首页    期刊浏览 2025年06月30日 星期一
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Expected price and user image for branded and co-branded sports apparel.
  • 作者:Wu, D. Gloria ; Chalip, Laurence
  • 期刊名称:Sport Marketing Quarterly
  • 印刷版ISSN:1061-6934
  • 出版年度:2013
  • 期号:September
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:Fitness Information Technology Inc.
  • 摘要:The economic impact of sport is due largely to the ways that sport ripples through manufacturing and consumer spending (Meek, 1997; Milano & Chelladurai, 2011). The economic ripples of sport become manifest, in part, through the effect of sport-related brands, such as sportswear brands--for example, Nike and Adidas. Sportswear brands have long been associated with athletes and teams, which typically display the brands on their apparel. Given the cultural salience and popular sport associations thereby imparted, sportswear companies began to widen their net during the 1990s by creating products aimed at an audience that wanted to project a sporty image, even if not an athlete. Given their growing economic clout, sport-related brands became attractive as co-branding partners for non-sport brands.
  • 关键词:Brand image;Brand name products;Brand names;Clothing;Clothing and dress

Expected price and user image for branded and co-branded sports apparel.


Wu, D. Gloria ; Chalip, Laurence


Expected Price and User Image for Branded and Co-Branded Sports Apparel

The economic impact of sport is due largely to the ways that sport ripples through manufacturing and consumer spending (Meek, 1997; Milano & Chelladurai, 2011). The economic ripples of sport become manifest, in part, through the effect of sport-related brands, such as sportswear brands--for example, Nike and Adidas. Sportswear brands have long been associated with athletes and teams, which typically display the brands on their apparel. Given the cultural salience and popular sport associations thereby imparted, sportswear companies began to widen their net during the 1990s by creating products aimed at an audience that wanted to project a sporty image, even if not an athlete. Given their growing economic clout, sport-related brands became attractive as co-branding partners for non-sport brands.

Co-branding is a brand alliance strategy in which two or more brands are simultaneously presented to consumers in order to strengthen their individual brand images (Geylani, Inman, & Ter Hofstede, 2008). This strategy has been recommended as a tool for differentiation that leverages brands through the transfer of positive associations among partners in the brand alliance, including brand equity, image, and awareness (McCarthy & Norris, 1999; Simonin & Ruth, 1998; Washburn, Till, & Priluck, 2000). Presumably, this should enhance consumers' perceptions of the value of a product (Simonin & Ruth, 1995), which would therefore render a price premium (Persson, 2010). Although the logic for a pricing effect is clearly articulated, no study to date has tested whether the predicted effect occurs.

The significance is elevated by the ways that sport associations are increasingly used in co-branding contexts to render the predicted effect. For example, sportswear brands (e.g., Nike, Adidas) are increasingly being used by fashion designers to enhance the equity in their brands (Thomas, 2010), while sportswear brands are seeking the same from their association with high-fashion brands (Mitchell, 2005). The case of co-branding between sportswear brands and fashion designer brands is particularly interesting not merely due to the importation of sport-related associations into a fashion context, and vice versa, but also because these two different kinds of brands have traditionally had different or even contradictory brand images, and have even targeted different market segments (cf. Holmes & Tierney, 2002; Lau & Axelrod, 2009).

It is possible, however, that the value of co-branding comes not merely from particular associations that are imported from a brand partner, but also from the symbolic impact engendered by co-branding, particularly when two very different kinds of brands are partnered. Consumers do not buy products just for their practical utility, but also for the symbolic meaning that products convey (Elliot, 1997; Levy, 1959), which has particular relevance to what consumers are saying about themselves by the choices they make. It has been argued that individuals may have a preference for particular brands over others because they perceive themselves to be similar to others who use brands they favor, and/or because they seek to identify themselves with others who are thought to use particular brands (see Sirgy, 1982 for a review, and Sirgy, 1986 for a comprehensive description of the underlying theory). Thus, the judgments that consumers make about what brands and, by extension, brand alliances say about users are important. For example, if sportswear brands and fashion brands convey different images of users, then the combination of the two should complicate or elaborate the image in ways that are not yet well-understood. Further, the image of users that are projected as a function of brand should affect perceptions of value, and consequently of price, because consumers are paying for a particular image of themselves and what they want to project to the world, as well as the practical utility of the purchase (Blackston, 2000; Evans, 1968). This is especially, albeit not uniquely, true for clothing (Braun & Wicklund, 1989).

To date, co-branding studies have focused primarily on consumers' attitudes towards the brand alliance per se, rather than how consumers evaluate price and user image with reference to co-branded products, their single-branded alternatives, or unbranded alternatives. The purpose of this study was to examine consumers' expectations regarding price and user image when a sport-related (i.e., sportswear) brand is paired with a fashion designer brand, and to determine whether the effect on expected price is, at least in part, a function of the effect on consumers' projections regarding user image.

Literature Review

Brand User Image

Over forty years of research in marketing (e.g., Elliott, 1994; Elliott & Wattanasuwan, 1998; Hirschman, 1981; Keller, 1998; Levy, 1959) has shown that the perceptions and associations consumers have about brands go beyond their functional attributes and benefits, and include non-functional, symbolic qualities. Researchers have argued that individuals often use symbolic brand meanings for personal-expression and social communication (Elliott, 1997; McCracken, 1988, 1993).

This argument is rooted in two complementary streams of work, symbolic self-completion theory and symbolic interaction. Symbolic self-completion theory (Wicklund & Gollwizter, 1982) argues that many behaviors, such as purchase choice, are intended to substantiate the consumer's definition of themselves. Accordingly, individuals are particularly likely to value symbols, such as possessions, that reinforce identities about which they may feel insecure but to which they are committed. Rather than merely value these possessions, individuals also like other people to recognize their symbolic worth.

Symbolic interaction theories (Leigh & Gabel, 1992; Blumer, 1969; Kinch, 1967; Mead, 1934; Solomon, 1983) also suggest symbols play a pivotal role in human behavior. Symbolic interaction contends that humans value meanings and devise ways to associate meanings to their actions (Kaiser, Nagasawa, & Hutton, 1991). A symbol can be a stimulus with a learned meaning and value, and the person's response to the stimulus is in terms of this meaning (Rose, 1962). There are three fundamental postulates in the symbolic interaction theory: "1. A consumer's self-concept is based on perceptions of the responses of others. 2. A consumer's self-concept functions to direct behavior. 3. A consumer's perception of the responses of others to some degree reflects those responses." (Solomon, 1983, p. 320).

Brands can be used by consumers as resources for the symbolic construction of the self (Elliott & Wattanasuwan, 1998). Claims that brands could affect user image are grounded in self-congruity theory. According to self-congruity theory (Sirgy, 1982, 1986), consumers evaluate and compare their self-concept with the image of a stereotypic and generalized brand user, which is referred as "user-image," and prefer to buy products that match their self-perceptions. By choosing brands with particular image associations (e.g., elegant, sporty), consumers seek to project an impression to others of the type of person they are (or that they want to be seen as)--a process that also enhances their own self-image and psychological well-being (Sirgy, 2012). Within a product category, the value that a particular brand has for the consumer depends in no small way on what it is thought to communicate about the user (Aquirre-Rodriguez, Bosnjak, & Sirgy, 2012; Parker, 2009).

