首页    期刊浏览 2024年11月29日 星期五
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Differentiating mass participant sport event consumers: traditional versus non-traditional events.
  • 作者:Buning, Richard J. ; Walker, Matthew
  • 期刊名称:Sport Marketing Quarterly
  • 印刷版ISSN:1061-6934
  • 出版年度:2016
  • 期号:March
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:Fitness Information Technology Inc.
  • 关键词:Event planners;Sports

Differentiating mass participant sport event consumers: traditional versus non-traditional events.


Buning, Richard J. ; Walker, Matthew


Introduction

Undeterred by the sometimes lofty financial costs and personal sacrifices required for participation, running races across the United States included approximately 19 million finishers at more than 28,000 events in 2013 (Running USA, 2014a). Exemplified by these data, mass participant sporting events (MPSEs) have increased in popularity due to an array of offerings that range from 5k running races to marathons. These events attract a variety of participants and offer challenges for a variety of skill levels and age groups. Murphy and Bauman (2007) identified three categories of mass physical activity-related events: (1) elite sporting events (e.g., Olympic Games), (2) non-elite mass events with the potential for community-wide participation, and (3) major population-level health promotion events. An emerging body of literature has focused on understanding the unique participant experiences of non-elite mass events (Funk, Jordan, Ridinger, & Kaplanidou, 2011; Kaplanidou & Vogt, 2010). Additional research on MPSEs has shown these events can catalyze physical activity (Funk et al., 2011; Crofts, Dickson, Schofield, & Funk, 2012), provide benefits to the host city through active sport tourism (Kaplanidou, Jordan, Funk, & Ridinger, 2012), and provide economic impacts to the host region (Coleman & Ramchandani, 2010).

Since running events have become increasingly popular, and the marketplace is becoming ever more crowded, event organizers must differentiate their offerings. Among the more novel approaches are nontraditional events, which are typically organized around a central theme such as obstacle races or color runs. Non-traditional events tend to offer a less competitive environment focused more on fun, fitness, camaraderie, and achievement compared to traditional events that focus only on running a specified distance for a desired time (Running USA, 2014b). Due to their unique appeal, non-traditional events often attract a large proportion of first-time participants with some events being comprised of up to 60% of entrants with no previous running event experience (Running USA, 2014b). This segment of the US running industry has grown in recent years as the number of finishers since 2009 has doubled annually to an estimated four million in 2014. These data affirm that traditional events (i.e., half marathons and marathons) are becoming less popular with only 2.5 million finishers over the same time period (Running USA, 2014b). Still, the running event market remains dominated by 5k and 10k distance events, which recorded a combined 9.78 million finishers in 2013 (Running USA, 2014a). One of the more popular non-traditional events are obstacle races, in which participants complete a 3-10 mile run over obstacles in muddy conditions. We might assume that consumers of these events have different motives for participation that focus on leisure and social bonding, rather than personal achievement due to the differences in organization and environment. However, research has yet to identify if differences actually exist.

The obstacle race industry is relatively new with modern events beginning in 1999 and current industry leaders beginning operations as recently as 2010 (Babbitt, 2012; Keneally, 2012). According to one source, in 2013 more than 100 companies organized obstacle races, resulting in 3.4 million event finishers and $290 million in revenue (Obstacle Race World, 2014). Still, an argument can be made against obstacle races as a legitimate sporting event. However, Loy (1972) contended that a commonly accepted description of sport is often debatable and difficult to interpret. Obstacle races possess several attributes akin to more general sport options since they test athletic skills, have documented rules, employ standard distances, and are time-based contests (Desena & Weinberg, 2012).

Given the relative infancy of obstacle race events, little (if any) research has examined participant motives in this context. Understanding the motives specific to different MPSEs is an important area of inquiry as event promoters seek to differentiate their events in a congested marketplace. Although numerous articles have been published on MPSEs (e.g., Funk et al., 2011; Ridinger, Funk, Jordan, & Kaplanidou, 2012; Murphy & Bauman, 2007; Sato, Jordan, Kaplanidou, & Funk, 2014), the existing scholarly attention has focused primarily on traditional running and multisport events and overlooked potential differences between event contexts. Different events may attract dissimilar individuals seeking varied motivational experiences. Thus, a better understanding of the motivational profiles of event participants will aid event organizers in event planning in leveraging the outcomes of event participation. The purpose of the study was to assess the validity of a traditional running motivation scale in a modern context and examine if motivational differences exist across event categories (i.e., obstacle races and half marathons).

Theoretical Framework

Defined as an internal factor that arouses, directs and integrates a person's behavior" (Murray, 1964, p. 7), motivation has been explored in a variety of fields but largely stems from psychology. According to Maslow (1970), human motivation is directed at the fulfillment of fundamental needs (i.e., physiological, safety, belonging,) to advanced needs (i.e., esteem, self-actualization). Researchers investigating motivation have often relied on the self-determining aspects of individual volitional behavior to drive motivations. Self-determination theory (SDT) is thought to be derived from intrinsic (e.g., interests) and extrinsic sources (e.g., rewards), which are influenced by personal and cultural forces associated with autonomy, competence, and relatedness (Deci & Ryan, 2002). Autonomy describes an individual's ability to govern his or her own behavior, whereas competence refers to an individual's perceived ability to control an outcome, and relatedness is concerned with the desire to be connected to others (Deci & Ryan, 2000).

As conditions are created that support an individual's autonomy, competence, and relatedness, an increased motivation that leads to higher engagement in an activity such as increased performance, persistence, and creativity is witnessed (Deci & Ryan, 2000). This idea is especially pronounced in the context of leisure activities since performing them reflects and reinforces the characteristics of SDT. Accordingly, leisure motivation is considered to be a need, reason, or satisfaction that catalyzes an individual's involvement in a leisure activity, and an internal factor that subsequently drives behavior (Crandall, 1980; IsoAhola, 1982).

