An examination of constraints and motivators as predictors of sport media consumption substitution intention.
Larkin, Ben ; Fink, Janet S. ; Trail, Galen T. 等
Introduction
In 2006, researchers Pritchard and Funk explained that there has
been a movement toward escalating consumption of sport through media.
Although sport teams still care deeply about gate receipts and their
impact on the bottom line (Gelb, 2013), the shift to media consumption
has, in fact, denoted a trend indicating attendance is becoming a less
critical aspect in an organization's profitability (Pritchard &
Funk, 2006). Fans' preference to watch games/events on television
was among the preeminent concerns facing sport industry practitioners as
of 2012 (Luker, 2012). This concern manifested itself in the National
Football League (NFL) in January 2014 when three of four first-round
playoff games were in danger of being blacked out on local television
due to sluggish ticket sales (Schwab, 2014). As Schwab (2014) explained,
this may simply be the "most stark example that NFL fans
aren't too excited to go to games anymore" (para. 5). This
topic has been covered extensively in general publications (e.g.,
Rovell, 2012); however, it has received scant attention in the academic
literature.
Pritchard and Funk (2006) looked at the relationship between
attending sporting events and consuming through media, finding that for
some this was more of a symbiotic relationship, while others actually
substitute media consumption for the more traditional attendance
behavior. Additionally, focus has begun to shift from motives (e.g.,
Funk, Mahony, & Ridinger, 2002; Trail & James, 2001; Wann, 1995)
to constraints that impede attendance to sport events (e.g., Kim &
Trail, 2010; Trail & Kim, 2011; Trail, Robinson, & Kim, 2008).
To this point, however, the impact of constraints on attendance
substitution through media has not been studied. Further, research has
yet to examine motivators for attendance substitution with sport media
consumption (hereafter SMC). In fact, although Pritchard and Funk (2006)
implied that constraints to attendance may serve to prompt consumption
through media, it is unclear whether the shift toward SMC is more a
result of the positive reinforcements of SMC substitution (motivators)
or constraining variables that serve as aversive stimuli for attending
live events, thus prompting substitution with SMC.
In this paper, the use of the term SMC will refer to the
consumption of sport events at home through media. By home, we are
referring to an individual's permanent and/or temporary residence.
For most fans, this would mean their permanent residence. However, other
fans may be residing in a temporary residence that for significant
periods of time is their home. For example, a college student's
apartment or dormitory may only be a temporary residence, but still
represent his or her home during the academic year. Similarly, an
individual traveling on business may spend several weeks in a hotel
room. While this is certainly not his or her permanent residence, it may
represent his or her home while away on business. Given that the
possession of a cable or satellite subscription enables individuals to
consume through multiple devices, this consumption may include not just
television, but also computers, smart phones, and tablets. Therefore,
sport media consumption could occur from virtually anywhere at any given
time. For example, individuals could be watching at a bar, enjoying the
social benefits of watching alongside other fans, and also having the
ability to follow their fantasy team as well as the conversation on
social media on a mobile device. It would seem the variables considered
in this study would be similarly applicable in this context; however, it
stands to reason that the impact of certain constraints (e.g., location
and cost) and motivators (e.g., ease and comfort) would be mitigated in
this case compared to the home setting.
Another element to consider is an individual's level of team
identification. Long believed to be the most significant factor in a
sport fan's consumption motivation (Wann, Melnick, Russell, &
Pease, 2001), team identification has been found to strengthen motives
to attend events (Kim, Trail, & Magnusen, 2013). However,
researchers have not explored how team identification impacts
constraints to attendance, or motivators to substitute attendance with
SMC. As such, this study applied Skinner's (1957, 1982) theoretical
work on natural consequences as a framework, in an effort to determine
the effect of both constraints and motivators on SMC substitution
intention and whether this was more a result of the pleasant stimuli
(motivators) stemming from SMC or the aversive stimuli for attendance
(constraints) that may prompt substitution. Finally, we sought to assess
how these relationships were affected by team identification.
Literature Review
Natural Consequences
In his reinforcement theory of motivation, Skinner (1953) proposed
that behavior is a function of consequences, both negative and positive.
Behavior met with positive consequences has the tendency to be repeated,
and in contrast, behavior met with negative consequences does not.
Skinner's (1953) theory rests on the notion that looking inside a
given organism for an explanation of behavior has the propensity to
cloud variables outside the organism that are accessible for analysis.
As Skinner (1953) suggested, these variables not only lie outside the
organism, but in its immediate environment and environmental history,
and make it possible to explain behavior. In this sense, the behaviorist
approach is viewed as more of an intervention than as an observation.
Illustrating this technique is the famous example where a pigeon raises
his head above a certain level and is rewarded with food in order to
reinforce the behavior (e.g., Skinner, 1953). As Skinner noted, however,
"the only defining characteristic of a reinforcing stimulus is that
it reinforces" (1953, p. 72). This brings about the concept of
intrinsic consequences, which originate in the behavior itself (Skinner,
1957, 1982). These are often referred to as "natural
consequences" (e.g., Skinner, 1982, p. 4). A behavior is reinforced
when the consequences produced act to strengthen the behavior. A
negative reinforcer is the removal of an unpleasant stimulus, whereas a
positive reinforcer is the addition of a pleasant stimulus (Catania,
1998).
This theory is similar in nature to Thorndike's (1927) law of
effect. In this work, Thorndike stated that "when
'annoyingness' is attached to a frequent connection and
'satisfyingness' to a rare connection, the latter gains and
the former loses until the latter becomes the habitual response"
(Thorndike, 1927, p. 212). In effect, the behavior that naturally
produces a satisfying rather than dissatisfying feeling becomes
habitually repeated. Comunidad Los Horcones (1992) explained that
natural reinforcement has suffered from a lack of experimental work and
the concept has remained quite vague since its inception; however, the
more commonly studied law of effect has remained relevant and has been
used as a framework for the study of human choice behavior between two
or more alternatives up to the present day (e.g., Navakatikyan, Murrell,
Bensemann, Davison, & Elliffe, 2013).
Within sport, constraints to attendance at an event can be aversive
stimuli for some individuals. For example, through prior experiences, an
individual learns that travel to the venue and parking at the venue is a
terrible experience (i.e., stuck in traffic for hours, parking is
non-existent and/or extremely expensive). These experiences become
aversive stimuli that prevent future consumption of the product (the
game) at the venue. However, the individual still wants to consume the
product, so they must look for another avenue of consumption; in this
case, staying at home and watching the game through media may make for a
more attractive option. Thus, the individual substitutes media
consumption of the product from home for attending the event at the
venue, thereby avoiding the aversive stimuli. In addition, pleasant
stimuli relative to watching at home may also exist, motivating the
individual to stay home. For example, the comfort and safety of the
individual's home may motivate them to stay at home and watch the
game rather than attend. Thus, it is critical that both the aversive
stimuli to attending and the pleasant stimuli from watching at home be
discussed in detail to develop specific hypotheses regarding the
variables under consideration in this study.
Aversive Stimuli: Constraints to Attendance
In relation to spectator sport, constraints to attendance are
understood to represent "factors that impede or inhibit an
individual from attending a sporting event" (Kim & Trail, 2010,
p. 191). Past researchers have explained that while they do not
necessarily act to block participation, constraints do serve to prompt
substitution (Pritchard & Funk, 2006). Constraints, then, can be
thought of as aversive stimuli to attendance and their presence could
prompt substitution by SMC.
