On the U.S. budget deficit dilemma: has television contributed?
Darrat, Ali F. ; Bowers, Bill P.
I. Introduction
According to many writers and observers, television has become one of
the most powerful vehicles ever invented to influence human behavior,
[e.g., LaHaye (1979), Potter (1990), Morrow (1992), and Andersen
(1993)]. They contend that television has largely shaped the young
generation's values, affected people's performance in the
workplace, and has been closely linked to many social problems like
violence and crime. Increasingly, people turn to television to learn
what society considers appropriate (or inappropriate) behavior in all
walks of life.
In this light, it seems reasonable to speculate that television
exposure may be at the center of many contemporary public policy
problems. The basic aim of this paper is to examine the nature of the
relationship (if any) between growth in television and one of the most
important economic issues of our time; namely, the federal budget
deficit delimma. More precisely, we attempt to address the following
question: Has the influence of television on the American public opinion
contributed significantly to the recent escalation in the federal budget
deficit? Section II of the paper provides some theoretical arguments
linking television to the federal budgetary process. Section III
discusses the data and empirical methodology used. Section IV presents
the empirical results. Section V concludes.
II. Some Theoretical Issues
The power of television to form, shape, mold and even direct public
opinion is well documented and is often a generally accepted contention.
Once aroused, public opinion then becomes a primary determinant of
public policy, or more precisely, the taxing and spending policies of
the federal government [see Erickson (1976), Monroe (1979), Page and
Shapiro (1983), and Williams (1990)].
In their recent examination of foreign policymaking, Jordan and Page
(1992) report that the statements and actions of television commentators
and the allegedly nonpartisan experts have had the largest impacts upon
the public policy preferences of the public. Similar findings linking
television to domestic policy actions have also been documented by Page,
Shapiro and Dempsey (1987). To the extent that television influences
public opinion, and given the reasonable assumption that policymakers
are somewhat sensitive to public choice, it follows that television
viewership could play a significant role in determining the federal
budget deficit.
That television has contributed to the budget deficit dilemma is also
suggested by the claim of some analysts that the television (news)
industry generally presents a "liberal" political bias
favoring government intervention in the economy [LaHaye (1979), Henry
(1992), and Zoglin (1992)]. This liberal bias generates increased
support for government funded programs and thus increased government
spending. Personal experience might also suggest that television has
influenced public opinion which ultimately lead to increased government
spending and thus resulted in the huge deficit. In this regard, we might
highlight the substantial role played by the media, particularly the TV,
to direct the attention of the public to demand government action about
child abuse, the homeless, the elderly, AIDS victims, and the like. More
recently, the almost daily images from places such as Somalia,
Bosnia-Herzegovina, Rwanda, Haiti and the former Soviet Union have
brought mounting pressures on the federal government to act, and that
action has almost always resulted in higher government spending [Goodman
(1993), and Neuman (1993)].(1) Others might hypothesize that TV
encourages military build ups and thus the deficit.
Preliminary evidence on the television/budgetary process linkage is
indicated by the positive and highly significant correlation (r = 0.79,
t = 8.25) between the growth in television, as proxied for example by TV
real advertising expenditures and the growth of the real national debt
over the past four decades (1950-1992). As seen in Figure 1, television
viewership (TV advertising) and national debt (both in real terms) tend
to be closely associated since the mid 1960s. A similar picture (not
shown here to conserve space) emerges for the close link between growth
in television and federal budget deficit (or government spending). Of
course, one cannot look at the figure and directly discern whether
television growth is behind the escalation in the federal deficit
(debt). To do that, a more formal test of "causality" is
required, a task performed in this paper.
The preceding discussion appears to suggest the presence of a linkage
from television-to-budget deficit which we subject below to empirical
testing. Both economic theory and empirical evidence have shown that a
number of other macroeconomic variables appear to have influenced the
size of the federal budget deficit. Therefore, confining the causality
test to the restrictive bivariate model (with only television growth and
the federal budget deficit) may yield biased causality inferences due to
the "omission of variables" phenomenon [see Sims (1980), and
Lutkepohl (1982)]. To avoid such a bias, we expand our testing model and
allow for the possible influence of other key determinants of the
federal budget deficit.