Little is known about the relationship between price and user image, although it has been shown that consumers associate higher status with higher prices (Leigh & Gabel, 1992; Wright, Claiborne, & Sirgy, 1992). This finding has been extended into studies of so-called luxury brands, which are normally high priced. Research demonstrates that the positive image associated with luxury brand purchases plays a key role in their purchase (Liu, Li, Mizerski, & Soh, 2012; Nueno & Quelch, 1998). Thus, higher priced goods are normally expected to convey a positive image of the user, but it is not clear how specific user characteristics projected by a brand might affect consumers' expectations about price. The fact of a relationship seems clear, although its specific mechanisms are not yet well understood. Since sportswear brands and designer brands convey different user images (Azevedo & Farhangmehr, 2005). Therefore, it is expected that consumers will describe wearers of each type of brand, as well as the combination of such brands, differently. That should affect the value and, consequently, the price that they expect to pay, as noted above. For the purpose of this study, it is necessary to find a scale that categorizes and explains the important dimensions represented by adjectives that people use to describe a product user's personality characteristics.

Brand User Image Measurement

If the image that is projected by using a brand is important, then it is necessary to identify appropriate measures of that image. It has proven difficult to untangle the theoretical issues from measurement concerns. Aguirre-Rodriguez, Bosnjak, and Sirgy (2012) argued that self-congruity evaluations during product choice can be either holistic or piecemeal. In other words, consumers can make global evaluations of the self-congruity associated with a product, or they can take a piecemeal approach wherein they identify particular traits they associate with product alternatives and make a choice based on the different traits. Although they found that the self-congruity effect is generally stronger and more complete when consumers use holistic (rather than piecemeal) evaluations for a product class, they also found that brand self-congruity effects are stronger when processing is piecemeal (rather than holistic). In other words, when evaluating a brand, evaluation with reference to particular traits is stronger than holistic evaluation. If that is the case, then a piecemeal approach to evaluation may better capture the detailed trait-by-trait comparisons used when evaluating brands (or co-brands).

It has been argued elsewhere that there might be a transfer from brand personality to consumer personality such that by building a particular brand personality, a brand could contribute to building a particular, perhaps ideal, self-image for consumers (Fennis, Pruyn, & Maasland, 2005), particularly in the case of clothing (Fennis & Pruyn, 2007). If this is true, then it might be appropriate to use a brand personality scale to measure user image. However, there are two reasons that might not be the best way to proceed. First, it has been argued that the current measures of brand personality do not, in fact, measure brand personality, but instead aggregate multiple dimensions of brand identity (Azoulay & Kapferer, 2003; Caprara, Barbaranelli, & Guido, 2001). Second, although brand personality and user image are expected to be associated through consumers' search for congruity, they are conceptually distinct components of an overall process by which consumers evaluate products and brands (cf. Aaker, 1997; Sirgy, 1983).

An alternative approach derives from the extensive work on semantic differentials deriving from Osgood, Suci, and Tannenbaum (1957), which consistently finds that three dimensions--which Osgood et al. call "evaluation, activity, and potency"--usefully and adequately capture judgments about people and things. Indeed, Mehrabian (1980) has argued that these three dimensions, which he calls "pleasure, arousal, and dominance" can (and should) be generally applied to measure people's judgments about the world. The basis for that argument begins with Snider and Osgood's (1969) contention that bipolar adjectives provide an ideal means to capture and describe people's emotions and attitudes. Conceptual support comes from semiotic theory which holds that humans employ opposites in the cognitive identification of concepts; we tend to understand what something is by first knowing what it is not (Chandler, 2002; Valentine & Evans, 1993).

A substantial body of work recommends the three dimensional model derived from semantic differentials. The model has been found to be highly portable, and to identify and describe the feelings people share about objects (Brewer, 2004; Westerink, Krans, & Ouwerkerk, 2011). The three-dimensional structure holds up with a wide variety of subjects, concepts, and scales (Heise, 1970), across cultures (Osgood, May, & Miron, 1975), and across applications. The measure has been shown to describe gendered traits (Kroska, 2002; Langford & MacKinnon, 2000), leadership styles (Schneider & Schroder, 2012), and the underlying meaning of occupational prestige scores (MacKinnon & Langford, 1994). The three dimensions have been shown to be particularly relevant to assessments of personality, as the evaluative dimension has been identified with status (Kemper & Collins, 1990), while the activity dimension has been associated with emotional energy (Collins, 1990) as well as self-identified expressiveness (Heise, 1989), and the potency dimension has been related with power (Kemper & Collins, 1990). The three dimensions have been extensively adopted by psychologists, consumer researchers, and other social scientists because they have demonstrated excellent reliability, validity, and parsimony (Abbott, Shackleton, & Holland, 2008; Heise, 1970).

In this study, we employ these same three dimensions. In previous work, authors have varied somewhat in the labels that they apply to each. We have endeavored to retain labels that are comparable to those used in most previous work, and that seem to best capture what the measures describe here. So we have called the dimensions "goodness," "engagement," and "dominance."

Brands and Expected Price

Since brands convey meaning to consumers, they also convey a sense of value. Forsythe (1991) showed that brand names affect shoppers' expectations regarding quality and price. Brand affects market share and consumer choice (Agarwal & Rao, 1996; Ailawadi, Lehmann, & Neslin, 2003), while price has been shown to be the most useful indicator of brand equity and associated profitability (Aaker, 1996; Blackston, 1995). Consequently, differences in the expected price of differently branded products are useful for determining the effect that particular brands have on consumer evaluations of a product.

Since co-branded products carry more than a single brand, the effect on expected price should be elevated, at least if the brand alliance enhances consumer perceptions of the product. Schema theory provides the bases for expecting an increase in price when a product is co-branded, as well as the added complexity of user image, as described at the beginning of this literature review. Schema theory explains how people make meaning out of information. A schema can be identified as a generic or abstract knowledge structure in an individual's memory that is used to guide encoding, organization, and retrieval of information (Stein & Trabasso, 1982). It consists of category attributes of stimulus domains as well as their links, prototypic exemplars, and affective tags assessing one's attitude toward members of the category (Robertson & Kassarijian, 1991; Goodstein, 1993). The process of integrating new information into an existing schema allows for held beliefs to be modified or reinforced. Accordingly, in the case of co-branding, each brand logo plays the role of a product cue that activates self-schemata. When consumers receive information about two different brands, the two different brand images each affect consumers' perceptions of the user image. Studies of co-branding do suggest that consumers' attitudes or perceptions regarding brand-user-image can be changed by the information they incorporate about the two brands (Ahn, Kim, & Forney, 2010).

This, in turn, should affect expected price. Some evidence comes from research demonstrating that co-branding can alter consumers' expected utility for products such that consumers commonly overestimate the price of a co-branded product (Chang, 2008). On the other hand, if there is asymmetry in what two brands contribute to a co-branded alliance or if the two brands match poorly, there can be a suppressive effect, which might then have a negative effect on expected price (Venkatesh & Mahajan, 1997, 2009). Thus, while a brand alliance should affect consumers' expectations regarding price, the direction of effect could vary as a function of which brands are allied.