Early research on leisure motivation by Beard and Ragheb (1983) identified four motivational components. First, the intellectual component is associated with motivation to participate in an activity that encompassed mental activities such as exploring, learning, discovering, creating, and/or imagining. Second, the social component pertains to social motivation, driven by needs for friendship and relationships, and esteem of other individuals. The third component, competence-mastery, pertains to individual desires to achieve, master, challenge, and compete. Lastly, the stimulus-avoidance component is associated with the intent to escape and get away from over-stimulating life situations.

Leisure motives vary based on individual circumstances and are often linked to event preferences. For instance, using a push-pull motivation approach, Snelgrove and Wood (2010) found motives of supporting others, learning about a destination, and identity predicted event choice. Push motives are considered internal needs that create a desire to participate, while pull motives are related to external factors such as an event attractiveness (Crompton, 1979; Dann, 1977, 1981). Further, Snelgrove and Wood (2010) argued that although first-time event visitors might be initially motivated by physical event characteristics, this motivation may decline over time as they create a stronger identity related to the sport.

Based on the unique aspects of non-traditional events and considering the potential differences between the participants attracted to traditional and non-traditional events the concept of serious leisure provides us a fundamental understanding of the range of leisure involvement. Stebbins (1992) argued that leisure involvement ranged from casual to serious and that serious leisure is "... the systematic pursuit of an amateur, hobbyist, or volunteer core activity that is highly substantial, interesting, and fulfilling and where, in the typical case, participants find a career in acquiring and expressing a combination of its special skills, knowledge, and experiences" (p. 3). Perhaps individuals that participate in traditional events are focused on the core product of running and competing, while non-traditional event participants are more concerned with the ancillary event products (e.g., obstacles and themes) making them less "serious." Research on traditional running events supports this idea by linking serious leisure to marathon running (Shipway & Jones, 2007, 2008).

[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]

The MOMS Scale

The Motivations of Marathoners Scale (MOMS), developed by Masters, Ogles, and Jolton (1993), is one of the most comprehensive and widely used scales for measuring endurance event participant motives. Although many leisure motives apply to these event types (Beard & Ragheb, 1983), Masters et al. (1993) suggested a comprehensive measure was needed to assess the specific motives of individuals participating in and training for running events. Based on previous distance running research (Carmack & Martens, 1979; Masters & Lambert, 1989; Curtis & McTeer, 1981), Masters et al. (1993) identified four broad categories of running motives, each containing two or more subdimensions: (1) physical health (i.e., general health orientation and weight concern), (2) social motives (i.e., affiliation and recognition), (3) achievement (i.e., competition and personal goal achievement), and (4) psychological motives (i.e., psychological coping, self-esteem, and life meaning). Masters et al. found the motives for health, personal achievement, and self-esteem to be more important than social motives for event participation. Subsequent research using the MOMS has continued to provide evidence of the validity and reliability of the related motives (Masters & Ogles, 1995; Ogles, Masters, Richardson, 1995; Ogles & Masters, 2000, 2003; Havenar & Lochbaum, 2007), but researchers have yet to empirically examine the nuanced differences among different event types.

Ogles and Masters (2000) found that marathon runner motives differed based on age, where older runners were more motivated by health concerns and affiliation, while younger runners were more motivated by achievement. Later, Ogles and Masters (2003) discovered that runners could be grouped into similar clusters based on motivation, and these clusters differed based on running experience, training patterns, and demographics. Havenar and Lochbaum (2007) used the MOMS to assess differences among first-time marathoners and found social motives to be higher for individuals who dropped out of competition compared to event finishers. Further, the MOMS has been used to assess motivation in a variety of other sporting contexts including cycling (LaChausse, 2006), 5k running (Bell & Stephenson, 2014), ultrarunning (Krouse, Ransdell, Lucas, & Pritchard, 2011), adventure races (Doppelmayr & Molkenthin, 2004), and triathlons (Rundio, Heere, & Newland, 2014). Although other scales have been developed to measure motives related to sport participation, the MOMS is the most comprehensive and applicable to running events. Other scales measuring sport participant motivation such as the Leisure Motivation Scale (Beard & Ragheb, 1983), the Sport Motivation Scale (Pelletier, Fortier, Vallerand, Tuson, & Bilas, 1995), and the Behavioral Regulation in Sport Questionnaire (Lonsdale, Hodge, & Rose, 2008) are quite general and fail to adequately measure motives related to health, weight loss, goals, and competition.

Recently, Rundio et al. (2014) compared the motives of participants from cause-related events and non-cause related events using the MOMS. The authors revealed that cause-related event participants rated motives related to self-esteem, personal goal achievement, competition, and recognition/approval significantly higher than the participants from the non-cause related events. Interestingly, the limited significance of social motives uncovered in previous research (Havenar & Lochbaum, 2007; Ogles & Masters, 2003; Masters et al., 1993) may be indicative of long-time participant samples in which attitudes toward participation were directly tied to competitive preferences (e.g., winning and best times). Recent shifts in participant motivation factors have suggested a need to reexamine this complex element of sport participant psychology. The discovery of a wider range of motives for MPSE participants (Funk et al., 2011) may be evidence of broadening leisure pursuits attached to MPSE participation (e.g., social bonding and getting out in nature). Funk et al. (2011) reported MPSE participants were propelled by a range of motives, but reiterated that social motives were relatively low among marathon participants.

Based on the lack of research on non-traditional participant sport events and the need to better understand participant motivation for modern MPSEs, the following research questions were developed to guide the study:

RQ 1: Does the Motivation of Marathoners Scale factor structure apply to traditional and non-traditional mass participant sport events?