Constraints have long been examined in the leisure domain (e.g.,
Crawford & Godbey, 1987; Crawford, Jackson, & Godbey, 1991).
Only recently, however, have they gained traction in a sport consumer
context (e.g., Kim & Trail, 2010; Trail & Kim, 2011; Trail et
al., 2008). Crawford and Godbey (1987) grouped leisure constraints into
three distinct categories: intrapersonal, interpersonal, and structural.
These categories cover the psychological inner states and the
interpersonal relationships impacting leisure preferences, as well as
the factors that intervene between preferences and participation.
Believing that some of the sport spectator constraints could be
considered both intrapersonal and interpersonal, Kim and Trail (2010)
modified the previously held conception of constraints, grouping the
factors into just two categories: internal and external. Internal
constraints are the psychological cognitions that curtail behavior (Kim
& Trail, 2010). These may include the perception that there is no
one with whom to attend, and the perception that significant others have
no interest in the sport or event. In contrast, external constraints are
the social or environmental aspects that deter or even prevent a
behavior (Kim & Trail, 2010; Trail & Kim, 2011), including cost,
time, location, and parking. Kim and Trail (2010) examined the
relationship between motivators and constraints and their impact on
attendance behavior. They found Lack of Success and Leisure Alternatives
to be the only significant constraints in the prediction of attendance.
One possible leisure alternative an individual could substitute for
attending an event is the option of sport media consumption (Trail et
al., 2008).
Despite the notion that constraints will prompt substitution
(Pritchard & Funk, 2006), we posit that not all constraints will
necessarily serve to prompt attendance substitution with SMC. For
example, an individual lacking knowledge about a particular sport is
unlikely to substitute SMC for attendance, but rather, would be more apt
to forego sport consumption altogether. Similarly, leisure alternatives
are not likely to trigger SMC over attendance. More likely, the option
of playing a recreational sport or attending a concert would serve as a
constraint not just for attendance, but for SMC as well. Additionally,
commitments to friends and family would constrain both attendance and
SMC. Considering such logic, it appears six constraint factors from
Trail and Kim's (2011) work could prompt substitution with SMC.
These include No Interest from Significant Others, No One to Attend
with, Location, Lack of Success, Parking, and Cost. In addition, as
Trail et al. (2008) suggested, weather may make for a viable
consideration.
H1: There will be a significant positive relationship between each
of the seven perceived constraints (a) Location, (b) Parking, (c) Cost,
(d) No One to Attend with, (e) Weather, (f) Lack of Success, and (g) No
Interest from Significant Others and SMC Substitution Intention.
Pleasant Stimuli: Motives for Sport Media Consumption Substitution
Despite the efficacy of constraints in explaining attendance
behavior (Kim & Trail, 2010; Trail & Kim, 2011), motives are
also a central predictor in sport consumption decisions (Trail, Fink,
& Anderson, 2003). They are understood to represent the
"energizing force that activates behaviors" (Hawkins, Best,
& Coney, 2004 p. 354), and are thus reflective of naturally
occurring positive reinforcement, as their presence increases the
likelihood of desired behaviors. Therefore, it is important to consider
the possible motivating variables that would positively reinforce SMC
substitution to better understand why individuals stay home to consume
sport events.
To this point, we are not aware of any existing instrument for the
measurement of SMC substitution motivation; however, there have been
many past efforts in scale development for motivation of general sport
consumption (e.g., Funk et al., 2002; Trail & James, 2001; Wann,
1995). These factors serve as motivating variables for attending events,
and could also foster SMC. However, choosing SMC rather than attending
in person is triggered by a unique set of motivating variables beyond
those historically understood to influence sport consumers. An
explanation for this concept lies in the uses and gratifications
paradigm (e.g., Katz, Blumler, & Gurevitch, 1973). As was explained
by Katz et al., media with different attributes are more likely to
satisfy different needs. Conversely, those needs that are
"psychologically related or conceptually similar" (Katz et
al., 1973, p. 515) will be just as well-fulfilled by options with
similar attributes. Indeed a live event and a broadcast event share some
of the same attributes. However, while needs such as achievement and
escape would be fulfilled by both options, the distinct attributes of
SMC create a different set of motives for this consumption mode. Funk et
al. (2002) provided further support for this notion in explaining that
different consumer motives may trigger different sport activities for
various consumer segments. On a related note, Pritchard and Funk (2006)
highlighted the fact that a great deal of previous research has focused
on the factors driving event attendance, while largely ignoring the
usage of media. If these arguments are to be accepted, that is, motives
for different sport consumer activities and segments differ, and that
literature on SMC remains sparse, then it is only logical to explore
this burgeoning consumption mode and search for differences in the
motives underlying this distinct consumptive activity.
While no scale has been developed specifically for the motivations
underlying SMC, a number of researchers have explored areas relevant to
this phenomenon. These areas include fantasy sport (Drayer, Shapiro,
Dwyer, Morse, & White, 2010; Dwyer & Drayer, 2010),
high-definition television (Dupagne, 1999; Kim & Lee, 2003), and the
experience of watching sport events at a local cinema (Fairley &
Tyler, 2012). Insights gleaned from this literature suggest that SMC
serves as something of a support mechanism for fantasy sport users due
to the variety of media sources available while watching a sport event
(Drayer et al., 2010; Dwyer & Drayer, 2010). Furthermore,
technological attributes such as picture clarity, screen size, and sound
have been shown to increase the draw to high-definition television
consumption (Kim & Lee, 2003), more specifically for sport
viewership (Dupagne, 1999). Fairley and Tyler (2012) corroborated this
notion in a qualitative study of sport fans consuming sport events at a
local cinema. However, in addition to the notion that consumers
appreciate the high degree of technological control (e.g., digital video
recording and time shifted playback abilities), Fairley and Tyler also
found that the draw of consuming at a local cinema was bolstered by
factors such as safety, comfort, broadcast enhancement, and the ease of
this consumption mode compared to attending in person. These factors
(technological attributes, and the comfort, safety, and ease of watching
at home) were in line with stimuli identified in past research (e.g.,
Gantz & Wenner, 1995; Trail et al., 2003b; Weed, 2010), and it seems
they would be equally as significant, or perhaps even more so, for
consumers deciding to substitute SMC for attendance.
Indeed, it is apparent that many elements that enhance the modern
day sport broadcast, in conjunction with factors such as fantasy sport,
technological attributes, and the comfort, safety, and ease of watching
at home, positively reinforce SMC. Moreover, much like motives for
traditional sport consumption (Trail et al., 2003a), it is evident that
these factors are all related to fundamental psychological and social
needs (e.g., subsistence, protection, and leisure). This, perhaps,
underlies the fact that motives have traditionally been very highly
correlated in past spectator motivation research (e.g., Trail &
James, 2001; Wann, 1995), which led past work to conceptualize a single
second order latent variable comprising all motives (e.g., Trail,
Anderson, & Fink, 2000, Trail et al., 2003). This is in contrast to
constraints, which have traditionally been studied individually in sport
spectator research (e.g., Kim & Trail, 2010). Although Kim and Trail
(2010) did examine motives individually, the SMC motives in this study
have never before been studied. We expect that--due to their strong
conceptual relationship--these factors will be very highly correlated
and load highly on a single latent motivator variable. Accordingly, a
second order latent variable will be used in the current study that
comprises the SMC motivators identified in this literature review (i.e.,
Fantasy Sport, Technological Attributes, Comfort at Home, Safety at
Home, Ease, and Enhancement).