In particular, the U.S. Employment Act of 1946, and its reaffirmation by the Humphrey-Hawkins' Balanced Growth Act of 1978, stipulate that the federal government is responsible for achieving and maintaining
low unemployment. Economic theory also suggests [see Naylor (1985) and
Tokheim (1993)] that fluctuations in private domestic investment impact
the federal budget deficit through changes in corporate income taxes,
investment tax credits, and depreciation schedules. Moreover, several
studies have found evidence of a significant effect of interest rates on
the size of the budget deficit [e.g., Laumas and McMillin (1984), and
Darrat (1991)]. Indeed, prominent political figures (like Congressman
and former Presidential candidate Jack Kemp) have repeatedly argued that
high interest rates, and the resulting increases in debt servicing, are
largely responsible for the recent escalation in federal deficits.
Another potentially important variable that could play a significant
role in shaping the deficit process is the trade deficit in what is now
known as the "twin deficits" phenomenon [see Hutchison and
Pigott (1984), and Darrat (1988)]. Darrat (1988), in particular,
theorizes that the falling off of net exports appears to have imposed
increasing pressures upon the federal government to aid domestic
industries and the farming sectors that are hard hit by declining
foreign market shares. The adverse economic and financial consequences
of trade deficits are often viewed with much concern by the U.S.
business community, by labor unions and, perhaps as a result, also by
government authorities. Besides these possible determinants, we have
also considered a number of other candidates like the monetary base,
real GNP and the inflation rate. However, none of these variables proved
empirically important for determining the budgetary process, and were
thus deleted.(2)
III. Data and Methodology
We use U.S. annual data spanning the period 1950 through 1992 to test
the effect of television growth on the size of the federal budget
deficit in the context of a multivariate Graner-causality model. Lack of
quarterly or monthly data on the television proxies necessitated the use
of annual time series.
One main proxy to measure growth in television viewership could be
total television advertising expenditures or revenues (TVA). It can be
reasonably hypothesized that television viewership and (TVA) are
strongly and positively correlated, and that (TVA) react to variations
in viewership rather quickly (within one year horizon). These
contentions have received some empirical support in the recent marketing
literature [e.g., Friedman (1989), and Schmuckler (1992)]. When the
rating (viewership) falls for any given TV program, rational behavior
dictates that advertisers switch their expenditures towards alternative
TV and other non-TV programs (like sales promotions and radio
advertising). Indeed, advertisers may even elect to reduce the total
amount of funds allocated to advertising when TV viewership declines
[see Schmalensee (1983), and Rust et al (1992)]. Besides TVA, growth in
television may also be measured by changes in the number of TV stations
(TVS), or perhaps also by changes in the number of cable subscribers
(TVC). Of course, these three alternative proxies do not exhaust all
possible ways of measuring television viewership. Indeed, three more
choices seem particularly promising; namely, the total number of
television sets, the average number of television sets per household,
and the average number of hours of television watched per year.(3)
Although data on these latter measures is relatively scanty, we use the
available data on these alternative proxies of television viewership to
check the robustness of our empirical results.
Based on the preceding discussion, other explanatory variables in the
budget deficit model include the unemployment rate (UN), gross private
domestic investment (DI), short-term interest rates (RS), and the trade
deficit (TD). As to the dependent variable, it is measured by the
national income account version of the federal budget deficit (BD).
Since disagreement exists regarding the proper measure of the budget
deficit, it seems advisable to use other measures in order to check the
sensitivity of the results to the choice of variables. Therefore, we
alternatively used the federal debt held by the public (PD) to represent
the dependent variable.(4)
Data sources are the Economic Report of the President, and the Survey
of Current Business for all variables except for the three television
proxies. The latter series are compiled from various issues of the
Historical Statistics of the U.S., the Statistical Abstract of the U.S.,
the Advertising Age, and several publications of the New York based
McCann-Erickson, Inc.