Gender Differences

Men and women have been considered as two different but important market segments, especially in the case of apparel. Gender differences have been reported in a wide range of studies in advertising and marketing. Research shows that male and female customers perceive brands differently (Elliott, 1994; Kamineni, 2005), and women were more likely to attribute positive characteristics to owners of fashion products (Mayer & Belk, 1985). Furthermore, females are generally more innovative in terms of fashion and shopping (Kim & Kim, 2004; McCracken & Roth, 1989; Stith & Goldsmith, 2006).

These differences extend to perceptions of user characteristics and prices. Culturally prescribed expectations regarding appropriate gender-based differences in user characteristics affect product and brand preferences (Jubas, 2011). Women are also more likely than men to view high prices as socially acceptable (Maxwell, 1999).

Taken together, these findings demonstrate that male and female consumers typically evaluate products and brands differently. These differences extend to projected user characteristics, and to price. For those reasons, it may be necessary to evaluate effects of brands and co-branding separately for males and females.

Research Questions

Although there are grounds in preceding studies for expecting that there might be an effect of combining a sportswear brand with a fashion brand, no previous work has examined whether there is an effect.

Consequently, nine questions derive from the preceding review:

1. Is there a general branding effect on perceptions of user image and expected price for a piece of clothing? In other words, is a brand of any kind (whether single or co-brand) better than no brand?

2. Is there a difference between sportswear brands and fashion brands on perceptions of user image and expected price for a piece of clothing?

3. Is there a general co-branding effect on perceptions of user image and expected price for a piece of clothing?

4. Does adding a fashion brand to a sportswear brand help the sportswear brand with reference to user image and expected price for a piece of clothing? In other words, is the fashion brand better off when a sportswear brand is added?

5. Does adding a sportswear brand to a fashion brand help the fashion brand with reference to user image and expected price for a piece of clothing? In other words, is the sportswear brand better off when a fashion brand is added?

6. Is the co-branding effect the same for different fashion designer brands with reference to user image and expected price for a piece of clothing?

7. Is the co-branding effect the same for different sportswear brands with reference to user image and expected price for a piece of clothing?

8. How well does consumer evaluation of user image for a piece of clothing predict expected price?

9. Do branding and co-branding effects vary as a function of gender?

The last question can be examined via MANOVA. Each of the first seven questions can be answered within the context of a single analysis (following MANOVA) using planned orthogonal comparisons (one for each question). Question 8 requires a regression analysis.

Method

Pretest: Choice of Sportswear Brands and Fashion Designer Brands.

Two initial studies of sportswear brands and fashion designer brands were conducted in order to select the brands to be used in this study. We sought two of each type of brand in order to determine if the effects we found were brand-specific or were comparable in each brand category (Research Question 6 and 7). To better understand the brands that are both familiar and liked, we sought to pinpoint the level to which fashion designer and sportswear brand names were familiar and liked.

Fashion designers were examined in the first calibration study. Four designers were selected from the list of "Top 10 American Fashion Designers" (Vercillo, 2009). The four designers--Marc Jacobs, Ralph Lauren, Tommy Hilfiger, and Oscar de la Renta--had no affiliation with any sportswear brand, and all four were male and American, which assured that subsequent findings would not be biased by previous alliances with a sportswear brand, or differences in designer gender or nationality. Further, four fake designer names were created and added into the list to test the overall reliability of participants' answers.

Forty students at a large American university rated how familiar they were with the designers and how likeable they thought the designers were using a 7-point Likert-type scale. A random order of brands was used on each questionnaire to eliminate order effects. One fake brand was reported to have positive familiarity by two students. Their questionnaires were removed from further analysis. The remaining 38 participants' mean ratings were then averaged. The results showed that Tommy Hilfiger and Ralph Lauren scored highest and near the top of the scale in categories of familiarity and likability. They were selected for the final study.

The second study followed similar procedures to determine which sportswear brands would be used in the final study. Initially, eight sportswear brands, Lotto, Nike, Starter, Adidas, Puma, Champion, Umbro, and Harnes, were selected from a list of local store brands. Lists in which the names were randomized were given to a different set of 40 students at the same university as the previous calibration study. Moreover, the same scales of familiarity and likability from the first study were used in this study. Nike and Adidas were determined to be the most familiar and best liked, so those two were chosen for the final study.

Participants

College students were deemed the best participants for the main study, because of their higher acuity of consumer involvement (O'Cass, 2000), and because they are a key target demographic for co-branded apparel (Cheng, 2010; Thomas, 2010). Participants were 275 students (110 men and 165 women) ranging in age from 18-42 years (M=22.65, SD=3.98). Participants were recruited from six undergraduate liberal arts classes at a large public university in the American southwest. No participant was in more than one of these classes. Most (50.5%) were Caucasian; the second highest ethnicity reported (23.6%) was Asian; the remainder (25.9%) reported an array of other ethnicities. To avoid prior knowledge about branding, co-branding, or pricing, students who had obtained coursework in advertising, economics, or marketing were eliminated, and did not participate. None of the potential participants was majoring in fashion design, which would also have been grounds for elimination.

Design

A 3 (fashion designer brand) X 3 (sportswear brand) experimental design was employed. The three levels of fashion designer brand were: absence of fashion designer brand logo, presence of fashion designer logo A (Ralph Lauren), and presence of fashion designer logo B (Tommy Hilfiger). The three levels of sportswear brand were: absence of sportswear brand logo, presence of sportswear brand logo A (Nike), and presence of sportswear brand logo B (Adidas). Thus, there were nine conditions in this experiment. Each participant was randomly assigned to one of the nine experimental conditions with the requirement that an equivalent number of women and men were in each condition.

Guided by Swartz (1983), polo shirts were chosen as an appropriate stimulus because polo shirts display different brands, and those brands make a difference in consumer perceptions. Further, polo shirts are widely worn by both men and women, and are widely available in both sportswear and fashion designer brands. Following Sproles (1981), nine black and white drawings were developed to correspond to the nine experimental conditions. All nine conditions were accompanied by the description, "All colors and sizes are available." To optimize logo location, a pretest was conducted with 30 students in an undergraduate class (who were not included in the main study) in which logos were located in different places on the shirt. All participants preferred the unbalanced logo placement (sportswear logo and fashion designer logo on the left side of the neckline). Therefore, the unbalanced placement (which is the same as is used in nearly all polo shirts on the market) was used in this study.

Measures

Two sets of scales needed to be identified or developed in order to answer the research questions: a scale to measure expected user characteristics and a scale to measure expected price.

Development of the user image scale. A trait adjective scale to describe wearers was developed to represent the three dimensions commonly found in semantic differential work: goodness, engagement, and dominance. Forty-five differential adjectives were drawn from previous studies (Hannover & Kuhnen, 2002; Holman, 1980; Peluchette & Karl, 2007). Following review by a group of 10 students who evaluated the suitability of each pair as a reasonable descriptor of persons wearing a polo shirt, 13 adjective pairs were selected to represent the three target dimensions. This yielded a list of 5 adjective pairs for the goodness dimension, 3 adjective pairs for the engagement dimension, and 5 adjective pairs for the dominance dimension, each of which was rated on a 7-point scale.