H2: There will be a significant positive relationship between
Motivators to stay home (pleasant stimuli) and SMC Substitution
Intention.
In review, based on Skinner's (1957, 1982) natural
consequences framework, the context of SMC can be viewed as a learned
behavior, reinforced through a series of natural consequences. With
individuals drawing on past experiences, SMC may be positively
reinforced or motivated by, for example, its suitability for fantasy
sport participation. Thus, SMC may be repeated because consumers find
the consequences "pleasing or satisfying" (Skinner, 1953, p.
81). SMC may also increase if constraints to attending live events are
perceived as unpleasant stimuli (e.g., parking, cost, etc.) and staying
home is viewed as a viable substitution. In short, an application of
Skinner's (1957, 1982) natural consequences framework suggests that
an individual's consumption experience will be reinforced through
the naturally occurring perception of constraints and motivators, and
thus may increase future SMC.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Overcoming Reinforcement: Team Identification
To this point, our discussion of constraints to attendance and
motivators for SMC has not taken into account team identification, which
has been shown to drastically impact sport fan behavior. Team
identification refers to the degree of a fan's psychological
connection to a team (Wann et al., 2001). Wann et al. (2001) suggested
that more highly identified fans exhibit different behavioral responses,
such as a higher propensity to attend games, even going so far as to
suggest that identification with a team and/or athlete is the most
significant psychological factor influencing attendance. More recently,
Kim and Trail (2010) found team identification to be significant in
predicting attendance, as it explained 21% of the variance. Further,
they suggested that lack of team success was a constraint that could be
overcome by high levels of team identification. Since Kim et al. (2013)
found team identification to positively moderate the relationship
between motives and attendance intention, it follows that team
identification may moderate the effect of constraints and motivators on
SMC substitution intention. Highly identified fans should be more apt to
overcome constraints to attendance and find the motivators for SMC less
appealing than attending. See Figure 1 for an illustration of the
proposed relationships.
H3: The relationship between (a) Location, (b) Parking, (c) Cost,
(d) No One to Attend with, (e) Weather, (f) Lack of Success, and (g) No
Interest from Significant Others and SMC Substitution Intention will be
significantly reduced for those high on Team Identification.
H4: The relationship between Motivators and SMC Substitution
Intention will be significantly reduced for those high on Team
Identification.
Methods
The study consisted of two phases. Both phases received IRB
approval. First, the measurement model was tested on a sample of
undergraduate students at a large Northeastern university. Second, the
measurement and structural models were assessed through a nationwide
sample of NFL fans and spectators.
Pilot Study
The goal of the pilot study was to assess the measurement model. A
sample of 201 undergraduate students was recruited to assess the
internal consistency of the factors and the fit of the measurement
model. (1) The questionnaire consisted of constraints, motivators, team
identification, and behavioral measures. Items were measured using a
7-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).
Constraints consisted of seven subscales adapted from previous studies
(e.g., Trail & Kim, 2011) with 21 items. These seven subscales
displayed strong psychometric properties, with alpha coefficients
ranging from .69 to .93 and average variance explained (AVE) values
ranging from .49 to .82 in recent research (e.g., Kim & Trail, 2010;
Trail & Kim, 2011). Weather, the final constraint, was suggested by
Trail et al. (2008) and was developed for use in this study.
Given that, to our knowledge, there is no instrument in the extant
research to measure SMC motivators, these were created for use in this
study based on insights gleaned from past sport consumption research
(e.g., Drayer et al., 2010; Dupagne, 1999; Dwyer & Drayer, 2010;
Fairley & Tyler, 2012; Gantz & Wenner, 1995; Trail et al.,
2003b; Weed, 2010), as discussed in our literature review. Prior to data
collection, two sport management faculty members with expertise in
consumer behavior reviewed these factors for content validity.
Modifications were made to some items to reduce ambiguity and improve
the clarity of the items. As discussed in our literature review, the
motivation dimensions included Fantasy Sport, Technological Attributes,
Comfort, Safety, Ease, and Enhancement. Team identification was adapted
from the Points of Attachment Index (PAI; Robinson, Trail, & Kwon,
2004). This factor, too, has shown strong psychometric properties,
recently exhibiting an alpha coefficient of .96 and construct
reliability (AVE) of .88 (Kim et al., 2013). The behavioral measures
were adapted from Trail and James (2012). They included single items
such as "I am likely to attend future games," and open-ended
questions depicting the number of games attended and viewed on
television for past and current seasons, as well as intent for future
seasons. Finally, the dependent variable representing SMC Substitution
Intention included a single item that read, "I am likely to watch
future games at home rather than attend." We felt it was
particularly important to word this item in such a way as to establish
substitution intention. Simply indicating one is likely to watch future
games at home does not necessarily capture substitution intentions;
however, we believe that indications that one is likely to watch future
games at home rather than attend does measure substitution intent.
Results and Discussion
After ensuring that the data was normally distributed, the
measurement model was assessed using Mplus 6.0 and showed evidence of
reasonable model fit, with [RMSEA = .066, CFI = .898, SRMR = .057, and
[chi square]/df ratio = (1375.055/728=1.89)]. However, two factors
(i.e., No Interest from Significant Others and No One to Attend with)
were very highly correlated. Further, No Interest from Significant
Others had poor internal consistency. We believe the poor internal
consistency was a result of the construct covering too wide of a domain.
Specifically, it covers no interest from family, spouse, and friends. It
is plausible that an individual could be deterred from attending an
event because his/her family is not interested in attending, but not
because of lack of interest from his/her friends. Furthermore, with
regard to the high correlation between No Interest from Significant
Others and No One to Attend with, we feel that the notion of one's
significant others not having interest in attending is conceptually
similar and is perhaps just a more specific manifestation of having no
one with whom to attend. As such, No Interest from Significant Others
was dropped from the analysis. In addition, although Location held
together reasonably well, one item reading "the accessibility of
the sport venue is difficult" was deemed conceptually different
from the other two items, which spoke to the poor neighborhood
surrounding the stadium/arena. Accordingly, Location was run as a
two-item factor and the model was reassessed. The goodness of fit
indices for the CFA on the measurement model indicated good model fit,
with [RMSEA = .055, CFI = .938, SRMR = .049, and [chi square]/df ratio =
(941.120/587=1.60)]. Moreover, the factors had strong psychometric
properties with AVE values ranging from .56 to .88, indicating good
construct reliability, and alpha coefficients ranging from .746 to .955,
indicating good internal consistency. In short, the preliminary
assessment of the measurement model indicated it was appropriate to
proceed and test both the measurement and structural model on a more
representative sample of sport consumers.
Main Study
The goal of the main study was to again test the measurement model
as well as the structural model and hypotheses. Participants were
solicited on sport blogs, message boards, and social media groups. Past
research has found collegiate sport message board subscribers to attend
more games than non-subscribers (Clavio, 2008). Furthermore, it was
suggested they were bigger fans than their non-subscribing counterparts,
who were characterized more as casual fans. We felt these
characteristics would hold for those who were active on both general and
NFL team specific sport blogs and message boards. At the very least, we
felt this sample would have a history of attendance and SMC experiences
on which to draw when assessing the items. A short invitation
accompanied by a link to a duplicate of the survey used in the pilot
study was posted on message boards as well as on social media pages and
the comment section of blogs on popular sport fan sites (e.g., ESPN,
Yahoo! Sports, SBNation, Facebook, Twitter, etc.). The short invitation
generally read "Hello sport fans, I am conducting some research on
sport viewing for my academic work. If you wouldn't mind doing me a
huge favor by sparing a few minutes to complete this short survey,
it'd be a tremendous help. Thanks so much," but was modified
slightly to accommodate social media platforms with character limits
(e.g., Twitter). This was posted in approximately 80-100 locations,
including general sport blogs and social media groups, as well as
team-specific blogs and groups. Data collection was carried out during
the NFL season, between the months of November and December of 2013.