RQ 2: Do participant motives differ across traditional and non-traditional mass participant sport events?

Method

Data Collection

To assess the research questions, data were collected using an online questionnaire distribution system. Four MPSEs in the Midwest United States throughout 2011 served as the research contexts. The event owners and researchers agreed to not have the events named in the research. Data were collected approximately two weeks prior to each event in order to remove potential bias from the influence of the actual event experience. Prospective respondents were invited to participate in the study via an emailed questionnaire link from the event organizers. Emails were obtained by the event organizers from the participant registration database. Following the recommendations of Dillman, Smyth, and Christian (2009), a three-contact strategy (i.e., initial invitation and two follow-up invitations) was utilized to encourage responses. Since the emails were confidential and protected by the event owner, the actual number distributed is unknown. Thus, an accurate response rate could not be calculated.

Instrumentation

The questionnaire was comprised of three main sections: (1) event evaluation items, (2) participant motivation items, and (3) demographics. The event evaluation items were included in the questionnaire as a requirement imposed by the event promoter. Participant motivations were measured using the multidimensional MOMS comprising 56 items under nine motivational factors (Masters et al., 1993): health orientation (six items), weight concern (four items), affiliation (six items), recognition (six items), competition (four items), personal goal achievement by (six items), psychological coping (nine items), self-esteem (eight items), and life meaning by (seven items). All of the items were rated on a seven-point scale (1 = not a reason to 7 = most important reason), as to the degree the participant considered the item a reason for event participation. Since the MOMS was administered to four different events, the wording of select individual scale items and the questionnaire directions were adapted to fit each event context. For example, the item "to share a group identity with other runners" was modified to "to share a group identity with others" for the non-traditional group. Finally, demographic items were used to assess participant gender and location. Traditional event data were collected at one half marathon, while the non-traditional event data were collected at three obstacle races. The obstacle races were a series of events held at multiple locations. All four events included in the study were produced and owned by the same event management company and were the primary offerings in their event portfolio.

Data Analysis

The data analysis consisted of several sequential steps. First, given the shift toward a broader definition of MPSE participant, the MOMS factor structure was assessed across the two event contexts via Mplus7.31 statistical modeling software. Due to the ordinal nature of the data and response distributions, a robust weighted least squares approach (WLSMV) was used to estimate the measurement model (Brown, 2006). Accordingly, single-group confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) models were examined for each group (i.e., traditional and non-traditional) to ensure the MOMS was a robust measure for both contexts. Second, a multi-group confirmatory factor analysis (MG-CFA) was performed to evaluate the MOMS structure between event types and to assess evidence of discriminant and convergent validity of the individual dimensions.

In order to test overall model fit for the individual CFA models and the MG-CFA, [chi square] goodness-of-fit, Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Tucker Lewis-Index (TLI), and Root Mean Square of Approximation (RMSEA) were calculated. Recommend cutoff values for fit indices vary. For example, Hu and Bentler (1999) suggest CFI and TLI values should be close to .95 and RMSEA should be close to .06 to decrease Type II error. Other sources suggest lower cutoff values as MaCallum, Browne, and Sugawara (1996) recommend RMSEA values of .01, .05, and .08 indicate excellent, good, and mediocre model fit, respectively, while Bentler and Bonnett (1980) suggest CFI and TLI scores greater than .90 designate good fit. When evaluating RMSEA the PCLOSE statistic is often evaluated concurrently. PCLOSE is a test for closeness of fit that RMSEA is below .05 in the population and should be greater than .05 (Browne & Cudek, 1993). However, Hair, Black, Babin, and Anderson (2010) argued that model fit should be assessed in regards to group size and the number of observed variables as simpler models with smaller samples should be subject to a stricter evaluation than complex models with larger samples.

Following the recommendations by Hair et al. (2010), for a model with more than 30 observed variables and a group size less than 250, goodness of fit is demonstrated by a significant [chi square] p-value, CFI and TFI above .92, and RMSEA less than .08. Internal consistencies of the nine factors were assessed with composite reliability scores (CR) while average variance extracted (AVE) values were used to assess evidence of convergent validity between latent variables (Hair et al., 2011; Fornell & Larcker, 1981). For descriptive purposes, summary statistics were calculated for the individual MOMS items and the nine motivational factors. Finally, to further compare the group differences between traditional and non-traditional events, a one-way multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted via SPSS 22.0 to examine the potential differences between each of the motivation factors based on event type (i.e., traditional and non-traditional).

Results

A total of 408 participants responded to the pre-event questionnaire from both event types, which exceeded the recommendations of Muthen, du Toit, and Spisic (1997) and Flora and Curran (2004) for the WLSMV estimation procedure. Responses from the divided sample based on event type consisted of non-traditional (n=275) and traditional (n=133) event participants. The total sample was 35.2% male and 64.8% female. The traditional event subsample was comprised of 63.5% females and 36.5% males, whereas the non-traditional event subsample was comprised of 65.4% females and 34.6% males. The highest-rated single item for the traditional sample was "to push myself beyond my current limits" (M=5.90, SD=1.47) and the lowest-rated single item was "to get a faster time than my friends" (M=2.26, SD=1.70). The non-traditional sample rated the single item "to participate with my family or friends" (M=5.84, SD=1.68) the highest and the single item "to have time alone in the world" (M=1.55, SD=1.19) the lowest.