The 13 semantic differential items were used in the main study, in which each respondent was asked to rate a person who might wear the polo shirt that was pictured. The direction of semantic differentials was varied to prevent response set.

Confirmatory factor analysis was used to test and refine the scale. The model with all 13 items yielded marginal fit ([chi square] = 236.27, df = 62, p<0.001, RMR = 0.11, GFI = 0.88, AGFI = 0.83, CFI = 0.79, RMSEA = 0.10). Examination of the standardized loadings for each item onto its latent trait revealed that four items (two for the goodness dimension and two for the dominance dimension) had loadings below 0.5. Those items were removed and the model was refit. The resulting model showed suitable fit ([chi square] = 68.92, df = 24, p<0.001, RMR = 0.07, GFI = 0.95, AGFI = 0.90, CFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.08). The average variance extracted for the three dimensions was substantially higher than the squared multiple correlations between the dimensions, indicating that the three dimensions were adequately independent (0.38<AVE<0.53; .11<[r.sup.2]<.26). The three items representing each of the three dimensions were averaged to obtain the score for each dimension that was used in the final study. Scores for each could therefore range from 1 to 7. Alpha coefficients and standardized factor loadings (from AMOS) for each subscale are shown in Table 1.

Price. Using prices for polo shirts in local stores, 12 different price ranges, from "less than $10" to "$111 or more," were listed. Each category had a $10 range. Participants were asked to select the price they expected would be charged for the polo shirt they were shown. For purposes of data analysis, the midpoint of the price range selected was entered. Thus, the difference between groups was the average number of dollars of difference in expected price. Since no participant picked the highest level price category, it was possible to estimate a midpoint for all categories chosen.

Procedure

The picture and survey instruments were distributed to participants in each class. Participants were instructed first to read a cover letter that explained what would follow along with a consent form. All students invited to participate consented to do so. The cover letter instructed participants to examine the picture and to read the words (which specified that the shirt was available in all colors and sizes) carefully before turning to the survey. They were also instructed to refer to the picture whenever they felt that was necessary when answering the questions. The only time they could not turn back was during the manipulation check immediately following their viewing of the picture, at which time they were asked to identify (from a checklist) which brands they had seen. Eleven respondents failed to identify the brands correctly, so their data was eliminated from further analysis. (There were initially 286 participants, but elimination of these 11 brought the total to the 275 reported above.) After the manipulation check, they responded to the scales for user image and expected price, followed by items measuring gender, age, major, and ethnicity.

Results

A 3x3x2 MANOVA was used to test the effects of fashion designer brand, sportswear brand, and gender on the three user image dimensions and expected price. The three levels of fashion designer brand (Ralph Lauren, Tommy Hilfiger, none), and the three levels of sportswear brand (Nike, Adidas, none) and gender (male and female) were the independent variables. The four dependent variables were expected price and the three dimensions of the user image scale (Goodness, Engagement, Dominance). The four dependent variables were sufficiently correlated to enable confidence in the statistics, but not so highly correlated as to be redundant (see Table 2.). The three-way interaction was statistically significant; Wilks' Lambda=0.885; F[16,777]=1.99, p=0.01; partial eta squared = 0.03. This three-way interaction suggests that other effects depend on the consumer's gender.

To consider that possibility, which is also Research Question 9, we examined the gender difference on means and on the relationships between price and the three user image dimensions. Examination of the univariate tests showed that the three way interaction was significant for dominance (F[4,257]=4.78, p=0.001; partial eta squared = 0.07) and goodness (F[4, 257]=4.53, p=0.001; partial eta squared = 0.07). The interaction for engagement (F[4,257]=1.39, p=0.23; partial eta squared = 0.02) and price (F[4, 257]=0.78, p=0.54; partial eta squared = 0.01) was not significant, although there was a significant main effect of gender on engagement(F[1,257]=3.32, p=0.05; partial eta squared = 0.02). Taken as a whole, these findings indicate that males and females evaluate the user image for these shirts differently. It is heuristically useful, therefore, to treat the two genders as separate populations when comparing means. Consequently, analyses addressing Research Questions 1-7 are conducted separately for males and females.

Treatment Effects for Males

ANOVAs with planned orthogonal contrasts provided precise tests of each of the first seven research questions for male participants (Seltman, 2012, pp. 319-338). The first contrast tested the mean of the eight conditions with at least one brand against the condition with no brand. The second tested the mean of the fashion designer brands against the mean of the sportswear brands. The third tested the mean of the single branded shirts against the mean of the cobranded shirts. The fourth tested the mean for shirts with only a sportswear brand against the mean for all cobranded shirts. The fifth tested the mean for shirts with only a fashion brand against the mean for all cobranded shirts. The sixth and seventh tested whether co-branding effects were comparable for the two fashion designer brands or the two sportswear brands, respectively. Results are summarized in Table 3. The contrast values shown in Table 3 are the weighted mean differences for the contrast required to provide a direct test of each research question. Table 3 also shows Cohen's d, which estimates the size of the difference between the two means compared in each contrast. The size of effect is shown in standard deviation units. As a rule of thumb, a d around 0.2 is considered small, a d around 0.5 is considered medium, and a d of 0.8 or greater is considered large.

Examination of Table 3 shows that men felt that a brand or co-brand would, on average, be $24 more expensive than no brand. This was a large and statistically significant effect. They also felt that a fashion brand was almost $4 more expensive than a sportswear brand, an amount that was also large and statistically signifcant. Finally, they felt that co-branding by adding a fashion brand to a sportswear brand would increase price by almost $6, an amount that was also large and statistically significant. However, they did not think that adding a sportswear brand to a fashion brand would have a significant effect one way or the other on price. In other words, a fashion brand added perceived value to the sportswear brand, but a sportswear brand had neither a positive nor a negative impact when added to the fashion brand. Other comparisons were also insignificant.

Brands also made a significant difference in the ways that males perceived wearers of the shirts. Having a brand of any kind rendered a perception that the wearer was significantly more engaged. If the product was single branded, a fashion brand rendered a significantly higher rating for engagement, dominance, and goodness than did a sportswear brand. These effects were medium to large. Co-branding had no significant effect on perceptions of goodness or engagement, but it had a significant negative impact on perception of dominance. In other words, a single branded shirt rendered a higher perception of dominance than did a cobranded shirt. This was due primarily to the effect that co-branding had for the fashion brand, for which perceptions of dominance were substantially reduced, whereas the effect for the sportswear brand was insignificant. The same was true in the case of goodness and engagement. In short, to the degree that goodness, engagement, and dominance are desirable qualities to represent, co-branding was detrimental for fashion brands, but neither helpful nor harmful for sportswear brands on average. However, under the cobranded condition, Nike was substantially (and significantly) better off than Adidas in terms of perceptions of wearer dominance and engagement, and Ralph Lauren was moderately (and significantly) better off than Tommy Hilfiger in terms of wearer goodness.