Informed consent was built in to the beginning of the survey, such that
participants had to read the consent form and indicate agreement to
participate prior to beginning the survey.
A total of 244 participants were obtained for this phase of the
investigation, which is sufficient for structural equation modeling
(Kline, 2005). The survey was set to operate with forced completion.
Therefore, all completed surveys contained no missing data; however, a
total of 78 individuals began, but failed to complete, the survey. These
78 surveys were discarded, leaving us with a completion rate of 76%. The
survey asked participants to assess the items in relation to their local
NFL team, which may or may not be the same as their favorite team. Given
that past research has characterized sport message board subscribers as
big sport fans (Clavio, 2008), we felt this step was necessary to ensure
that we got a range of team identification levels sufficient for a
multigroup moderation analysis. Had participants assessed items in
relation to their favorite NFL team, we felt all team identification
responses would have been high. The NFL was chosen as the context for
the study because we believed constraints such as cost and weather, as
well as motivators such as fantasy sport, were most impactful in this
context. The NFL is notorious for high price points, and given the time
of year the sport is played and the location of some outdoor stadiums,
it can also come with potentially harsh weather conditions (Schwab,
2014). Moreover, according to the Fantasy Sport Trade Association,
fantasy football is the world's preferred fantasy sport (Fantasy
Sport Trade Association, 2014). All NFL markets were represented at
least once with the exception of Tennessee, Indianapolis, and Denver.
The median for number of times represented was four.
Results
Sample Characteristics
The sample was 77% male with an average age of 31.26 years old. A
total of 16.9% of participants had a household income below $19,999 in
2012, while 22.7% made between $20,000 and $49,999, 23.1% made between
$50,000 and $79,999, 13.8% made between $80,000 and $109,999, and 9.3%
made between $110,000 and $139,999. Just 14.2% of participants made
$140,000 or more in 2012. Only 15.5% of participants indicated they had
not attended a game in either the previous or current NFL season.
Measurement Model
The means and standard deviations of the variables in this study
can be found in Table 1. The study employed the two-step modeling
approach, using maximum likelihood estimation to first assess the fit of
the measurement model before proceeding to assess the structural model.
Subsequently, a multi-group CFA was conducted before assessing whether
the hypothesized paths differed across groups, as was hypothesized in H3
and H4. Prior to the assessment of the measurement model, normality of
distribution was assessed through an examination of the skewness and
kurtosis of the data. All values were within the [+ or -]2.58 range for
skewness and [+ or -]2.56 range for kurtosis recommended by Hair, Black,
Babin, and Anderson (2009) and thus could be considered normally
distributed.
The measurement model was assessed initially and exhibited good
model fit, with [RMSEA = .057, CFI = .948, and [chi square]/df ratio =
(1058.456/586=1.81)]. Widely accepted guidelines state that the [chi
square]/df ratio should fall between two and three (Bollen, 1989), while
a CFI of at least .90 and a RMSEA between .06 and .08 are considered
adequate (Hu & Bentler, 1999). Therefore, the measurement model fit
quite well. Factor loadings of the subscales ranged from .639 to .984,
while AVE's ranged from .69 to .91, indicating strong construct
reliability (see Table 2 for a list of factor loadings, AVE, and
Cronbach's alphas). Discriminant validity was established for all
first order factors except for the motive of Ease. The AVE of the Ease
subscale failed to exceed the squared correlations between Ease and
Comfort and between Ease and Technological Attributes, thus failing the
discriminant validity test as per Fornell and Larcker (1981) (see Table
3 for the correlations between latent variables). However, given that we
conceptualized the motivators to load on a single second order latent
variable, the lack of discriminant validity between these individual
motivator variables did not pose a problem in assessing the structural
model. Despite the lack of discriminant validity, all items were
retained and motivators were run as a second order variable. In brief,
the assessment of the measurement model indicated it was appropriate to
assess the structural model and evaluate hypotheses.
Structural Model
Accordingly, the first order latent variables representing
motivators were loaded onto a second order Motivator latent variable,
while the first order variables representing constraints were left to
run individually in testing the structural model. Per widely accepted
SEM guidelines (e.g., Bollen, 1989; Hu & Bentler, 1999), the
structural model exhibited adequate model fit [RMSEA = .063 CFI = .932,
[chi square]/df ratio = (1112.744/561 = 1.98)]. Furthermore, the factor
loadings of the first order latent motives on the Motivator factor
ranged from .639 to .983.
Evaluating Hypotheses 1 & 2
In H1 we proposed that all constraints would have a significant
positive relationship with SMC Substitution Intention. There was no
significant relationship between Location ([beta] = -.070, p = .485),
Parking ([beta] = .021, p = .828), No One to Attend with ([beta] =
-.057, p = .429), Weather ([beta] = .028, p = .720), and Lack of Success
([beta] = -.059, p = .416) and SMC Substitution Intention. Therefore,
H1a, b, d, e, and f were not supported. The relationship between Cost
and SMC Substitution Intention ([beta] = .161, p = .023) was positive
and significant, with 2.6% variance explained. Thus, H1c was supported.
In H2 we proposed that Motivators would have a significant positive
relationship with SMC Substitution Intention. There was a significant
positive relationship between Motivators and SMC Substitution Intention
([beta] = .240, p = .003), with 5.8% variance explained. Thus, H2 was
supported.
Evaluating Hypotheses 3 & 4
Finally, in H3 and H4 we proposed that Team Identification would
moderate the relationship between both the constraints and Motivators
and SMC Substitution Intention, such that the effect would be reduced
for those with higher levels of Team Identification. To test this
moderating effect, a multi-group SEM was conducted where Team
Identification levels were split into two groups to ensure significant
differences in identification levels; participants who rated their Team
Identification at the mid-point and below were placed in the low group
(N = 98; M = 2.15) and participants who assessed Team Identification
above the mid-point were placed in the high group (N = 146; M = 6.35).
Results from a t-test indicated that the means of these two groups were
significantly different (t = -34.063, p < .001).
The multi-group CFA fit reasonably well [RMSEA = .076, CFI = .905,
and [chi square]/df ratio = (1889.690/1101 = 1.72)], indicating the
measurement model did not differ significantly across Team
Identification groups. Furthermore, the multi-group structural model
showed that constraining the paths to be equal across groups did not
make the model fit significantly worse [RMSEA = .076 CFI = .903, and
[chi square]/df ratio = (1970.374/1157 = 1.70)], indicating there is no
moderating effect of Team Identification on the paths between the
independent variables and SMC Substitution Intention. The effects of
Location,
Parking, No One to Attend with, Weather, and Lack of Success on SMC
Substitution Intention for both low and high Team Identification were
not significant and were not different across groups, indicating no
moderating effect of Team Identification on the relationship between
these factors and SMC Substitution Intention. Therefore, H3a, b, d, e,
and f were not supported. The relationship between Cost and SMC
Substitution Intention for low Team identification was not significant
([beta] = -.016, p = .904); however, the relationship between Cost and
SMC Substitution Intention for high Team Identification was significant
([beta] = .235, p = .005). Although one path coefficient was significant
and the other was not, the two paths exhibited overlapping confidence
intervals, indicating that they were not significantly different from
each other. Therefore, H3c was not supported.