Factor Analysis

For the first step of the factor analysis, separate CFAs were performed for each event type. Model fit criteria were based on the recommendations cited above. The traditional and non-traditional groups demonstrated mixed support for the hypothesized models (see Table 1 for all model fit statistics). The fit of the traditional group (CFI=.93, TLI=.92, RMSEA=.07) and the nontraditional group (CFI=.91, TLI=.90, RMSEA=.07) can be considered mediocre to good. Thus, single-step post hoc model modifications were performed to respecify the models by examining the modification indices and expected parameter change statistics (Kline, 2011; Kaplan, 1990, 1991; Boomsa, 2000). This procedure resulted in the deletion of three items (i.e., "to participate with family or friends," "to visit with friends," and "to beat someone I've never beaten before") due to low factor loadings and implausible modification indices.

The respecification process also resulted in three items (i.e., "to compete with myself," "to improve my sense of self-worth," and "to have time alone with the world") being set to load on different factors than originally designed by Masters et al. (1993). The subsequent 53-item multi-group model provided evidence of good model fit (CFI=.94, TLI=.94, RMSEA=.06). Although the PCLOSE statistic was not above the preferred value of .05, the multi-group model provided evidence of acceptable model fit according to the recommendations by Hair et al. (2011) among others.

Testing for measurement invariance was conducted to determine whether motivation was measured similarly across groups. Three measurement invariance tests were conducted as described by (Dimitrov, 2010): configural invariance (equal factor structure), metric invariance (invariant factor loadings), and scalar invariance (invariant factor loadings and intercepts).

As shown in Table 1, model fit remained stable as constraints were added, supporting measurement invariance across groups.

Reliability and Validity

We tested for evidence of convergent and discriminant validity. First, convergent validity was assessed based on three criteria: (1) the size of the individual factor loadings (average [lambda] = .81), (2) for each scale dimension the composite reliability score should exceed .70, and (3) the AVE score for each scale dimension should exceed .50 (Hair et al., 2010; Fornell & Larcker, 1981). For all nine motivation factors all of these criteria were successfully met for both groups. Each of the nine factors for both groups demonstrated high internal consistency with composite reliability scores ranging from .795-.967, which were above the recommended criterion of greater than .70 (Hair et al., 2010). With regard to evidence of convergent validity, all nine factors had acceptable AVE scores according to recommendations by Hair et al. (2010).

We next examined the multi-group model for discriminant validity, which is particularly challenging for multi-dimensional scaling because of the strong relationships between dimensions. Therefore, and considering some high correlations between dimensions, one additional test was performed to ensure the dimensions were independent. Following the suggestion of Fornell and Larcker (1981), if the square root of each construct's AVE is greater than the inter-construct correlations, evidence of discriminant validity is present. The results revealed this final condition was met (see Table 3). Correlations and descriptive statistics are presented in Table 3. The highest rated motivation factor for the overall participant sample was self-esteem (M=4.59, SD=1.54), followed by affiliation (M=3.76, SD=1.58), personal goal achievement (M=3.73, SD=1.65), health-orientation (M=3.55, SD=1.78), weight concern (M=2.98, SD=2.03), life meaning (M=2.83, SD=1.77), competition (M=2.80, SD=1.55), recognition (M=2.64, SD=1.53), and psychological coping (M=2.34, SD=1.38).

Traditional vs. Non-Traditional Events

To answer RQ2, a MANOVA with event type (i.e., traditional vs. non-traditional) as the independent variable and the revised MOMS factors as the dependent variables revealed a statistically significant omnibus main effect (Pillai's Trace=.436, F (9, 398)=34.231, p<.001, [[eta].sup.2]=.436). Thus, significant differences between event participants based on the nine motivation factors were present (see Table 4). Traditional event participants rated personal goal achievement (M=4.93, SD=1.46), health orientation (M=4.85, SD=1.59), self-esteem (M=4.62, SD=1.46), and weight concern (M=4.29, SD=1.80) as the most important motives, whereas the least important motives were recognition (M=2.80, SD=1.55) and psychological coping (M=3.14, SD=1.61). The traditional event participants were moderately motivated by competition (M=3.64, SD=1.32), affiliation (M=3.50, SD=1.64), and life meaning (M=3.30, SD=1.78). The non-traditional event participants rated affiliation (M=3.89, SD=1.54) and self-esteem (M=3.83, SD=1.59) as the most important motives for participation, while psychological coping (M=2.14, SD=1.18), life meaning (M=2.23, SD=1.42), recognition (M=2.40, SD=1.49), and weight concern (M=2.43, SD=1.64) were the least important. Other moderately important motives included health orientation (M=3.10, SD=1.63), personal goal achievement (M=3.19, SD=1.46), and competition (M=3.36, SD=1.43).

Subsequent post hoc analysis revealed that the motivations significantly differed by event type on several factors (see Table 4). In addition to the statistically significant results, we examined the data for practical significance. Cohen (1988) noted that a value of [[eta].sup.2]=0.01 is a small effect, a value of [[eta].sup.2]=0.06 is a moderate effect, and a value of [[eta].sup.2]=0.14 is considered a large effect. Traditional event participants were more highly motivated than non-traditional event participants on health orientation (p<.001, [[eta].sup.2]=.205), weight concern (p<.001, [[eta].sup.2]=.211), and personal goal achievement (p<.001, [[eta].sup.2]=.250), which were all large effects according to Cohen (1988). The small to moderate effects included psychological coping (p<.001, [[eta].sup.2]=.112), life-meaning (p<.001, [[eta].sup.2]=.097), and self-esteem (p<.001, [[eta].sup.2]=.054). However, non-traditional event race participants were more strongly motivated by affiliation (p<.05, [[eta].sup.2]=.013) while no significant difference between the motives of competition and recognition was found between groups.

