Blink. Think. Blank. Bunk. Solid snap judgements are deeply grounded.
Ravitch, Diane
Blink: The Power of Thinking without Thinking
By Malcolm Gladwell
Little, Brown, 2005, $25.95; 288 pages.
Think: Why Crucial Decisions Can't Be Made in the Blink of an
Eye
By Michael R. LeGault
Threshold Editions, 2006, $24.95; 386 pages.
Blank: The Power of Not Actually Thinking at All
By Noah Tall
Harper, 2006, $11.95; 96 pages.
I wish I had a dollar for every time I have heard or read a paean to the importance of critical thinking skills. (Just for fun, I Googled
the phrase and came up with over one million hits.) Often, the
pedagogues who champion critical thinking skills insist that such skills
are of far greater value to children than "mere knowledge,"
"mere facts," or what they derisively refer to as
"content." In other words, if students learn how to think,
then it matters not at all if they never read great literature or study
history.
Now along comes celebrated author Malcolm Gladwell to tell us in
his best-selling Blink that intuition is far superior to the critical
thinking skills that so many educators prize. Reflection and deep
thought are out, it seems, and judgments made on the fly are in.
Not only was Blink a huge, long-running bestseller, but it boosted
Mr. Gladwell into the ranks of megastars on the lecture circuit, where
he is now paid $40,000 or so to dispense his theories to corporate
executives. Gladwell's ideas refute the schools' labored
efforts to teach critical thinking, which usually refers to gathering
facts, reflecting on their meaning, and analyzing available evidence to
reach a judgment. Instead, Gladwell celebrates instinct, first
impressions, decisions made "at a glance," the power of the
unconscious. If the schools were to take his advice to heart, they would
soon be teaching neither knowledge nor critical thinking skills, and we
could treat them as daycare centers rather than academic institutions.
Gladwell is surely a talented writer, as one would expect of a
regular contributor to the New Yorker. He skillfully relates a series of
tales intended to show the power of snap judgments. His first example
involves a decision by the Getty Museum to buy a remarkably intact Greek
sculpture from the 6th century B.C. for nearly $10 million. Since this
investment demanded a high degree of caution, the museum hired
scientists to investigate the age of the piece. The scientists probed
and analyzed and concluded that the sculpture was genuine.
When the museum invited several art experts to look at the statue,
they immediately and correctly called it a fake, based on their
instincts about what was real and what was not. But in this tale, as in
most of the others that Gladwell cites, the person who makes the alleged
snap judgment is someone who has spent years accumulating the knowledge
to make a fast and accurate decision. It was not as if the Getty called
in a dozen Joe Six-Packs from the street; no, it listened with anguish
to people who had spent their professional lives learning to tell the
difference between real and fake.
Gladwell's argument simply doesn't hold water. Blink
decisions are only worthwhile when they are made by people with years of
experience. Even then, as he readily acknowledges, blink decisions are
often wrong. Sometimes they are simply prejudice. Other times, they are
wrong because acting on instinct can lead to wrong judgments.
The notorious killing of African immigrant Amadou Diallo in 1999 by
four members of the New York City Police Department was a blink
decision. The police saw Diallo late at night standing in front of an
apartment building in a poor neighborhood in the Bronx. They called to
him and he didn't answer. They shouted, and he reached into his
pocket for his wallet. They made a snap decision that he was reaching
for a gun; they drilled 41 shots into him. They were wrong, and he was
dead.
Michael R. LeGault apparently had a book in the works about the
decline of American culture and society and his publisher was looking
for a title to hold the thing together. When Gladwell's work became
a big bestseller, it seemed like good marketing sense to call
LeGault's book Think, as if it were written in response to
Gladwell. Think contains no primary research, no fresh insights. Mostly
it is an unremitting complaint about the degradation of American life by
purveyors of pop culture, pop psychology, feel-good experts, and
marketing gurus.
In his book Blank, Noah Tall (a pseudonym) gives an excellent
reason to read Malcolm Gladwell's Blink: you can't understand
the brilliant humor of Blank until you have read Blink. Tall offers an
ironic version of each of Gladwell's case histories to show how
ridiculous the blink judgment actually is. He, too, finds psychologists
working on exotic theories of human behavior. For example, there is the
TAVIACI syndrome, which means The Average Voter Is A Complete Idiot.
"By a coincidence that can only be called extraordinary," he
writes, "it was discovered by Dr. Gaetano Taviaci of Vesuvio
University & Pizzeria in Naples." Even Abraham Lincoln,
"an ambitious gay activist from Illinois, failed to impress voters
until he started wearing a hat taller than anyone else's."
That distinction enabled him to capture the entire idiot vote, which was
enough to get him elected.
Then there is Dr. Ian Plegg, who mapped out "the third
hemisphere" of the brain. Plegg, the first man ever to receive a
doctorate in Scientology, discovered the "little-known lower
subbasement hemisphere, or the LSBH." He would have won a Nobel
Prize for his work, "but some of his more envious colleagues
pointed out that 'hemi' comes from the Greek meaning
'half,' and that technically you can't have a third
half."
As for the Greek sculpture that fooled the Getty Museum, Blank
transports it to the Oprah Winfrey Museum of Fine Art in Cicero,
Illinois. The curator brings in the scientists, who confirm its
antiquity; along comes a postal worker eating a limburger sandwich who
says the statue doesn't smell right. Then a bevy of art experts
declares it a fake. The curator takes it to the ultimate experts at the
television program Antiques Roadshow, who declare it to be definitely
19th century. Having paid $9.7 million for the phony statue, the
dejected curator auctions it on eBay for $2,750. (When the statue turns
out to be genuine, the curator shoots himself.)
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
The bottom line, I surmise, is that it takes years and years of
deep study to become truly expert so that you are then qualified to make
snap decisions.
Diane Ravitch is research professor of education, New York
University, and a member of the Koret Task Force at the Hoover
Institution, Stanford University.
As reviewed by Diane Ravitch