The paths between Motivators (positive reinforcement) and SMC
Substitution Intention for both low ([beta] = .290, p = .037) and high
([beta] = .227, p = .023) Team Identification were positive and
significant, but not significantly different across groups. Therefore,
H4 was not supported (see Table 4 for a list of all tested
relationships).
Post-hoc Tests
Following our hypothesis testing, we conducted follow-up analyses
on the behavioral items collected as part of the study. On average,
participants indicated they watched 7.76 games on television during the
previous season, 11.39 games on television during the current season,
and intended to watch 10.42 games on television during the following
season. The overall mean score for substitution intention was 6.04, but
was higher for the high team identification group (M = 6.21) than the
low team identification group (M = 5.79). Per the results of a one-way
ANOVA, we found this difference in substitution intention to be
significant (F = 4.79, p = .03). Even when controlling for the
importance of the cost constraint, participants in the high
identification group maintained significantly higher substitution
intentions (F = 4.99, p = .026). However, when controlling for income,
the difference between team identification groups was not significant (F
= 2.01, p = .158). We also collected information on participants'
attendance behavior and intentions. The overall mean score for
attendance intention was 4.63; however, this was significantly higher (F
= 72.394, p < .001) for those in the high identification group (M =
5.47) than the low identification group (M = 3.38). As for actual
attendance behavior, participants indicated they had attended an average
of just under one game in the previous NFL season (M = 0.91), and more
than a game on average during the current season (M = 1.42).
Participants also intended to attend more than a game per season on
average in the following season (M = 1.16). There were, however, no
significant differences in actual attendance behavior between
identification groups for the previous NFL season (F = 2.23, p = .137)
or the current NFL season (F = 1.32, p = .253), but consistent with
responses on the attendance intention scale, highly identified
participants indicated they intended to attend a significantly higher
number of games in the following season (M = 1.37, F = 3.992, p = .047)
than participants in the low identification group (M = 0.87).
General Discussion
The current research provides initial evidence of the effect of
both constraints to attendance and SMC motivators on SMC substitution
intention, thereby substantiating the role natural consequences play in
this context. In total, the model explains approximately 10% of the
variance in SMC Substitution Intention. Nevertheless, the only
meaningful constraint in explaining consumers' SMC Substitution
Intentions was Cost. The significance of Cost stood in contrast to the
work of Kim and Trail (2010); the finding is understandable given the
current study was conducted in the context of individuals' local
NFL teams, a sport notorious for high price points (Schwab, 2014). Kim
and Trail's (2010) work, on the other hand, utilized Women's
National Basketball Association (WNBA) consumers, a sport that is more
affordably priced. Moreover, it seems cost is a more insurmountable
constraint than others. For example, a fan may be willing to overcome
frigid weather, a bad neighborhood, and the difficulty of parking to
attend an event; however, if he/she simply does not possess the
financial means to attend the event, there is little they can due to
negotiate and/or rectify such a constraint. Further support for this
notion can be gleaned from our finding that highly identified fans
exhibited significantly higher SMC substitution intentions than those
who were low on identification, but the difference was not significant
in the presence of income.
The finding that constraints (aversive stimuli for attending) were,
for the most part, insignificant in predicting SMC Substitution
Intention is consistent with the results from previous research (Kim
& Trail, 2010). From a theoretical standpoint, there is a precedent
of pleasant stimuli as a stronger predictor of behavior than aversive
stimuli given that, in past work, the only two constraints to explain
any significant amount of variance in attendance were Lack of Success
(10%) and Leisure Alternatives (3%; Kim & Trail, 2010). In that
particular study, the survey was distributed at the venue of a team in
second to last place in their conference. As such, it would be expected
that an unsuccessful team would serve to constrain attendance for many
spectators.
The current study, on the other hand, was conducted using a
nationwide sample of NFL fans and spectators, many of whom lived in
markets with successful teams. In addition, given that SMC is a leisure
alternative in and of itself, Leisure Alternatives was not identified as
a constraint that would serve to prompt an individual to substitute SMC
for sport event attendance. In short, the only two constraints of any
significance in Kim and Trail's (2010) study were either not as
pertinent or not applicable in this study and could be part of the
reason for the lack of significance of Constraints in this study.
Nevertheless, the results of this study should give NFL
officials--particularly those in cold weather climates--some peace of
mind, as it seems that fans are not substituting with SMC at home due to
constraints such as weather, location, etc.
Motivators (positive natural reinforcement) had a statistically
significant effect on SMC Substitution Intention. The Motivators factor
encompassed the dimensions of Fantasy Sport, Technological Attributes,
Comfort, Safety, Ease, and Enhancement, factors that do not necessarily
constrain attendance, but were posited to reinforce SMC. The path
loadings of the second order Motivator latent variable suggest this
factor was driven by Technological Attributes, Comfort, and Ease.
Therefore, consistent with past research (Dupagne, 1999; Fairley &
Tyler, 2012; Gantz & Wenner, 1995; Trail et al., 2003b), it seems
individuals exhibit SMC substitution intentions in part due to the
ability to watch a crystal clear broadcast of the event from the comfort
and relaxation of their own home. Four of the seven SMC Motivators
(e.g., Technological Attributes, Comfort, Ease, and Enhancement) were at
least marginally important (mean greater than 4 on a 1 to 7 scale) to
participants, whereas just one Constraint (e.g., Cost) was assessed in
this way. These results suggest that, with the exception of cost,
constraints do not necessarily prompt individuals to substitute SMC for
attendance. Rather, it seems perhaps some consumers simply prefer the
home experience due, at least in small part, to these positive
reinforcers.
We expected Team Identification to moderate the relationship
between constraints and Motivators and SMC Substitution Intention, such
that more highly identified individuals would be able to overcome these
factors and the strength of the relationships would be reduced. In
contrast to past work (e.g., Kim et al., 2013), however, team
identification did not serve to alter the strength of these
relationships in the SMC context. Furthermore, highly identified fans in
the current study exhibited significantly higher substitution intentions
that held even when controlling for the importance of cost. Since
individuals in the high team identification group in this study
exhibited significantly greater and more frequent SMC behaviors than
attendance behaviors, the findings lend some credence to Pritchard and
Funk's (2006) suggestion that media-dominant consumers are perhaps
as highly involved as those placing an emphasis on attendance. At the
root of this finding could be the fact that the home viewing experience
has improved at a rate far greater than the stadium experience over the
last several decades. For example, following from social identity
theory, Smith and Smith (2012) indicated that highly identified fans can
now publicly display their association with their favorite team by way
of social media, an ability previously reserved for the live event.
Their content analysis of tweets sent out during the 2012 College
Baseball World Series suggested that this was a medium used for
commentary, cheering, encouragement, celebration, and jeers.
Interestingly, these behaviors practiced on social media are akin to
BIRGing (basking in reflected glory) and derogating the out-group,
practices long associated with the behavior of highly identified fans
(Branscombe & Wann, 1992). In addition, commentators have argued
that fans' interest in a sport and/or league as a whole, rather
than just one team, has increased rapidly in recent years (Duffey,
2013). Since fans are better able to satisfy these multiple interests
while watching at home, foregoing the live event may be an attractive
option. In short, it seems the home viewing experience has changed quite
substantially in the last 15-20 years in ways particularly well-suited
for highly identified fans of sport. This has created an environment
that seems particularly attractive for this population, though
substantial research is needed to assess the validity of these proposed
explanations.