Discussion

The motivational profiles of participants from modern MPSEs across two different event contexts were examined. As such, the MOMS was successfully adapted as a measure of MPSE motives outside a traditional marathon setting, which extends it applicability to non-traditional events. However, the results herein should be viewed cautiously since many of the scale dimension mean scores were centered around the scale midpoint. This means that while many of the motives are applicable to modern events, there may be additional (more salient) motives that might exist. That said, the results of the multi-group CFA and the generalized linear model revealed three distinct participant motivational profiles of MPSE consumers. We confirmed that regardless of event type, individuals participated in a MPSE in order to maintain or enhance their self-esteem, a motivational factor overlooked by Funk et al. (2011). The self-esteem construct is comprised of items that exhibit positive feelings about oneself such as confidence, pride, and achievement. Similarly, Ogles and Masters (2000) reported in a comparison of older and younger marathon participants that self-esteem was highly related as a motive regardless of age. Further, Masters and Ogles (1995) found event motivation was geared towards enhancing or maintaining self-esteem.

The event contexts did, however, appear to have attracted individuals with different motivational profiles. For example, the traditional event participants were a more highly motivated group compared to the non-traditional event participants. In the pursuit of traditional event training and completion, participants were motivated by health and weight concerns such as becoming physically fit, losing weight, and staying/becoming physically attractive. Further, traditional event participants were motivated by achieving personal goals (e.g., running faster, to perform better), mentally coping with daily life (e.g., distraction from stress, to get away), and deriving life meaning from their event involvement (e.g., adding purpose to life, to feel whole). Non-traditional event participants were less interested in the motivational factors associated with traditional MPSEs and placed greater emphasis on socializing with others, sharing a mutual identity/interest with other runners, and meeting new people. Thus, the non-traditional events attracted a more socially motivated participant compared to the traditional MPSE.

This finding is particularly salient in light of the work of Havenar and Lochnaum (2007), who found that marathon dropouts are more socially motivated than marathon finishers. Thus, other types of MPSEs should seek to attract socially motivated participants. However, the analysis revealed that competition against others through better times, finishing position, and general competition along with gaining recognition from others did not vary between event types. This suggests that although these non-traditional events appear to be more socially oriented, the participants were still as motivated compared to the traditional event to compete against others and gain recognition. These results are similar to research on spectator sport motivations since motives to spectate sport events also differ based on event type (James & Ross, 2004).

The researchers proposed that different MPSEs attract specific participants, which has implications for managerial practice. James and Ross (2004) argued that understanding interpersonal motives that stimulate consumer interests allow sport marketers to target certain motives. Results from the current research affirmed this point and demonstrated how event marketers could gain a heightened understanding of the MPSE consumption process, which could enable more effective future marketing initiatives. Event organizers should focus their marketing efforts toward the prospective MPSE participants specific to their event. Event characteristics such as distance, obstacles, and other unique aspects should be acknowledged as they attract participants and satisfy specific motives. Ultimately, if traditional events seek to compete with the growth of non-traditional events, organizers should consider adding unique event characteristics to differentiate their offering. Maximizing the efficiency of MPSE marketing efforts is important, beyond cost savings, since these events foster health and social outcomes (Funk et al., 2011).

Limitations and Future Research

The current study is not without limitations that necessitate acknowledgement to improve future research efforts. First, as with any model modification procedure, replication is needed to confirm the proposed factors structure. As argued by Steiger (1990), replication is an important step in ensuring the accuracy of the post hoc modifications. Thus, future work should consider using a similar MOMS factor structure to assess participant motivation to ensure validity. Given the connection of expectations-importance behaviors identified in previous research (Pritchard & Funk, 2010), continued examination and a deeper understanding of post-event attitudes and behaviors is recommended to further understand how participant pre-event psychology affects post-event consumption. This can be accomplished through interviews and focus groups with event participants, which would also assuage concerns over common method bias for the current study. Second, a pre-/post-event analysis would enhance our understanding of how the motives of MPSE participants matched their post-event satisfaction levels.

Future research could also be centered on developing an MPSE-specific scale that can be applied to both traditional and non-traditional event types. Given the relative ubiquity and mass appeal of these events, it makes this point even more germane. Additionally, since many of the mean scores in the MOMS were close to median values on the Likert-type scale, the possibility exists that a more refined version of the scale could be developed. Since the MOMS scale is somewhat dated, it may not accurately capture all the motives for event participation. Third, the data revealed high correlations between some related factors in the MOMS (i.e., health orientation, weight concern, and life meaning). Thus, future research should consider collapsing these factors into "health orientation" and "psychological" dimensions, which would improve instrument parsimony. Understanding how the MOMS factors translate into patronage intentions and actual purchasing behavior should also be considered. Lastly, non-traditional events might be more apt to attract and change an individual's physical activity level, as these events seem to cater to a unique target market that consists of a high proportion of individuals with no previous running experience. Thus, future research should continue to investigate the thriving non-traditional event market as well as acknowledge differences between event types.

References

Babbitt, B. (2012). Muddy Buddy history. Competitor Group Inc. Retrieved from http://muddybuddy.com/about/history/

Beard, J. G., & Ragheb, M. G. (1983). Measuring leisure motivation. Journal of Leisure Research, 15, 219-228.

Bell, N., & Stephenson, A. L. (2014). Variation in motivations by running ability: Using the theory of reasoned action to predict attitudes about running 5k races. Journal of Policy Research in Tourism, Leisure and Events, 6, 231-247.

Bentler, P. M., & Bonnet, D. C. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588-606.

Boomsma, A. (2000). Reporting analyses of covariance structures. Structural Equation Modeling, 7, 461-483.

Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Browne, M. W., & Cudek, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136-162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Carmack, M. A., & Martens, R. (1979). Measuring commitment to running: A survey of runners' attitude and mental states. Journal of Sport Psychology, 1, 25-42.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Coleman, R., & Ramachandani, G. (2010). The hidden benefits of non-elite mass participation sport events: An economic perspective. International Journal of Sport Marketing & Sponsorship, 12, 24-36

Curtis, J., &, McTeer, W. (1981). The motivation for running. Canadian Runner, 1, 18-19.