Treatment Effects for Females

ANOVAs with planned orthogonal contrasts provided precise tests of each of the first seven research questions for female participants (Seltman, 2012, pp. 319338). Contrasts were the same as those for males. Results are summarized in Table 4. The contrast values shown in Table 4 are the weighted mean differences for the contrast required to answer each research question directly, and Cohen's d provides an estimate of effect size in standard deviation units.

Examination of Table 4 shows that as with the men, the presence of any brand whatsoever (single or co-brand) was expected to render a substantially and significantly higher price (almost $21 higher) than when no brand was displayed. Females also felt that a fashion brand was worth significantly more ($2.55 more) than a sportswear brand. This was a large effect. They felt that when a fashion brand co-branded with a sportswear brand, it was worth significantly less ($3 less) than when it was displayed on its own. This was a moderate effect. They did not feel that a sportswear brand was helped or hurt significantly by being paired with a fashion brand.

Branding and co-branding had fewer effects on females' perceptions regarding the wearer of the shirt than either did for males. Females felt that a fashion brand demonstrated moderately (but significantly) higher engagement than did a sportswear brand, and they felt that allying a sportswear brand with a fashion brand moderately (but significantly) lowered the fashion designer brand's demonstration of engagement. They felt that any form of co-branded shirt demonstrated moderately lower wearer goodness than did a single-branded alternative. Finally, if there was to be co-branding, they felt that Nike was moderately (but significantly) better off than was Adidas in terms of goodness and dominance.

Effects of User Image on Expected Price

Although mean differences between treatment groups differ as a function of gender, the effect of user image dimensions on price may not. This is Research Question 8. To test whether the relationship varies as function of gender, tests for linear restrictions were conducted (Katos, Lawler, & Seddighi, 2000, pp. 4059). The relationship between the three user image dimension variables and expected price does not significantly differ by gender (F[4, 267]=0.726, p>0.05). Therefore, to answer Research Question 8, the regression analysis is conducted for males and females jointly.

In order to examine the relationship between evaluations of user image and expected price, expected price was regressed on the three user image dimensions. The three user image dimensions provided small but significant prediction of expected price ([R.sup.2] = 0.07, p<0.01). Examination of Table 5 shows that both engagement and dominance provide significant prediction of expected price. The higher a participant's rating of the wearer's engagement or dominance, the higher they expect the price to be.

Discussion

Findings here are partially but not fully consistent with Blackett and Boad's (1999) suggestion that co-branded products should command a premium above the prices enjoyed by equivalent products bearing only a single brand. Although display of any brand or co brand rendered a much higher expected price than did the display of no brand, allying sportswear brands with fashion designer brands was only helpful for sportswear brands, and only among males. The price males expected to pay for fashion designer brands was neither helped nor hurt by co-branding. For females, on the other hand, the expected price for fashion designer brands was hurt by co-branding with a sportswear brand, and the sportswear brands' expected price was not significantly helped by co-branding. The reasons for these effects are unclear, as the observed asymmetries are not adequately accounted for by current theories of brand associations or gender differences.

The percentage of variance attributable to gender in the three-way interaction between brand types and gender was small, and was not significant in univariate tests on each variable. Although the overall pattern of results here is consistent with the expectation that males and females will evaluate clothing (in this case in terms of projected user characteristics) differently, the effect is subtle. It is not merely that males and females provide different ratings; the pattern of rating differences varies by gender, which suggests that they are processing clothing brands differently. The differences are apparent when user characteristics are considered, but not in terms of price. Future work should explore differences in the cognitive processing males and females apply when evaluating brand cues, particularly with reference to images and evaluations of a product other than price.

Cognitive consistency theory (Schewe, 1973) predicts that consumers will seek to maintain consistency and internal harmony among their attitudes. Accordingly, when evaluating a product with two brands, consumers should assimilate their attitudes towards the parent brands such that their attitudes towards the co-brand would be an average of the parent brand attitudes (Levin, Davis, & Levin, 1996). That seems to be the case in part. Females reduce the value they associate with a fashion designer brand, and males increase the value they associate with a sportswear brand. Thus, for females the lower value of the sportswear brand does reduce the perceived value of the fashion designer brand when the two are paired, as cognitive consistency theory predicts. Similarly, for males, the higher perceived value of the fashion designer brand does increase the perceived value of the sportswear brand, as the theory predicts.

However, the effects are asymmetric. The decrease in the value of a fashion brand that females perceive, and the increase in the value of the sportswear brand that males perceive are not matched by comparable effects for the sportswear brand (in the case of women), or the fashion designer brand (in the case of men). Cognitive consistency theory does not accommodate asymmetric effects of this kind.

Perhaps the asymmetry can be explained by gender role expectation regarding apparel preferences and shopping styles (cf. Bae & Miller, 2009; Belk, Mayer, & Driscoll, 1984; Campbell, 1997; Workman & Studak, 2006), as well as gender differences in information processing when shopping (Laroche, Saad, Cleveland, & Browne, 2000). Thus, women would establish the fashion brand as the point of reference because that is popularly deemed most gender appropriate for them, while men would take the sports brand as their point of reference because that is popularly deemed most gender appropriate for them. The effect of a co-brand would depend on how evaluation of the partner brand would raise or lower the value of the reference brand, but the same would not be true for a brand that is not a reference brand. In other words, the effect of co-branding may depend on which kind of brand is preferred in the first place, rather than on simple averaging of the value for partner brands or on aggregation of the two brands' equity. Future research should test that possibility.

Worth, Smith, and Mackie (1992) suggest that gender self-image affects evaluation of products described in gender-relevant terms. Regardless of the traditional image of the product, and regardless of the gender of the perceiver, people prefer a product that is described in terms that matched the gender attributes that they perceived as both characteristic of and important to themselves. This may suggest that women respond less favorably toward fashion brands co-branding with a sport brand because they associate sportswear brands with a masculine user image and fashion brands with a feminine user image. On the other hand, if this were true, it should also be the case that females would give more positive ratings to the fashion and sportswear cobranded product than the sportswear product without a fashion co-brand. That was not the case here. Although gender schematicity may provide some insight into the gender differences observed here, it may not fully predict our findings.

Nevertheless, gender differences are certainly apparent here in the ways that user image varies as a function of brand and co-brand. When considered in terms of the number of significant effects, males' image of users was far more affected than was females' image of users. It would tempting, therefore, to suggest that males are more sensitive to what clothing brands symbolize than are women, but that would be inconsistent with other research which demonstrates the opposite (e.g., Belk et al., 1984). In general, women demonstrate higher involvement in clothing purchases than do men (O'Cass, 2000). Consequently, their attitudes and the evaluative criteria they apply are more highly crystallized (Belk, 1978). That may be what is demonstrated by the findings here, as females provided highly focused evaluations of the wearer, particularly in terms of goodness, whereas males spread their evaluations in a far less focused manner, as would be expected in the presence of a less crystallized evaluative framework. Further, women did not differentiate the shirts in terms of dominance, but men did. From the standpoint of gender role expectations, dominance is popularly perceived to be more relevant to men than to women (Rudman & Glick, 2008)--a matter that resonates into judgment and choice when the two genders evaluate consumption alternatives (Coleman, 2012). It is clear that males and females interpret brand symbolisms differently, particularly when evaluating clothing. The fact of such differences should inform future research into perceptions of brands and brand alliances. Such work may benefit by treating males and females as distinct populations.

