of
each additional year of actual attendance at a charter school between
5th and 8th grade.
Our results show that each year of attendance at an oversubscribed
Boston charter school increases the math test scores of students in our
sample by 13 percent of a standard deviation. This is a noteworthy
effect, equivalent to roughly a 50 percent increase in the academic
progress students typically make in a school year (see Figure 1).
Charter school attendance also appears to have a modest positive effect
on reading scores, though this estimate falls short of statistical
significance due to the relatively small number of students in our
lottery sample. Even as students benefit academically, however, their
fluid cognitive skills hardly budge. The estimated effect of charter
school attendance for each of our measures is very small in magnitude;
none is statistically significant.
Are Test-Score Gains "Real"? There is ample reason to
believe that the test-score gains generated by these schools are
meaningful, despite the lack of corresponding improvement in fluid
cognition. State tests are aligned to standards that specify the
knowledge and capabilities students are expected to acquire--the very
things cognitive psychologists call crystallized knowledge. And there is
strong evidence that crystallized knowledge, which also bears a strong
resemblance to E. D. Hirsch's notion of Core Knowledge, matters a
great deal for success in school and beyond. Recent studies by Harvard
economist Raj Chetty and colleagues confirm that teachers who improve
student test scores also improve their students' earnings as adults
(see "Great Teaching," research, Summer 2012). Moreover,
lottery-based evaluations of the Boston charter sector show that
attending high schools affiliated with three of the charter schools in
our sample increases Advanced Placement test-taking and performance and
the likelihood of attending a four-year college.
Indeed, in our view, the unique data we gathered for this study
make these schools' accomplishments all the more impressive. They
show that the schools that are most effective in raising student test
scores do so in spite of the strength of the underlying relationship
between math achievement and fluid cognitive skills. In other words,
these schools have figured out ways to raise students' academic
achievement well above what is expected given the students'
baseline fluid cognitive skills.
A compelling way to see this is to look at the relationship across
schools between the average test-score gain students make between the
4th and 8th grade and our summary measure of their students' fluid
cognitive ability at the end of that period (see Figure 2). Each dot
represents a school, and the diagonal line shows the overall
relationship between test-score gains and fluid cognitive ability across
the full sample of schools. The extent to which a school is above or
below that line indicates whether the average test-score improvement
among its students has been greater or less than would be predicted
based on their fluid cognitive skills.
Most schools fall relatively close to the regression line,
indicating that their students' academic progress is roughly as
expected given the students' fluid cognitive skills, but there are
clear exceptions. Most notably, each of the five oversubscribed charter
schools is well above the regression line. A few open-enrollment
district schools also show the ability to drive similarly outsized
gains, an important reminder that while governance matters, what counts
in the end is effective practice. Finally, while exam-school students
have considerably higher fluid cognitive skills (as would be expected of
students who gain admission via test scores and grades), attending one
of these locally renowned schools in the company of other bright
students confers no systematic advantage. This last finding is
consistent with recent evidence showing no academic benefits of
attending a Boston or New York City exam school for students who just
met the admissions criteria (see "Exam Schools from the
Inside," features, Fall 2012).
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
What do these differences in school performance mean in
layman's terms? Among students who fell below the midway point on
our summary measure of fluid cognitive ability, only 20 percent of those
attending a district school were deemed proficient in math as defined by
Massachusetts on its 8th-grade math test. In oversubscribed charter
schools, 71 percent of such students were deemed proficient. This is a
remarkable difference for students who rank lower than their peers on a
key enabling capacity. For district students, success is the rare
exception (2 in 10), while for oversubscribed charter school students,
it is closer to the rule (7 out of 10).
At the same time, fluid cognitive skills remain potent predictors
of academic progress even among students attending oversubscribed
charter schools. While these schools succeed in generating test-score
gains for students of all cognitive abilities, it is still the case that
students with strong fluid cognitive skills learn more. Indeed, the
strength of the correlation between fluid cognitive skills and
test-score growth in oversubscribed charter schools is statistically
indistinguishable from the correlations we observe among students in
open-enrollment district schools and exam schools.
Could Schools Boost Fluid Cognitive Skills, Too? Our research
sought to examine whether schools that have demonstrated success in
raising test scores also boost students' fluid cognitive
skills--either as a byproduct or perhaps as a principal pathway for
improvements in test scores. That turns out not to be the case. This
result does not, in our view, call into question the value of the
improvements in crystallized knowledge captured by improvements in test
scores.