Crandall, R. (1980). Motivations for leisure. Journal of Leisure Research, 12, 45-54.

Crofts, C., Dickson, G., Schofield, G., & Funk, D. (2012). Post-event behavioural intentions of participants in a women-only mass participation sporting event. International Journal of Sport Management & Marketing, 12, 260-274.

Crompton, J. L. (1979). Motivations for pleasure vacation. Annals of Tourism Research, 6, 408-424.

Dann, G. M. (1977). Anomie ego-enhancement and tourism. Annals of Tourism Research, 4, 184-194.

Dann, G. M. (1981). Tourism motivations: an appraisal. Annals of Tourism Research, 8, 189-219.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The "what" and "why" of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227-268.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. (Eds.), (2002). Handbook of self-determination research. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

Desena, J., & Weinberg, A. (2012). You'll know at the finish line: A Spartan guide to the sport of obstacle racing. Pittsfield, VT: Spartan Race, Inc. Retrieved from http://info.spartanrace.com/spartan-race-ebook/

Dillman, D. A., Smyth, J. D., & Christian, L. M. (2009). Internet, mail, and mixed-mode surveys: The tailored design method (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Dimitrov, D. M. (2010). Testing for factorial invariance in the context of construct validation. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 43, 121-149.

Doppelmayr, M., & Molkenthin, A. (2004). Motivation of participants in adventure ultramarathons compared to other footraces. Biology of Sport, 24, 319-323.

Flora, D. B., & Curran, P. J. (2004). An empirical evaluation of alternative methods of estimation for confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data. Psychological Methods, 9, 466-491.

Fornell, C., & Larker D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 18, 39-50.

Funk, D., Jordan, J., Ridinger, L., & Kaplanidou, K. (2011). Capacity of mass participant sport events for the development of activity commitment and future exercise intention. Leisure Sciences, 33, 250-268.

Hair, J., Black, W., Babin, B., & Anderson, R. (2010). Multivariate data analysis (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Havenar, J., & Lochbaum, M. (2007). Differences in participation motives of first-time marathon finishers and pre-race dropouts. Journal of Sport Behavior, 30, 270-279

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological Methods, 3, 424-453.

Iso-Ahola, S. E. (1982). Toward a social psychological theory of tourism motivation: A rejoinder. Annals of Tourism Research, 9, 256-262.

James, J. D., & Ross, S. D. (2004). Comparing sport consumer motivations across multiple sports. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 13, 17-25.

Kaplanidou, K., Jordan, J. S., Funk, D., & Rindinger, L. L. (2012). Recurring sport events and destination image perceptions: Impact on active sport tourist behavioral intentions and place attachment. Journal of Sport Management, 26, 237-248.

Kaplanidou, K., & Vogt, C. (2010). The meaning and measurement of a sport event experience among active sport tourists. Journal of Sport Management, 24, 544-566.

Kaplan, D. (1990). Evaluating and modifying covariance structure models: A review and recommendation (with discussion). Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25, 137-155.

Kaplan, D. (1991). On the modification and predictive validity of covariance structure models. Quality & Quantity, 25, 307-314.

Keneally, S. (2012, November). Playing dirty. Outside Magazine. Retrieved from http://www.outsideonline.com/outdoor-adventure/multisport/ Playing-Dirty-November-2012.html

Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford.

Krouse, R., Ransedell, L., Lucas, S., & Pritchard, M. (2011). Motivation, goal orientation, coaching, and training habits of women ultrarunners. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 25, 2835-2842.

LaChausse, R. G. (2006). Motives of competitive and non-competitive cyclists. Journal of Sport Behavior, 29, 304-314.

Lonsdale, C., Hodge, K., & Rose, E. A. (2008). The behavioral regulation in sport questionnaire: Instrument development and initial validity evidence. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 30, 323-355.

Loy, J. W. (1968). The nature of sport: A definitional effort. Quest, 10, 1-15.

MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., & Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological Methods, 1, 130-149.

Maslow, A. H. (1970). Motivation and personality. New York, NY: Harper & Row.

Masters, K. S., & Lambert, M. J. (1989). The relations between cognitive coping strategies, reasons for running, injury, and performance of marathon runners. Journal of Sport Exercise Psychology, 11, 161-170.

Masters, K. S., Ogles, B. M., & Jolton, J. A. (1993). The development of an instrument to measure motivation for marathon running: The motivations of marathoners scales (MOMS). Research Quarterly in Exercise and Sport, 64, 134-143.

Masters, K. S., & Ogles, B. M. (1995). An investigation of the different motivations of marathon runners with varying degrees of experience. Journal of Sport Behavior, 18, 69.

Murphy, N. M., & Bauman, A. (2007). Mass sporting and physical activity events--Are they "bread and circuses" or public health interventions to increase population levels of physical activity? Journal of Physical Activity & Health, 4, 193-202.

Murray, E. J. (1964). Motivation and emotion. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Muthen, B.O, du Toit, S., & Spisic, D. (1997). Robust inference using weighted least squares and quadratic estimating equations in latent variable modeling with categorical and continuous outcomes. Unpublished manuscript.

Obstacle Race World. (2014). Obstacle Race World: The state of the mud run business. Retrieved from http://www.obstacleraceworld.com/go/ocrreport-a/

Ogles, B. M., & Masters, K. S. (2000). Older versus younger adult male marathon runners: Participative motives and training habits. Journal of Sport Behavior, 23, 1-14.

Ogles, B. M., & Masters, K. S. (2003). A typology of marathon runners based on cluster analysis of motivations. Journal of Sport Behavior, 26, 69-85.