The focus of the paper is on the causal linkage between television
growth and the federal budget deficit. As is common in applied
econometrics, we employ the concept of causality in the sense of Granger
(1969).(5) A given time series (x) is said to Granger-cause another time
series (y) if y can be better predicted by using past values of y and x
than by just using past values of y alone. The Granger-causality tests
require the use of stationary time series to generate reliable
inferences. Therefore, before inclusion in the empirical analysis, each
variable is transformed to achieve stationarity. This is done by
checking for the presence of unit roots using the Dickey-Fuller (DF) and
the Augmented Dickey-Fuller (ADF) tests. As Stock and Watson (1989)
point out, F and [[Chi].sup.2] statistics will not have standard
distributions if the variables under examination exhibit unit roots and
are thus nonstationary. Table 1 reports the test results from applying
the DF and the ADF tests (the latter test with one and two lags for the
dependent variable).(6) These results suggest that most variables are
stationary in first differences except for the three TV proxies (TVA,
TVS, and TVC), and also for (BD) which appear rather stationary in
levels.
Granger-causality tests further require the selection of the
appropriate lag profiles for the variables. Some studies, e.g., Mishkin
(1982), use a common lag on each variable. However, as Ahking and Miller
(1985) note, the assumption of an equal lag length on each variable is
too restrictive and could potentially bias the results. Therefore, we
use the Akaike final prediction error (FPE) criterion to determine the
proper lag profile for each variable in the testing model. This
procedure has recently gained popularity in applied econometrics and
thus its full details are not reproduced here to conserve space.(7)
Essentially, the FPE procedure uses a stepwise construction of
univariate, then, bivariate, trivariate . . . etc., regressions such
that the FPE value is minimized as each variable (lag) is added
sequentially. This FPE procedure can be thought of as a sequential
F-test where the appropriate lag is that which balances the decline in
the residual variance due to the added lag against the reduced
efficiency of the estimation as the lag length increases.
Since the tests performed here are multivariate in nature, another
issue arises; namely, the order by which the variables are included in
the testing equations. This aspect of the methodology is important in
light of the fact that causality inferences may not be invariant with
respect to the entry order. Following recent literature, we rank the
variables for inclusion based on the "specific gravity"
criterion of Caines et al (1981). The resulting equations are also
examined for the presence of residual autocorrelation and for structural
instability of the parameters. [TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE 1 OMITTED] In an
important, but surprisingly neglected work, Lutkepohl (1989)
demonstrates that structural stability of the estimated equation is a
key prerequisite for reliable Granger-causality tests. The final step in
the procedure is to test for the joint significance of the lagged
coefficients and then derive the Granger-causality inferences.
IV. Empirical Results
Based on the procedures outlined in the previous section, Table 2
reports the empirical results from estimating alternative models of the
U.S. federal budget deficit over the annual period 1950-1992.(8) Panel
(A) in the table displays the results using the television advertising
expenditures proxy (TVA), Panel (B) displays the results using instead
the number of TV cable subscribes (TVC), while panel (C) reports the
results using the number of TV stations (TVS) as the measure of TV
viewership.
As the table shows, the proposed model fits the data quite well,
judged by the high and significant values of adjusted R-squares (ranging
from 0.72 to 0.89). Several autocorrelation tests could not reject the
hypothesis of independent errors. This implies, among other things, that
the t-values reported in the table are reliable measures of the
significance of the coefficients, and that the estimated equations are
unlikely to suffer from a serious omission of variables. Another
evidence for the adequacy of the estimated equations is the apparent
absence of significant structural instability using alternative testing
procedures. For several breaking dates, the Chow test could not reject
the null hypothesis of parameter stability, a finding corroborated by
the Farley and Hinich test.(9)
The most striking finding in the results of Table 2 is that
television viewership, however measured, exerts a significant causal and
positive impact upon the U.S. budget deficit.(10) Specifically, the null
hypothesis that television viewership (TVA, TVC or TVS) does not
Granger-cause higher federal budget deficits is soundly rejected at
better than the 5 percent level (the corresponding t-statistics are
7.71, 7.69, and 3.61 respectively). Of course, the Granger-causality
inferences are usually tested by means of the F-statistics. These
F-statistics are reported in Table 3 and they too corroborate the
evidence that television viewership exerts a significant causal effect
on the federal budget deficit (the associated F-values are 49.13 for
TVA, 48.73 for TVC, and 12.19 for TVS). Interestingly, the FPE procedure
reveals that the appropriate lag length for each of the TV viewership
proxies is rather short (one year). [TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE 2 OMITTED]
Therefore, it appears that the impact of television viewership on the
budgetary process is both significant and relatively swift as well. To
verify the sensitivity of these results to using alternative measures of
television viewership, we use the total number of television sets, the
[TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE 3 OMITTED] average number of television sets per
household, or the average number of hours of television watched per year
to represent television viewership in our equations. For the available
data (from the same sources as for other viewership proxies) over the
annual series 1975-1993, the estimated coefficients are all positive as
hypothesized and are generally statistically significant at least at the
10 percent level (the t-statistics are 1.95, 2.33, and 1.12 for the
coefficients on TVA, TVC, and TVS respectively).