Implications, Limitations, and Future Research
To our knowledge, the current research is the first to examine the
factors underlying the phenomenon of intentions to substitute SMC for
attendance, a consumption mode proliferating in appeal in recent years
(Pritchard & Funk, 2006). In so doing, the study provides a
foundation and starting point for future research to further examine
constraints and motivators for SMC intention and also provides
theoretical contributions to the field. First, by exhibiting the
significance of Motivators (positive natural reinforcement) and Cost on
SMC Substitution Intention, it extends Skinner's (1957, 1982)
natural consequences framework to a sport consumption context. That is,
although aversive stimuli for attendance had only limited effectiveness
in predicting SMC Substitution Intentions, SMC Motivators were shown to
represent positive natural reinforcements that were significant even for
those highly identified with the team. Thus, the current study shows
that, while literature has suggested constraints serve to prompt
substitution (Pritchard & Funk, 2006), the phenomenon of consumers
substituting SMC for attendance might be more a result of Motivators, or
the pleasant stimuli of home. In addition, the study extends several
research streams in the spectator sport literature. First, it extends
the work done by Trail and his colleagues (e.g., Kim & Trail, 2010;
Trail & Kim, 2010; Trail et al., 2008) by measuring the effect of
constraints to sport event attendance in the context of consumers'
intentions to substitute with SMC. Second, it extends the team
identification literature by examining whether one's psychological
connection to a team can influence their SMC intention. The finding that
the impact of constraints and motivators did not differ between highly
and lowly identified fans, and those in the high team identification
group actually exhibited slightly stronger SMC substitution intentions,
represents a potential contribution to the literature. It suggests the
consumption habits of fans may be changing slightly, a finding with
implications for future research, though further investigation and/or
replication is needed. Finally, the study answers Pritchard and
Funk's (2006) call for future research to examine the relationship
between media use and game attendance, specifically exploring how
motivation and constraints alter consumption.
In addition to these theoretical and empirical contributions, the
study also makes several contributions to practice. With NFL teams
recently struggling to avoid local TV blackouts due to sluggish ticket
sales for the first round of the playoffs (Schwab, 2014), it is no
surprise that fans preferring to watch games at home was identified as
one of the most prominent concerns among sport industry practitioners in
a recent industry survey (Luker, 2012). The current study affords sport
practitioners some potential reasons why fans may seem to prefer to stay
home. As this study shows, it may be more a product of the appealing
elements of the home experience including comfort, enhancement, and
technological attributes, than factors such as location, parking, and
weather that serve to constrain attendance. While cost was statistically
significant, no other constraints appear to play a significant role in
prompting individuals to substitute SMC for attendance.
While the current study offers some initial evidence, sport
marketers should replicate the study on their own consumers to determine
the extent to which the above aspects impact attendance, as many factors
could vary by location. If there are negative impacts, then sport
marketers would know which aspects to address to reduce potential
constraints and improve the venue experience to improve attendance and
possibly avoid future local TV blackouts. For example, organizations
could enhance the live experience by improving facility comfort,
technological capabilities, and making it as easy as possible for fans
upon arriving at the venue. This could include amenities such as
cushioned seats, in-seat concession ordering, and wider concourses and
stairwells to make entry and exit as easy as possible. As Ourand (2015)
noted, organizations are starting to leverage technology to enhance the
live experience, as the Washington Capitals of the National Hockey
League recently introduced an in-arena fantasy game for spectators. In
addition to increasing fan engagement and the enjoyment of the
experience, this also allows the organization to acquire valuable
information about their fans. Furthermore, teams could provide fans at
the stadium with an experience fans at home cannot access. This may
include exclusive looks into the locker room during halftime as well as
audio of players and coaches wearing microphones during games that are
available only to those with a game ticket.
On the other hand, sport organizations may want to embrace the fact
that there is going to be a large contingent of fans watching from home
and leverage technology to increase engagement for this segment. For
example, teams may want to take advantage of the dual-screening trend
(i.e., fans following the conversation on social media and managing
their fantasy teams during the game). By participating in the
discussion--both about the game as well as its implications for fantasy
sport--teams could increase the engagement level and enjoyment of the
home experience.
Despite these theoretical and practical contributions, this
research does come with some limitations. First, data was collected in
relation to just one sport and league (i.e., NFL). It is possible that
consumption of other sports may differ from professional football. With
respect to the sample, two more limitations must be acknowledged.
Although efforts were made to obtain a nationwide representative sample,
a convenience sample and not a true random sample was obtained. While
all markets in the NFL, save three, were represented in the study, it
remains possible that different results could emerge with a true random
sample. Finally, females were underrepresented in the main study, which
is noteworthy given that females have demonstrated significant
differences in consumer motivations in past work (e.g., Fink &
Parker, 2009).
As for future research, given the lack of significance of most of
the constraints in this study, future studies could attempt to identify
constraints that may be more impactful on individuals' SMC
substitution intentions. Second, future work should examine additional
variables beyond team identification that may serve to moderate and/or
mediate the relationships between constraints and motivators and SMC
substitution intentions, thereby explaining a higher amount of variance.
Third, future studies could take a cross-sectional look at males and
females and search for differences in their SMC intentions. Finally,
given that highly identified fans exhibited no significant differences
in SMC intentions and a significantly greater substitution intention,
future research should investigate why even highly identified sport
fans--a segment long shown to exhibit a higher preference to attend--may
now be just as content to watch sport events at home.
References
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equation with latent variables.
New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Branscombe, N. R. & Wann, D. (1992). Role of identification
with a group, arousal, categorization processes, and self-esteem in
sport spectator aggression. Human Relations, 45, 1013-1033.
Catania, A. C. (1998). Learning (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Clavio, G. (2008). Demographics and usage profiles of users of
college sport message boards. International Journal of Sport
Communication, 1, 434-443.
Comunidad Los Horcones. (1992). Natural reinforcement: A way to
improve education. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 71-75.
Crawford, D. W., & Godbey, G. (1987). Reconceptualizing
barriers to family leisure. Leisure Sciences, 9, 119-127.
Crawford, D. W., Jackson, E. L., & Godbey, G. (1991). A
hierarchical model of leisure constraints. Leisure Sciences, 13,
309-320.
Drayer, J., Shapiro, S. L., Dwyer, B., Morse, A. L., & White,
J. (2010). The effects of fantasy football participation on NFL
consumption: A qualitative analysis. Sport Management Review, 13,
129-141.
Duffey, K. (2013, October 1). The real reason college football live
game attendance is down. Saturday down South. Retrieved from
http://www.saturdaydownsouth.com/2013/college-football-live-game-attendance/
Dupagne, M. (1999). Exploring the characteristics of potential
high-definition television adopters. Journal of Media Economics, 12,
35-50.
Dwyer, B., & Drayer, J. (2010). Fantasy sport consumer
segmentation: An investigation into the differing consumption modes of
fantasy football participants. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 19, 207-216.
Fairley, S., & Tyler, B. D. (2012). Bringing baseball to the
big screen: Building sense of community outside of the ballpark. Journal
of Sport Management, 26, 258-270.
Fantasy Sport Trade Association. (2014). Industry demographics.