It has long been understood that price and status are interrelated (Lichtenstein, Ridgway, & Netemeyer, 1993). It is not clear whether price signals status, or the status indicators in a garment signal higher value, which is then reflected in price. The user characteristics measured here are not direct indicators of status, but they are reflections of ways that the user would be perceived, which should be associated with status (Izard, 1959). The small but significant effects found here for engagement and dominance on price are consistent with those interrelationships. In other words, the effect of perceived user characteristics on price may be a consequence of ways that personality and price are relate to status. That should be tested in future research. Further, since the effect of personality on status is independent of the effect that appearance has on status, at least for males (Anderson, John, Keltner, & Kring, 2001), the effect of single-branded and cobranded on ratings of appearance might also be useful to incorporate.

The effects of engagement and dominance on price did not differ for males and females. That is somewhat surprising given the moderating effect of gender on mean ratings found here, and other work showing that males and females evaluate clothing differently (Elliott, 1994; Kamineni, 2005; Mayer & Belk, 1985). The failure to find a difference in the test for linear restrictions may be an artifact of the small effect that perceived user characteristics had here. If user image dimensions are incorporated in future research, stronger measures of user image may prove useful. Although the AVE and alphas for our measures were acceptable, they were somewhat low. Stronger measures may yield more portent exploration of the causes and effects of user image.

In general, our two sportswear brands and our two fashion designer brands behaved comparably. Thus, for the most part, our findings are not brand-dependent. Nevertheless, there was a moderate but significant difference in the ways that the brand alliance was evaluated when Nike was paired with a fashion designer brand than when Adidas was paired with a fashion brand. Further the direction was different for men than for women. The size of effect in the context of other effects demonstrated here suggests that this effect is not substantial, and it was not found in the case of fashion designer brands. Nevertheless, the finding does demonstrate that brand alliances can impact different brands differently. The basis for such differences is not well understood, and warrants further study.

With the exception of work on licensed products, sport marketing research has not yet concerned itself with the ways that brands associated with sport, but that are not team or league brands, ripple through the economy. As the example of emerging alliances between sportswear brands and fashion designer brands demonstrates, sport associations continue to penetrate the economy beyond sport competitions and licensed products. The effects of such alliances are neither straightforward nor well understood. By exploring the ways that sport-derived associations ramify, we can better understand sport's impact on markets and consumer behavior.

References

Aaker, D. (1996). Measuring brand equity across products and markets. California Management Review, 38(3), 102-120.

Aaker, J. L. (1997). Dimensions of brand personality. Journal of Marketing Research, 24, 347-356.

Abbott, M., Shackleton, J., & Holland, R. (2008). Measuring the brand category through semantic differentiation. Journal of Product and Brand Management, 17, 223-234.

Agarwal, M., & Rao, V. (1996). An empirical comparison of consumer-based measures of brand equity. Marketing Letters, 7, 237-247.

Ahn, S., Kim, K. J., & Forney, J. C. (2010). Fashion collaboration or collision? Examining the match-up effect in co-marketing alliances. Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management, 14, 6-20.

Ailawadi, K. L., Lehmann, D. R., & Neslin, S.A. (2003). Revenue premium as an outcome measure of brand equity. Journal of Marketing, 67(4), 117.

Anderson, C., John, O. P., Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (2001). Who attains social status? Effects of personality and physical attractiveness in social groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 116-132.

Aquirre-Rodriguez, A., Bosnjak, M., & Sirgy, M. J. (2012). Moderators of the self-congruity effect on consumer decision-making: A meta-analysis. Journal of Business Research, 65, 1179-1188.

Azevedo, A., & Farhangmehr, M. (2005). Clothing branding strategies: Influence of brand personality on advertising response. Journal of Textile and Apparel, Technology and Management, 4(3), 1-13.

Azoulay, A., & Kapferer, J. N. (2003). Do brand personality scales really measure brand personality? Journal of Brand Management, 11, 143-155.

Bae, S., & Miller, J. (2009). Consumer decision-making styles for sport apparel: Gender comparisons between college consumers. International Council for Health, Physical Education, Recreation, Sport, and Dance, 4(1), 40-45.

Belk, R. (1978). Assessing the effects of visible consumption on impression formation. Advances in Consumer Research, 5, 39-47.

Belk, R., Mayer, R., & Driscoll, A. (1984). Children's recognition of consumption symbolism in children's products. Journal of Consumer Research, 10, 386-397.

Blackett, T., & Boad, B. (1999). Co-branding: The science of alliance. New York, NY: St. Martin's Press.

Blackston, M. (1995). The qualitative dimension of brand equity. Journal of Advertising Research, 35(4), RC2-RC7.

Blackston, M. (2000). Observations: Building brand equity by managing the brand's relationships. Journal of Advertising Research, 40(6), 101-105.

Blumer, H. (1969). Symbolic interactionism: Perspective and method. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Braun, O. L., & Wicklund, R. A. (1989). Psychological antecedents of conspicuous consumption. Journal of Economic Psychology, 10, 161-187.

Brewer, W. F. (2004). Charles Osgood: The psychology of language. In L. Hoddeson (Ed.), No boundaries: University of Illinois vignettes (pp. 210-225). Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.

Campbell, C. (1997). Shopping, pleasure and the sex war. In P. Falk & C. Campbell (Eds.), The shopping experience (pp. 166-176). London, UK: Sage.

Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., & Guido, G. (2001). Brand personality: How to make the metaphor fit? Journal of Economic Psychology, 22, 377-395.

Chandler, D. (2002). Semiotics: The basics. London, UK: Routledge.

Chang, W. L. (2008). A value-based pricing system for strategic co-branding goods. Kybernetes, 37, 978-996.

Cheng, A. (2010, May 14). Adidas tones up Reebok for growth. Market Watch. Retrieved from http://articles.marketwatch.com/2010-0514/ industries/30752868_1_reebok-adidas-group-namesake-brand

Coleman, C. A. (2012). Construction of consumer vulnerability by gender and ethics of empowerment. In C. C. Otnes & L. T. Zayer (Eds.), Gender, culture, and consumer behavior (pp. 3-32). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.

Collins, R. C. (1990). Stratification, emotional energy, and the transient emotions. In T.D. Kemper (Ed.), Research agendas in the sociology of emotions (pp. 27-57). Albany, NY: The State University of New York Press.

Elliott, R. (1994). Exploring the symbolic meaning of brands. British Journal of Management, 5, S13-S19.

Elliott, R. (1997). Existential consumption and irrational desire. European Journal of Marketing, 31, 285-296.

Elliott, R., & Wattanasuwan, K. (1998). Brand as symbolic resources for the construction of identity. International Journal of Advertising, 17, 131-144.

Evans, F. B. (1968). Automobiles and self imagery: Comment. Journal of Business, 41, 484-485.