Still, one might also argue that it is the size of the government,
rather than the deficit itself, that matters in most public policy
discussions. Thus, we perform another set of regressions using the
deficit as a percent of GDP to represent the dependent variable. The
estimated coefficients on all three measures of television viewership
are positive as expected and are highly statistically significant at
better than the 5 percent level (t = 4.04 on the TVA proxy, t = 3.39 on
the TVC proxy, and t = 4.09 on the TVS proxy).
All in all, then, it appears that the increase in television
viewership (however measured) has contributed significantly to the
escalation in government budget deficit (measured either in absolute
values or as a percent of GDP).
Before concluding, we should point out that these multivariate
Granger-causality tests are also useful for detecting other potential
causal factors for the federal budget deficit. For example, Tables 2 and
3 suggest that domestic private investment, the trade deficit,
unemployment and interest rates are all important determinants of the
U.S. federal budget deficit. It also bears emphasis that the
coefficients on the interest rate variable are statistically significant
and consistently positive across equations. That is, rising interest
rates have Granger-caused high federal budget deficits. This finding
supports recent studies on fiscal policy reaction functions [e.g.,
Laumas and McMillin (1984), Bradley and Potter (1986), and Darrat
(1991)]. It further suggests that the observed positive association
between federal budget deficits and interest rates in the U.S. is, at
least partly, the outcome of interest rates causing budget deficits as
some prominent analysts and observers have recently contended [e.g.,
Jack Kemp and Robert Eisner]. If valid, these results imply that budget
deficits cannot be treated as an exogenous variable in interest rate
single-equation models as some researchers have done [for example,
DeLeeuw and Holloway (1985), Hoelscher (1986), Evans (1987), Plosser
(1987), and Zahid (1988)]. Failure to recognize the endogeneity of
budget deficits to interest rates may cast some doubts on the
reliability of these studies due to the simultaneity bias problem. This
also suggests that a more fruitful inquiry into the relationship between
budget deficits and interests rates in the U.S. should be performed in
the context of a simultaneous-equation model.
V. Concluding Remarks
Based on several theoretical arguments, this paper examines
empirically whether growth in television viewership has significantly
contributed to the recent escalation in the U.S. budget deficits over
the last four decades. We specify a model for the federal budget that
includes alternative measures of the importance of television viewing
along with a number of other potentially important macro determinants of
the deficit. In the context of such a multivariate model, we focus on
the Granger-concept of causality and use the FPE procedure in
conjunction with the specific gravity criterion to derive our
inferences. Before implementing the empirical procedure, we check the
stationarity requirements for all series by means of alternative unit
root tests.
The empirical results suggest that recent growth in the U.S.
television viewership (however measured) has significantly contributed
to the size of the federal budget deficit, over and above the effects of
several other possible determinants (most notably interest rates,
private domestic investment, and the unemployment rate). This finding
implies some support to the view that there exists a "liberal"
bias within the media and within television in particular that
undermines fiscal conservatism.
Of course, given the relative brevity of our sample and in light of
possible estimation or measurement problems, the results of this paper
are only suggestive and should be interpreted with caution. At the very
least, though, these results do suggest that the recent growth in the
U.S. television viewership is an important contributing factor behind
the escalating federal budget deficit. Policy-makers, therefore, should
not ignore the role of television if they hope to understand and
ultimately control the U.S. budget deficit dilemma.
Notes
1. For further discussion, interested readers are referred to the
recent survey of some relevant issues in Alesina and Perotti (1994).