Retrieved from http://www.fsta.org/?page=Demographics
Fink, J. S., & Parker, H. M. (2009). Spectator motives: Why do
we watch when our favorite team is not playing? Sport Marketing
Quarterly, 18, 210-217.
Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. (1981). Evaluating structural
equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error.
Journal of Marketing Research, 18(1), 39-50.
Funk, D. C., Mahony, D. F., & Ridinger, L. L. (2002).
Characterizing consumer motivation as individual difference factors:
Augmenting the sport interest inventory (SII) to explain level of sport.
Sport Marketing Quarterly, 11, 33-43.
Gantz, W., & Wenner, L. A. (1995). Fanship and the television
sports viewing experience. Sociology of Sport Journal, 12, 56-74.
Gelb, M. (2013, April 26). Why Phillies attendance numbers matter.
Philly.com. Retrieved from
http://www.philly.com/philly/blogs/phillies_zone/Why-Phillies-attendance-numbers-matter.html
Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E.
(2009). Multivariate data analysis (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Pearson Prentice Hall.
Hawkins, D. I., Best, R. J., & Coney, K. A. (2004). Consumer
behavior: Building marketing strategy. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill Irwin.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. (1999). Fit indices in covariance
structure modeling: Sensitivity under parameterized model
misspecification. Psychological Methods, 3, 424-453.
Katz, E., Blumler, J. G., & Gurevitch, M. (1973). Uses and
gratifications research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 37, 509-523.
Kim, H. S., & Lee, S. T. (2003). Exploring the characteristics
of DVD home theater system adopters. Mass Communication & Society,
6, 267-290.
Kim, Y. K., & Trail, G. (2010). Constraints and motivators: A
new model to explain sport consumer behavior. Journal of Sport
Management, 24, 190-210.
Kim, Y. K., Trail, G. T., & Magnusen, M. J. (2013). Transition
from motivation to behaviour: Examining the moderating role of
identification (ID) on the relationship between motives and attendance.
International Journal of Sports Marketing & Sponsorship, 14,
190-211.
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation
modeling (3rd ed.). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Luker, R. (2012, October 1). Shifting interest by age, gender gives
MMA a fighting chance. Street & Smith's SportsBusiness Journal,
15, 17.
Navakatikyan, M. A., Murrell, P., Bensemann, J., Davison, M., &
Elliffe, D. (2013). Law of effect models and choice between many
alternatives. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 100,
222-256.
Ourand, J. (2015, March 2). In arena fantasy game? That's a
capital idea. Street & Smith's SportsBusiness Journal.
Retrieved from http://www.sportsbusinessdaily.com/Journal/Issues/2015/03/02/Media/Sports-Media.aspx
Pritchard, M. P., & Funk, D. C. (2006). Symbiosis and
substitution in spectator sport. Journal of Sport Management, 20,
299-321.
Robinson, M. J., Trail, G. T., & Kwon, H. (2004). Motives and
points of attachment of professional golf spectators. Sport Management
Review, 7, 167-192.
Rovell, D. (2012, November 16). Best seat in the house. ESPN
Outside the Lines. Retrieved from
http://espn.go.com/espn/otl/story/_/id/8636927/nfl-taking-note-many-fans-watching-games-tv-beats-going-stadiums
Schwab, F. (2014, January 1). NFL should be alarmed that three of
four playoff games, including Green Bay's home game, still not sold
out. Yahoo! Sports. Retrieved from
http://sports.yahoo.com/blogs/nfl-shutdown-corner/nfl-alarmed-three-four-playoff-games- including-green-213137570-nfl.html
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. Toronto,
Ontario: The MacMillan Company.
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York, NY:
Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Skinner, B. F. (1982). Contrived reinforcement. The Behavior
Analyst, 5, 3-8.
Smith, R. L., & Smith, K. D. (2012). Identity in Twitter's
hashtag culture: A sport-media-consumption culture case study.
International Journal of Sport Communication, 5, 539-557.
Thorndike, E. L. (1927). The law of effect. The American Journal of
Psychology, 39, 212-222.
Trail, G. T., Anderson, D. F., & Fink, J. S. (2000). A
theoretical model of sport spectator consumption behavior. International
Journal of Sport Management, 1, 154-180.
Trail, G. T., Fink, J. S., & Anderson, D. F. (2003a). Sport
spectator consumption behavior. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 12, 8-17.
Trail, G. T., & James, J. D. (2001). The motivation scale for
sport consumption: Assessment of the scale's psychometric
properties. Journal of Sport Behavior, 24, 108-127.
Trail, G. T., & James, J. D. (2012). Sport consumer behavior.
Seattle, WA: Sport Consumer Research Consultants LLC.
Trail, G. T., & Kim, Y. K. (2011). Factors influencing
spectator sport consumption: NCAA women's basketball. International
Journal of Sports Marketing & Sponsorship, 13, 60-82.
Trail, G. T., Robinson, M. J., Dick, R. J., & Gillentine, A. J.
(2003b). Motives and points of attachment: Fans versus spectators in
intercollegiate athletics. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 12, 217-227.
Trail, G. T., Robinson, M. J., & Kim, Y. K. (2008). Sport
consumer behavior: A test for group differences on structural
constraints. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 17, 190-200.
Wann, D. L. (1995). Preliminary validation of the sport fan
motivation scale. Journal of Sport & Social Issues, 19, 377-396.
Wann, D. L., Melnick, M. J., Russell, G. W., & Pease, D. G.
(2001). Sport fans: The psychology and social impact of spectators. New
York, NY: Routledge.
Weed, M. (2010). Sport fans and travel--Is 'being there'
always important? Journal of Sport & Tourism, 15, 103-109.
Endnote
(1) Participants in the pilot study consisted of sport management
undergraduate students at a large northeastern university who were asked
to complete a paper and pencil survey. This survey was completed during
class time. There were no surveys that included more than a few items of
missing data. Accordingly, any missing items were replaced with the mean
score.
Ben Larkin, MS, is a doctoral candidate in the Department of Sport
Management at the University of Massachusetts Amherst. His research
interests include marketing, consumer behavior, sport media consumption,
and consumer emotion.
Janet S. Fink, PhD, is a professor of sport management and chair of
the Mark H. McCormack Department of Sport Management at the University
of Massachusetts Amherst. Her research interests include sport consumer
behavior, particularly the marketing of female athletes and women's
sport.
Galen T. Trail, PhD, is a professor and director of the
Master's of Sport Administration and Leadership program at Seattle
University. His research focuses on sport consumer behavior.
Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations
Factors Mean SD
Constraints 3.25
No Interest from Significant Others 3.51 1.81
Location 2.41 1.57
Parking 2.87 1.86
Cost 5.44 1.80
No One to Attend with 2.84 1.84
Weather 2.99 1.86
Lack of Success 2.72 1.82
Motivators 4.13
Fantasy 3.46 1.97
Technological Attributes 4.28 2.01
Comfort 4.73 1.96
Safety 3.62 2.00
Ease 4.66 1.94
Enhancement 4.03 1.90
Team Identification 4.67 2.34
SMC Substitution Intention 6.04 1.50
Table 2
Second Order Factor Loadings ([beta]), First Order Factor
Loadings ([lambda]), Alpha Coefficients ([alpha]), and
Average Variance Extracted
Items [beta] [lambda]
Team Identification
Being a fan of the team is important to me .893
I am a committed fan of the team .984
I consider myself to be a "real" fan of the team .978
Constraints
Lack of Success
The team loses more than they win .884
The team is in the bottom half of the .965
conference/league
The team doesn't win many games .932
No One to Attend with
There is no one to go to the game with me .871
A lack of friends to go to the game with me .953
My spouse/significant other won't go to the .639
game with me
Parking
The accessibility of parking for the sport .864
venue is poor
Ease of parking at the arena/stadium is bad .891
A lack of close parking to the sport venue .872
Cost
The financial cost of going to a game is too high .869
The cost of attending games is too much .920
Tickets cost too much .909
Weather
It is often too hot/cold at the stadium/arena .863
The weather makes it uncomfortable at the sport .948
venue
The weather conditions at the sport venue are .909
not ideal
Location
The area/neighborhood surround the arena/stadium .854
is not nice
The sport venue is located in a bad part of town .850
Motivators
Fantasy .639
My home is a better environment for .786
participating in fantasy sport
It is easier to monitor the progress of my .894
fantasy sport team(s) at home
There are a variety of media sources available .907
at home which are useful for fantasy sport
Technological Attributes .887
Technological aspects (e.g., HDTV, surround .827
sound, DVR, playback, etc.) available when
watching at home
When watching at home there are more .917
technological attributes (e.g., HDTV, surround
sound, DVR, playback, etc.) that add to the
experience
The technological characteristics of the home .929
environment (e.g., HDTV, surround sound, DVR,
playback, etc.) have made staying home more
enjoyable
Comfort .983
I am more comfortable at home .787
The seats are more comfortable at home .911
My home is more comfortable than the sport venue .946
Safety
I feel safer at home .765 .845
My home environment provides a sense of security .941
My home is more comfortable than the sport venue .949 .940
Ease
It is easier to watch sport events at home .768
It requires less effort than attending a game .823
I don't have to exert myself as I do when going .735 .754
to the sport venue
Enhancement
The commentators, statistics, live updates, and .869
coverage enhance the consumption experience
The media broadcast enhances the consumption .965
experience
The media broadcast adds to the experience of .932
watching a sport event at home
Items [alpha] AVE
Team Identification .966 0.91
Being a fan of the team is important to me
I am a committed fan of the team
I consider myself to be a "real" fan of the team
Constraints
Lack of Success .948 0.86
The team loses more than they win
The team is in the bottom half of the
conference/league
The team doesn't win many games
No One to Attend with .854 0.69
There is no one to go to the game with me
A lack of friends to go to the game with me
My spouse/significant other won't go to the
game with me
Parking .907 0.77
The accessibility of parking for the sport
venue is poor
Ease of parking at the arena/stadium is bad
A lack of close parking to the sport venue
Cost .926 0.81
The financial cost of going to a game is too high
The cost of attending games is too much
Tickets cost too much
Weather .932 0.82
It is often too hot/cold at the stadium/arena
The weather makes it uncomfortable at the sport
venue
The weather conditions at the sport venue are
not ideal
Location .838 0.73
The area/neighborhood surround the arena/stadium
is not nice
The sport venue is located in a bad part of town
Motivators
Fantasy .896 0.75
My home is a better environment for
participating in fantasy sport
It is easier to monitor the progress of my
fantasy sport team(s) at home
There are a variety of media sources available
at home which are useful for fantasy sport
Technological Attributes .920 0.80
Technological aspects (e.g., HDTV, surround
sound, DVR, playback, etc.) available when
watching at home
When watching at home there are more
technological attributes (e.g., HDTV, surround
sound, DVR, playback, etc.) that add to the
experience
The technological characteristics of the home
environment (e.g., HDTV, surround sound, DVR,
playback, etc.) have made staying home more
enjoyable
Comfort .908 0.78
I am more comfortable at home
The seats are more comfortable at home
My home is more comfortable than the sport venue
Safety
I feel safer at home .935 0.83
My home environment provides a sense of security
My home is more comfortable than the sport venue .821 0.61
Ease
It is easier to watch sport events at home
It requires less effort than attending a game
I don't have to exert myself as I do when going .942 0.85
to the sport venue
Enhancement
The commentators, statistics, live updates, and
coverage enhance the consumption experience
The media broadcast enhances the consumption
experience
The media broadcast adds to the experience of
watching a sport event at home
Table 3
Correlations among Latent Variables
1 2 3 4
1. Team Identification 1
2. Location -0.222 1
3. Parking -0.159 0.612 1
4. Cost -0.018 0.141 0.322 1
5. No One to Attend with -0.113 0.385 0.246 0.099
6. Weather -0.195 0.262 0.443 0.238
7. Fantasy Sport -0.147 0.271 0.358 0.168
8. Technological Attributes -0.122 0.276 0.375 0.338
9. Comfort -0.134 0.297 0.430 0.411
10. Safety -0.160 0.515 0.527 0.278
11. Ease -0.108 0.292 0.388 0.434
12. Enhancement -0.084 0.380 0.411 0.248
13. Lack of Success -0.180 0.402 0.225 0.005
5 6 7 8
1. Team Identification
2. Location
3. Parking
4. Cost
5. No One to Attend with 1
6. Weather 0.209 1
7. Fantasy Sport 0.176 0.492 1
8. Technological Attributes 0.207 0.456 0.653 1
9. Comfort 0.150 0.452 0.612 0.869
10. Safety 0.211 0.452 0.493 0.613
11. Ease 0.213 0.468 0.529 0.806
12. Enhancement 0.211 0.425 0.513 0.763
13. Lack of Success 0.346 0.140 0.222 0.284
9 10 11 12 13
1. Team Identification
2. Location
3. Parking
4. Cost
5. No One to Attend with
6. Weather
7. Fantasy Sport
8. Technological Attributes
9. Comfort 1
10. Safety 0.762 1
11. Ease 0.958 0.731 1
12. Enhancement 0.695 0.546 0.654 1
13. Lack of Success 0.243 0.279 0.214 0.221 1
Table 4
Testing of Hypothesized Relationships on SMC Substitution Intention
Low Team ID (N = 98)
Location [beta] = -.145 (-.603, .304), p = .397
Parking [beta] = .105 (-.344, .554), p = .548
Cost [beta] = -.016 (-.348, .317), p = .904
No One to Attend with [beta] = .069 (-.253, .391), p = .582
Weather [beta] = .104 (-.244, .452), p = .443
Lack of Success [beta] = .040 (-.242, .323), p = .712
Motivators [beta] = .290 (-.068, .648), p = .037
High Team ID (N = 146)
Location [beta] = -.066 (-.375, .245), p = .588
Parking [beta] = .023 (-.273, .324), p = .824
Cost [beta] = .235 (.021, .449), p = .005
No One to Attend with [beta] = -.087 (-.308, .134), p = .312
Weather [beta] = -.018 (-.260, .224), p = .849
Lack of Success [beta] = -.142 (-.380, .097), p = .127
Motivators [beta] = .227 (-.031, .485), p = .023
Whole Group (N = 244)
Location [beta] = -.070 (-.328, .188), p = .485
Parking [beta] = .021 (-.231, .274), p = .828
Cost [beta] = .161 (-.022, .343), p = .023
No One to Attend with [beta] = -.057 (-.241, .127), p = .429
Weather [beta] = .028 (-.170, .226), p = .720
Lack of Success [beta] = -.059 (-.245, .127), p = .416
Motivators [beta] = .240 (.029, .450), p = .003
* 90% Confidence Intervals