Ogles, B. M., Masters, K. S., & Richardson, S. A. (1995). Obligatory running and gender: An analysis of participative motives and training habits. International Journal of Sports Psychology, 26, 233-248.

Pelletier, L. G., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., Tuson, K. M., Briere, N. M., & Blais, M. R. (1995). Toward a new measure of intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and amotivation in sports: The Sport Motivation Scale (SMS). Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 17, 35-53.

Pritchard, M. P., & Funk, D. C. (2010). The formation and effect of attitude importance in professional sport. European Journal of Marketing, 44, 1017-1036.

Ridinger, L. L., Funk, D. C., Jordan, J. S., Kaplanidou, K. (2012). Marathons for the masses: Exploring the role of negotiation efficacy and involvement on running commitment. Journal of Leisure Research, 44, 155-178.

Rundio, A., Heere, B., & Newland, B. (2014). Cause-related versus noncause-related sport events: Differentiating endurance events through a comparison of athlete's motives. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 23, 17-26.

Running USA. (2014a). 2014 state of the sport--Part III: U.S. race trends.

Retrieved from http://www.runningusa.org/2014-state-of-the-sportpart-ni-us-race-trends?returnTo=main Running USA. (2014b). 2014 state of the sport--Part I: Non-traditional running events. Retrieved from http://www.runningusa.org/state-of-sportno ntraditional?returnT o=annual-reports

Sato, M., Jordan, J. S., Kaplanidou, K., & Funk, D. C. (2014). Determinants of tourists' expenditure at mass participant sport events: A five-year analysis. Current Issues in Tourism, 17, 763-771.

Shipway, R., & Jones, I. (2007). Running away from home: Understanding visitor experiences and behaviour at sport tourism events. International Journal of Tourism Research, 9, 373-383.

Shipway, R., & Jones, I. (2008). The great suburban Everest: An 'insiders' perspective on experiences at the 2007 Flora London Marathon. Journal of Sport & Tourism, 13, 61-77.

Snelgrove, R., & Wood, L. (2010). Attracting and leveraging visitors at a charity cycling event. Journal of Sport & Tourism, 15, 269-285 Stebbins, R. A. (1992). Amateurs, professionals, and serious leisure. Buffalo, NY: McGill-Queen's University Press.

Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25, 173-180.

Richard J. Buning and Matthew Walker

Richard J. Buning, PhD, is an assistant professor in the Department of Tourism, Conventions, and Event Management at Indiana University-Purdue University Indianapolis. His research interests include sport tourism, event management, participant sport events, and sport development.

Matthew Walker, PhD, is an associate professor, sport management division chair, and associate department head in the Department of Health and Kinesiology at Texas A&M University. His research interests include corporate citizenship, environmental responsibility, social impact, and social responsibility.

Author Correspondence

Richard J. Buning

IU School of Physical Education and Tourism Management - IUPUI Department of Tourism, Conventions, and Event Management 901 West New York Street, Ste.. 250 Indianapolis, IN 46202 Email: rjbuning@iu.edu
Table 1 Model Comparisons

Models              [chi square]    df    CFI   TLI   RMSEA   PCLOSE

Traditional Group     2466.61      1394   .93   .92    .07     .000
Non-Traditional       3547.34      1394   .91   .90    .07     .000
  Group
Multi-group Model     5233.16      2871   .94   .94    .06     .000
Configural            4815.78      2572   .94   .94    .06     .000
  Invariance
Metric Invariance     4867.23      2616   .94   .94    .06     .000
Scalar Invariance     5233.16      2872   .94   .94    .06     .000

Table 2 Multi-group Confirmatory Factor Analysis (MG-CFA) Results

Factors and Items                        [lambda]   [lambda]
                                           (a)        (b)

Weight Concern
  To help me control my weight             .754       .714
  To reduce my weight                      .624       .724
  To look leaner                           .927       .949
  To stay physically attractive            .951       .925
Health Orientation
  To improve my health                     .788       .778
  To prolong my life                       .903       .883
  To become more physically fit            .857       .884
  To reduce my chance of having            .870       .872
    a heart attack
  To stay in physical condition            .824       .860
  To prevent illness                       .918       .891
Personal Goal Achievement
  To improve my running speed              .888       .684
  To try to run faster                     .802       .766
  To beat someone I've never beaten        .616       .747
    before
  To make my body perform better           .558       .664
  To feel mentally in control of my        .881       .844
    body
Competition
  To compete with others                   .489       .594
  To see how high I can place in races     .768       .825
  To get a faster time than my friends     .871       .814
  To compete with myself                   .700       .779
Recognition
  To earn respect of peers                 .784       .727
  To earn the respect of people in         .790       .764
    general
  To make my family or friends proud       .820       .862
    of me
People look up to me                       .923       .897
Brings me recognition                      .911       .908
  To get compliments from others           .886       .883
Affiliation
  To socialize with other runners          .747       .543
Have something in common with other        .947       .891
  people
  To meet people                           .798       .690
  To share a group identity with           .867       .659
    other runners
Psychological Coping
  To become less anxious                   .770       .750
  To become less depressed                 .832       .787
  To distract myself from daily            .794       .801
    worries
  To improve my mood                       .866       .783
  To have time alone to sort things        .919       .866
    out
  To concentrate on my thoughts            .912       .926
  To solve problems                        .823       .842
  To blow off steam                        .816       .746
  To get away from it all                  .937       .727
  To have time alone with the world        .954       .874
Life-Meaning
  To add a sense of meaning to life        .828       .833
  To make my life more purposeful          .874       .897
  To make myself feel whole                .906       .915
  To make my life more complete            .899       .890
  To feel a sense of belonging in          .872       .834
    nature
  To feel at peace with the world          .883       .891
  To improve my sense of self-worth        .960       .865
Self-Esteem
  To improve my self-esteem                .893       .785
  To feel more confident about myself      .859       .819
It is a positive emotional experience      .700       .767
  To feel proud of myself                  .775       .769
  To feel a sense of achievement           .676       .728
  To feel mentally in control of my        .798       .861
    body
  To feel like a winner                    .827       .801