2. The growth in government budget deficits since the 1970s may also
be related to the dramatic increase of entitlement expenditures. No
attempt, however, is made here to examine every possible reason for the
deficit explosion. Our primary objective in this paper is to examine
whether television viewership in particular has played any important
role in the recent escalation of the U.S. budget deficit.
3. We thank the anonymous referee for suggesting these proxies to us.
4. Of course, other measures may also be used for the dependent
variable like federal government spending, for some related
disaggregates (e.g., spending or social programs).
5. Note that controversy surrounds the Granger-causality concept and
the implied tests. For a concise account of these difficulties, see
Jacobs et al (1979), and Zellner (1979). Many researchers view the
Granger-causality procedure as merely a test of the "predictive
content" of the time series.
6. A linear time trend was also included in all testing equations and
proved statistically significant.
7. For a useful account, see Ahking and Miller (1985). Note that
Thornton and Batten (1985) report evidence supporting the use of the FPE
criterion over several other lag-length selection procedures, including
the Akaike Information (AIC) and the Schwartz criterion.
8. To ensure statistical efficiency of the estimations, we used the
Beach-Mckinnon maximum-likelihood procedure to estimate all equations.
9. The Chow test yielded the following F-statistics for the TVA, TVC
and TVS equations respectively: 0.51, 0.59, and 2.85 for the mid-point;
1.35, 2.77, and 1.30 for the pre- and post-Reagan periods; 0.11, 0.91,
and 0.11 for the pre- and post- Johnson periods; and 0.11, 0.91, and
0.11 for the pre- and post-Vietnam years. The Farely and Hinich test of
a gradual structural shift produced the following F-statistics: 1.57,
2.70, and 1.71 or the three equations respectively.
10. The use of the federal debt held by the public as the dependent
variable produced similar conclusions. These results are not reported
here to conserve space but are available from the authors upon request.
References
Ahking, F. W., and S. M. Miller, "The Relationship Between
Government Deficits, Money Growth, and Inflation." Journal of
Macroeconomics 7 (Fall 1985), 447-467.
Alesina, A., and R. Perotti, "The Political Economy of Budget
Deficits." NBER Working Paper No. 4637 (February 1994).
Andersen, K. E., "The Politics of Ethics and the Ethics of
Politics." American Behavioral Scientist 32 (March/April 1989),
479-492.
Bradley, M. D., and S. M. Potter, "The State of the Federal
Budget and the State of the Economy: Further Evidence." Economic
Inquiry 24 (January 1986), 143-153.
Caines, P. E., C. W. Kang, an S. P. Sethi, "Causality Analysis
and Multivariate Autoregressive Modelling with an Application to
Supermarket Sales Analysis." Journal of Economic Dynamics and
Control 3 (August 1981), 267-298.
Darrat, A. F., "Have Large Budget Deficits Caused Rising Trade
Deficits?" Southern Economic Journal 54 (April 1988), 879-887.
-----, "Policy Impacts Under Rational Expectations."
Journal of Banking and Finance 15 (April 1991), 257-271.
DeLeeuw, G., and T. M. Holloway, "The Measurement and
Significance of the Cyclically Adjusted Federal Budget and Debt."
Journal of Money, Credit and Banking 17 (May 1985), 232-242.
Erickson, R., "The Relationship Between Public Opinion and State
Policy: A New Look at Some Forgotten Data." American Journal of
Political Science 20 (February 1976), 25-36.
Evans, P., "Interest Rates and Expected Future Budget Deficits
in the United States." Journal of Political Economy 95 (February
1987), 34-58.
Friedman, L., "How Good is the Seven-Day TV Diary Now?"
Journal of Advertising Research 29 (August/September 1989), RC1-RC3.
Goodman, W., "Silent Partner Emerging in Policy Councils:
TV." The New York Times (March 6, 1993), section L, 47.
Granger, C. W. J., "Investigating Causal Relations by
Econometric Models and Cross-Spectral Methods." Econometrica 37
(July 1969), 424-438.
Henry, W.A., "Are the Media Too Liberal?" Time (October 19,
1992), 46-47.
Hoelscher, G. P., "New Evidence on Deficits and Interest
Rates." Journal of Money, Credit and Banking 18 (February 1986),
1-17.
Hutchison, M. M., and C. Pigott, "Budget Deficits, Exchange
Rates and the Current Account: Theory and U.S. Evidence." Federal
reserve Bank of San Francisco, Economic Review (Fall 1984), 5-25.