Fennis, B. M., & Pruyn, A. T. H. (2007). You are what you wear: The impact of brand personality on consumer impression formation. Journal of Business Research, 60, 634-639.

Fennis, B. M., Pruyn, A. T. H., & Maasland, M. (2005). Revisiting the malleable self: Brand effects on consumer self-perceptions of personality traits. Advances in Consumer Research, 32, 371-377.

Forsythe, S. (1991). Effect of private, designer and national brand name on shoppers' perception of apparel quality and price. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 9(2), 1-6.

Geylani, T., Inman, J. J., & Ter Hofstede, F. (2008). Image reinforcement or impairment: The effects of co-branding on attribute uncertainty. Marketing Science, 27, 730-744.

Goodstein, R. C. (1993). Category-based applications and extensions in advertising: Motivating more extensive ad processing. Journal of Consumer Research, 20, 87-99.

Hannover, B., & Kiihnen, U. (2002). "The clothing makes the self via knowledge activation. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 25132525.

Heise, D. R. (1970). The semantic differential and attitude research. In G. F. Summers (Ed.), Attitude measurement (pp. 235-253). Chicago, IL: Rand McNally.

Heise, D. R. (1989). Effects of emotion displays on social identification. Social Psychology Quarterly, 52, 10-21.

Hirschman, E. C. (1981). Comprehending symbolic consumption: Three theoretical issues. In E. C. Hirschman & M. B. Holbrook (Eds.), Symbolic consumer behavior (pp. 4-6). Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research.

Holman, R. H. (1980). Clothing as a communication: An empirical investigation. Advances in Consumer Research, 7, 372-377.

Holmes, S., & Tierney, C. (2002, November 4). How Nike got its name back. Business Week, 129-131.

Izard, C. E. (1959). Personality correlates of sociometric status. Journal of Applied Psychology, 43(2), 89-93.

Jubas, K. (2011). Shopping for identity: Articulations of gender, race and class by critical consumers. Social Identities, 17, 319-333.

Kaiser, S. B., Nagasawa, R. H., & Hutton, S. S. (1991). Fashion, post-modernity and personal appearance: A symbolic interactionist formulation. Symbolic Interaction, 14, 165-185.

Kamineni, R. (2005). Influence of materialism, gender and nationality on consumer brand perceptions. Journal of Targeting, Measurement and Analysis for Marketing, 14, 25-32.

Katos, A., Lawler, K. A., & Seddighi, H. (2000). Econometrics: A practical approach. London, UK: Routledge.

Keller, K. L. (1998). Strategic brand management: Building, measuring, and managing brand equity. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Kemper, T. D., & Collins, R. (1990). Dimensions of microinteraction. American Journal of Sociology, 96, 32-68.

Kim, E. Y., & Kim, Y. K. (2004). Predicting online purchase intentions for clothing products. European Journal of Marketing, 38, 883-897.

Kinch, J. W. (1967). A formalized theory of the self-concept. In J. G. Manis & B. N. Meltzer (Eds.), Symbolic interaction: A reader in social psychology (pp. 232-240). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Kroska, A. (2002). Does gender ideology matter? Examining the relationship between gender ideology and self- and partner-meanings. Social Psychology Quarterly, 65, 248-265.

Langford, T., & MacKinnon, N. (2000). The affective bases for the gendering of traits: Comparing the United States and Canada. Social Psychology Quarterly, 63, 34-48.

Laroche, M., Saad, G., Cleveland, M., & Browne, E. (2000). Gender differences in information search strategies for a Christmas gift. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 17, 500-522.

Lau, V., & Axelrod, N. (2009, November 24). Who wears the clothes? Balancing brand image and customer reality. Women's Wear Daily, p. 1.

Leigh, J. H., & Gabel, T. G. (1992). Symbolic interactionism: Its effects on consumer behavior and implications for marketing strategy. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 9, 27-38.

Levin, A. M., Davis, J. C., & Levin, I. P. (1996). Theoretical and empirical linkages between consumers' responses to different branding strategies. Advances in Consumer Research, 23, 296-300.

Levy, S. J. (1959). Symbols for Sale. Harvard Business Review, 37(4), 117-124.

Lichtenstein, D. R., Ridgway, N. M., & Netemeyer, R. G. (1993). Price perceptions and consumer shopping behavior: a field study. Journal of Marketing Research, 30, 232-245

Liu, F., Li, J., Mizerski, D., & Soh, H. (2012). Self-congruity, brand attitude, and brand loyalty: A study on luxury brands. European Journal of Marketing, 46, 922-937.

MacKinnon, N. J., & Langford, T. (1994). The meaning of occupational prestige scores: A social psychological analysis and interpretation. The Sociological Quarterly, 35, 215-245.

Maxwell, S. (1999). Biased attributions of a price increase: Effects of culture and gender. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 16, 9-23.

Mayer, R., & Belk, R. (1985). Fashion and impression formation among children. In M. Solomon (Ed.), The psychology of fashion (pp. 293-307). Lexington: MA, Lexington Books.

McCarthy, M., & Norris, D. (1999). Improving competitive position using branded ingredients. Journal of Product and Brand Management, 8, 267-283.

McCracken, G. (1988). Culture and consumption: New approaches to the symbolic character of consumer goods and activities. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

McCracken, G. (1993). The value of the brand: An anthropological perspective. In D. Aaker & A. Biel (Eds.), Brand Equity and Advertising (pp. 125-142). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

McCracken, G., & Roth, V. (1989). Does clothing have a code? Empirical findings and theoretical implications in the study of clothing as a means of communication. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 6, 13-33.

Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Meek, A. (1997). An estimate of the size and supported economic activity of the sports industry in the United States. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 6, 15-21.

Mehrabian, A. (1980). Basic dimensions for a general psychological theory: Implications for personality, social, environmental and developmental studies. Cambridge, MA: Oelgeschlager, Gunn & Hain.

Milano, M., & Chelladurai, P. (2011). Gross domestic sport product: The size of the sport industry in the United States. Journal of Sport Management, 25, 24-35.

Mitchell, R. (2005, May 9). Is fashion design a team sport? Brand Channel. Retrieved from http://www.brandchannel.com/features_effect.asp?pf_id=262

Nueno, J. L., & Quelch, J. A. (1998). The mass marketing of luxury. Business Horizons, 41(6), 61-68.

O'Cass, A. (2000). An assessment of consumers product, purchase decision, advertising and consumption involvement in fashion clothing. Journal of Economic Psychology, 21,545-576.

Osgood, C. E., May, W. H., & Miron, M. S. (1975). Cross-cultural universals of affective meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.

Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.

Parker, B. (2009). A comparison of brand personality and brand user-imagery congruence. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 26, 175-184.

Peluchette, J., & Karl, K. (2007). The impact of workplace attire on employee self perceptions. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 18, 354360.

Persson, N. (2010). An exploratory investigation of the elements of B2B brand image and its relationship to price premium. Industrial Marketing Management, 39, 1269-1277.

Rose, A. M. (Ed.). (1962). Human behavior and social processes: An interactionist approach. Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin.

Robertson, T., & Kassarjian, H. H. (1991). Handbook of consumer behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (2008). The social psychology of gender: How power and intimacy shape gender relations. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Schewe, C. D. (1973). Selected social psychological models for analyzing buyers. Journal of Marketing, 37, 31-39.