Factors and Items                        AVE     CR    AVE     CR
                                         (a)    (a)    (b)    (b)

Weight Concern                           .680   .892   .698   .901
  To help me control my weight
  To reduce my weight
  To look leaner
  To stay physically attractive
Health Orientation                       .742   .945   .743   .945
  To improve my health
  To prolong my life
  To become more physically fit
  To reduce my chance of having
    a heart attack
  To stay in physical condition
  To prevent illness
Personal Goal Achievement                .580   .870   .553   .860
  To improve my running speed
  To try to run faster
  To beat someone I've never beaten
    before
  To make my body perform better
  To feel mentally in control of my
    body
Competition                              .519   .806   .576   .842
  To compete with others
  To see how high I can place in races
  To get a faster time than my friends
  To compete with myself
Recognition                              .730   .942   .711   .936
  To earn respect of peers
  To earn the respect of people in
    general
  To make my family or friends proud
    of me
People look up to me
Brings me recognition
  To get compliments from others
Affiliation                              .711   .907   .500   .795
  To socialize with other runners
Have something in common with other
  people
  To meet people
  To share a group identity with
    other runners
Psychological Coping                     .747   .967   .660   .951
  To become less anxious
  To become less depressed
  To distract myself from daily
    worries
  To improve my mood
  To have time alone to sort things
    out
  To concentrate on my thoughts
  To solve problems
  To blow off steam
  To get away from it all
  To have time alone with the world
Life-Meaning                             .791   .964   .766   .958
  To add a sense of meaning to life
  To make my life more purposeful
  To make myself feel whole
  To make my life more complete
  To feel a sense of belonging in
    nature
  To feel at peace with the world
  To improve my sense of self-worth
Self-Esteem                              .629   .922   .626   .921
  To improve my self-esteem
  To feel more confident about myself
It is a positive emotional experience
  To feel proud of myself
  To feel a sense of achievement
  To feel mentally in control of my
    body
  To feel like a winner

Note. Standardized estimates are presented.

(a) Traditional group. (b) Non-Traditional group.

Table 3 Mean Scores, Standard Deviations, and Correlations

Construct                       M      SD       1         2

1. Weight Concern              3.03   1.90    1.00      .616
2. Health Orientation          3.67   1.81   .785 **    1.00
3. Personal Goal Achievement   3.76   1.63   .586 **   .742 **
4. Competition                 3.45   1.40   .281 **   .457 **
5. Recognition                 2.53   1.52   .414 **   .475 **
6. Affiliation                 3.76   1.58   .150 **   .331 *
7. Psychological Coping        2.46   1.41   .546 **   .653 **
8. Life Meaning                2.58   1.63   .513 **   .610 **
9. Self-Esteem                 4.09   1.59   .530 **   .662 **

Construct                         3         4         5         6

1. Weight Concern               .343      .079      .171      .023
2. Health Orientation           .551      .209      .226      .110
3. Personal Goal Achievement    1.00      .426      .224      .081
4. Competition                 .653 **    1.00      .193      .144
5. Recognition                 .473 **   .439 **    1.00      .199
6. Affiliation                 .284 **   .379 **   .446 **    1.00
7. Psychological Coping        .568 **   .331 **   .549 **   .404 **
8. Life Meaning                .498 **   .300 **   .649 **   .408 **
9. Self-Esteem                 .636 **   .452 **   .693 **   .463 **

Construct                         7         8       9

1. Weight Concern               .298      .263     .281
2. Health Orientation           .426      .371     .438
3. Personal Goal Achievement    .323      .248     .404
4. Competition                  .109       .09     .204
5. Recognition                  .301      .421     .480
6. Affiliation                  .163      .167     .214
7. Psychological Coping         1.00      .591     .464
8. Life Meaning                .769 **    1.00     .506
9. Self-Esteem                 .681 **   .711 **   1.00

Note. ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
N=408. Above 1.00 are the squared correlations for each construct.

Table 4 Multiple Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) for Event Type Effects

                            Traditional   Non-Traditional
                            (n=133)       (n=275)

Motives                      M      SD     M      SD    df

Weight Concern              4.29   1.80   2.43   1.64   1
Health Orientation          4.85   1.59   3.10   1.63   1
Personal Goal Achievement   4.93   1.31   3.19   1.46   1
Competition                 3.64   1.32   3.36   1.43   1
Recognition                 2.80   1.54   2.40   1.49   1
Affiliation                 3.50   1.64   3.89   1.54   1
Psychological Coping        3.14   1.61   2.14   1.18   1
Life Meaning                3.30   1.78   2.23   1.42   1
Self-Esteem                 4.62   1.46   3.83   1.59   1

Motives                       F         P       [[eta].sup.2]

Weight Concern              310.95   .000 ***       .211
Health Orientation          274.84   .000 ***       .205
Personal Goal Achievement   269.23   .000 ***       .250
Competition                  7.11      .057         .009
Recognition                 14.93      .11          .016
Affiliation                 13.38     .020 *        .013
Psychological Coping        91.09    .000 ***       .112
Life Meaning                104.08   .000 ***       .097
Self-Esteem                 55.87    .000 ***       .054

Note. Pillai's Trace = .436, F (9, 398) = 34.231, p<.001. All items
measured from 1 (not a reason) to 7 (a most important reason).
* p<.05; * p<.01; *** p<.001.


联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有