What we do not yet know, however, is which long-term outcomes are
more strongly influenced by fluid cognitive skills and which by
crystallized knowledge. One reason is that, as we see in our study
sample, fluid cognitive skills and crystallized knowledge tend to be
highly correlated. In fact, it may be accurate to say that schools like
the most effective schools in our study may be the first to produce
students for whom these two types of cognitive ability are consistently
decoupled, providing an opportunity to study just which kinds of
outcomes are enabled by gains in crystallized knowledge alone. For
example, it is possible that the oft-discussed challenges some students
from high-performing urban schools experience in college (see
"'No Excuses' Kids Go to College," features, Spring
2013) stem in part from deficits in fluid cognitive skills.
Indeed, perhaps the most important implication that we draw is that
educators seeking to innovate should get about the business of
developing and rigorously testing the effects of interventions to raise
these fluid cognitive skills. Improved abstract-reasoning capacity
likely has important benefits in its own right and is highly related to
important skills such as reading comprehension. Deficits in
students' fluid cognitive skills may also prevent even the most
effective schools from raising all of their students' academic
performance to the desired level.
The question of whether processing speed, working memory, and fluid
reasoning skills can be developed through intentional efforts is an area
of active debate among cognitive psychologists. Several researchers have
published studies claiming that they have improved these skills through
deliberate practice aimed at one or more of these skills and, in a few
cases, have shown that such improvements have translated into gains in
other, broader measures of cognitive ability. None of these
interventions has yet been shown to improve long-term outcomes such as
college completion or earnings, however, and other researchers have
failed to replicate even the narrower impacts that have been reported.
Meanwhile, private companies such as Lumosity are aggressively marketing
software-based training programs derived from this line of research to
the general public as "brain training."
This is a perfect time for cognitive psychologists, educators, and
perhaps even game and software developers to join forces in rapid-cycle
experimentation to explore whether and how schools can broadly and
permanently raise students' fluid cognitive skills. Successful
schools have demonstrated their ability to dramatically increase
crystallized knowledge and thereby raise test scores, improving other
important student outcomes in the process. Boosting fluid cognitive
skills might have an equally profound impact on students' academic
and life outcomes.
Stretching the cognitive limits on achievement
Psychologists now consider cognitive ability to have two primary
components: crystallized knowledge and fluid cognitive skills.
Crystallized knowledge comprises acquired knowledge such as
vocabulary and arithmetic, while fluid cognitive skills are
abstract-reasoning capabilities.
Effective schools help their students achieve at higher levels than
expected based on their fluid cognitive skills.
Educators seeking to innovate should look to develop and rigorously
test the effects of interventions to raise fluid cognitive skills.
by MARTIN R. WEST, CHRISTOPHER F. O. GABRIELI, AMY S. FINN, MATTHEW
A. KRAFT, and JOHN D. E. GABRIEL!
Martin West is an associate professor at the Harvard Graduate
School of Education (HGSE) and deputy director of the Program on
Education Policy and Governance at the Harvard Kennedy School.
Christopher Gabrieli is adjunct lecturer at HGSE and executive chairman
of the National Center on Time & Learning. Matthew Kraft is
assistant professor of education at Brown University. Amy Finn is a
postdoctoral fellow at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, where
John Gabrieli is professor of health sciences and technology and
cognitive neuroscience. This article is based on a study published in
the March 2014 issue of Psychological Science.
Gains in Crystallized Knowledge, but Not Fluid Cognitive
Skills (Figure 1)
Each year of attendance at an oversubscribed Boston
charter school increases math test scores by 13
percent of a standard deviation, but does not
significantly change fluid cognitive skills.
Crystallized knowledge (acquired
(acquired knowledge and skills)
Math 0.13*
Reading 0.06
Fluid cognitive skills
(abstract-reasoning capabilities)
Processing speed 0.03
Working memory -0.01
Fluid reasoning 0.02
Fluid cognitive skills (summary) 0.04
* indicates statistical significance at the 95
percent confidence Level
NOTE: Math and reading test scores are from the
Massachusetts Comprehensive Assessment System
(MCAS).
SOURCE: Authors' calculations