Jordan, D., and B. Page, "Shaping Foreign Policy Opinions: The
Role of TV News." Journal of Conflict Resolution 36 (June 1992),
227-241.
Jacobs, R. L., E. E. Leamer, and M. P. Ward, "Difficulties with
Testing for Causation." Economic Inquiry 17 (July 1979), 401-413.
LaHaye, T. The Battle for the Family (Old Tappan, New Jersey: Fleming
H. Revell Company, 1979).
Laumas, G. S., and W. D. McMillin, "Anticipated Fiscal Policy
and Real Output." Review of Economics and Statistics 66 (August
1984), 468- 471.
Lutkepohl, H., "Non-Causality Due to Omitted Variables."
Journal of Econometrics 19 (August 1982), 367-378.
-----, "The Stability Assumption in Tests of Causality Between
Money and Income," in Econometrics of Structural Changes: Studies
in Empirical Economics, edited by Watter Kramer (Gelmany: physica-Verlag
Heidelberg, 1989).
Mishkin, F. S., "Does Anticipated Aggregate Demand Policy
Matter? Further Econometric Results." American Economic Review 72
(September 1982), 688-802.
Monroe, A. D., "Consistency Between Public Preferences and
National Policy Decisions." American Politics Quarterly (March
1979), 3-19.
Morrow, L., "Folklore in a Box." Time 140 (September 21,
1992), 591-602.
Naylor, J. A., "A Survey of Post-1970 Empirical Studies of
Investment Expenditures," in Modern Concepts in Macroeconomics,
edited by T. Haverlesky, and J. T. Boorman (Arlington Heights, ILL:
Harlan Davidson, Inc., 1988).
Neuman, J., "Few Bosnia Options for Clinton." USA Today (April 19, 1993), p. 4A.
Page, B., and R. Shapiro, "Effects of Public Opinion on
Policy." American Political Science Review 77 (March 1983),
175-190.
-----. and G. Dempsey, "What Moves Public Opinion?"
American Political Science Review 81 (March 1987), 23-44.
Potter, W. J., "Adolescents' Perceptions of the Primary
Values of Television Programming." Journalism Quarterly 67 (Winter
1990), 843-851.
Rust, R., W. Kamakura, and M. Alpert, "Viewer Preference
Segmentation and Viewing Choice Models for Network Television."
Journal of Advertising 21 (March 1992), 1-16.
Schmalensee, D. H., "Today's Top Priority Advertising
Research Questions." Journal of Advertising Research 23 (April/May
1983), 49-60.
Schmuckler, E., "Buyers are Salivating Over Increases in Net GRP Pool." Mediaweek (June 1, 1992), 4.
Sims, C. A., "Macroeconomics and Reality." Econometrica 48
(January 1980), 1-48.
Thornton, D. L., and D. S. Batten, "Lag-Length Selection and
Tests of Granger-Causality Between Money and Income." Journal of
Money, Credit and Banking 17 (May 1985), 164-178.
Tokheim, A. B. Legislative and Policy Update (Washington, D.C.:
Council on Competitiveness, March 29, 1993).
Williams, W. Mismanaging America (Lawrence, Kansas: The University of
Kansas Press, 1990).
Zahid, K. H., "Government Budget Deficits and Interest Rates:
The Evidence Since 1971 Using Alternative Deficit Measures."
Southern Economic Journal 54 (January 1988), 725-731.
Zellner, A., "Causality and Econometrics," in Three Aspects
of Policy and Policymaking: Knowledge, Data and Institutions, edited by
K. Brunner and A. H. Meltzer (Amsterdam, North Holland: North-Holland
Publishing Company, 1979).
Zoglin, R., "Sitcom Politics." Time (September 21, 1992),
44-47.
Ali F. Darrat and Bill P. Bowers
Professor of Economics, Department of Economics and Finance,
Louisiana Tech University, Ruston, LA 71272
Assistant Professor of Business Administration, Department of
Business Administration and Economics, Charleston Southern University,
Charleston, SC 29423
We wish to thank, without implicating, G. Brown, M. El-Baghdadi, and
an anonymous referee for several helpful comments and suggestions.