Schneider. A., & Schroder, T. (2012). Ideal types of leadership as patterns of affective meaning: A cross-cultural and over-time perspective. Social Psychology Quarterly, 75, 268-287.

Seltman, H. J. (2012). Experimental design and analysis. Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie-Mellon University.

Sirgy, M. J. (1982). Self-concept in consumer behavior: A critical review. Journal of Consumer Research, 9, 287-300.

Sirgy, M. J. (1983). Social cognition and consumer behavior. New York, NY: Praeger.

Sirgy, M. J. (1986). Self-congruity: Toward a new theory of personality and cybernetics. New York, NY: Praeger.

Sirgy, M. J. (2012). The psychology of quality of lifehedonic well-being, life satisfaction, and eudaimonia. Dordrecht, NL: Springer.

Simonin, B. L., & Ruth, J. A. (1995). Bundling as a strategy for new product introduction: Effects on consumers' reservation prices for the bundle, the new product, and its tie-in. Journal of Business Research, 33, 219230.

Simonin, B. L., & Ruth, J. A. (1998). Is a company known by the company it keeps? Assessing the spillover effects of brand alliances on consumer brand attitudes. Journal of Marketing Research, 35, 30-42.

Snider, J. G., & Osgood, C. E. (Eds.). (1969). Semantic differential technique: A sourcebook. Chicago, IL: Aldine.

Solomon, M. R. (1983). The role of products as social stimuli: A symbolic interactionism perspective. Journal of Consumer Research, 10, 319-329.

Sproles, G. B. (1981). The role of aesthetics in fashion-oriented consumer behavior. In G.B. Sproles (Ed.), Perspectives of fashion (pp. 120-127). Minneapolis, MN: Burgess.

Stein, N. L., & Trabasso, T. (1982). What's in a story? An approach to comprehension. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of instruction (Vol. 2, pp. 213-268). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Stith, M. T., & Goldsmith, R. E. (2006). Race, sex, and fashion innovativeness: A replication. Psychology & Marketing, 6, 249-62.

Swartz, T.A. (1983). Brand symbols and message differentiation. Journal of Advertising Research, 23(5), 59-64.

Thomas, J. (2010, January 20). Armani and Reebok to roll out co-branded clothing. Marketing Magazine. Retrieved from http://www.marketingmagazine.co.uk/news/978635/Armani-Reebok-roll-co-branded-clothing/

Valentine, V., & Evans, M. (1993). The dark side of the onion: Rethinking "rational" and "emotional" responses. Journal of the Market Research Society, 35, 125-143.

Venkatesh, R., & Mahajan, V. (1997). Products with branded components: An approach for premium pricing and partner selection. Marketing Science, 16, 146-165.

Venkatesh, R., & Mahajan, V. (2009). Design and pricing of product bundles: A review of normative guidelines and practical approaches. In V.

R. Rao (Ed.), Handbook of pricing research in marketing (pp. 232-257). Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar.

Vercillo, K. (2009, December). Top ten American fashion designers. Hub Pages. Retrieved from http://kathrynvercillo.hubpages.com/hub/Top10-American-Fashion-Designers

Washburn, J., Till, B., & Priluck, R. (2000). Co-branding: Brand equity and trial effects. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 17, 591-604.

Westerink, J., Krans, M., & Ouwerkerk, M. (Eds.). (2011). Sensing emotions in context: The impact of context on behavioral and physiological experience measurements (Vol. 12). Berlin, DE: Springer.

Wicklund, R. A., & Gollwizter, P. M. (1982). Symbolic self-completion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Workman, J. E., & Studak, C. M. (2006). Fashion consumers and fashion problem recognition style. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 30, 75-84.

Worth, L. T., Smith, J., & Mackie, D. M. (1992). Gender schematicity and preference for gender-typed products. Psychology & Marketing, 9, 17-30.

Wright, N. D., Claiborne, C. B., & Sirgy, M. J. (1992). The effects of product symbolism on consumer self-concept. Advances in Consumer Research, 19, 311-318.

D. Gloria Wu, PhD candidate, is a teaching assistant in sport management in the Department of Kinesiology and Health Education at the University of Texas at Austin. Her research interests include sport branding and sport event design.

Laurence Chalip, PhD, is a professor and chair of the Department of Recreation, Sport and Tourism at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. His research focuses on sport policy.
Table 1

Measurement Model for User Image Scale

                               Standardized
                              Factor Loadings

Goodness                        Alpha = .74
  Deceitful--Trustworthy           0.54
  Unreliable--Dependable           0.73
  Disreputable--Respectable        0.74
Engagement                      Alpha = .70
  Laid back--Assertive             0.58
  Playful--Serious                 0.57
  Easy-going--Competent            0.67
Dominance                       Alpha = .71
  Weak--Powerful                   0.79
  Submissive--Influential          0.82
  Apathetic--Ambitious             0.51

N = 275.

Table 2

Correlations Among Dependent Variables

             Goodness   Engagement   Dominance   Price
Goodness        1
Engagement   0.282 **       1
Dominance    0.412 **    0.227 **        1
Price         0.052      0.186 **    0.212 **      1

** p < .01

Table 3

Planned Contrasts for Male Participants on User Image and
Expected Price

                 Research   Value of    SE      101)      Cohen's t
                 Question   Contrast

Goodness            1         1.69     2.05     0.68        0.26
                    2         1.73     0.54   10.34 **      1.06
                    3        -1.41     0.73     3.73        0.43
                    4         0.32     0.85     0.14        0.10
                    5        -3.14     0.96   10.75 ***     0.96
                    6        -0.98     0.50    3.92 *       0.60
                    7        -0.68     0.50     1.90        0.42
Engagement          1         5.75     2.29    6.32 *       0.74
                    2         2.15     0.60   12.89 ***     1.11
                    3        -1.31     0.82     2.59        0.34
                    4         0.84     0.95     0.78        0.22
                    5        -3.46     1.07   10.52 **      0.89
                    6        -0.10     0.55     0.03        0.05
                    7        -1.24     0.55    5.04 *       0.64
Dominance           1         3.10     2.02     2.36        0.45
                    2         1.37     0.53    6.71 *       0.79
                    3        -1.70     0.72    5.60 *       0.49
                    4        -0.33     0.84     0.16        0.10
                    5        -3.07     0.94    10.63 *      0.89
                    6         0.04     0.49     0.01        0.02
                    7        -1.13     0.49    5.41 *       0.65
Expected Price      1        24.34     4.13   34.68 ***     1.78
                    2         3.87     1.08   12.74 ***     1.13
                    3         1.87     1.48     1.61        0.27
                    4         5.74     1.72   11.11 ***     0.84
                    5        -2.00     1.93     1.07        0.29
                    6         0.52     1.00     0.27        0.15
                    7        -0.75     1.00     0.57        0.22

* p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001

Table 4

Planned Contrasts for Female Participants on User Image and
Expected Price

                 Research   Value of    SE      156)      Cohen's t
                 Question   Contrast

Goodness            1        -2.76     1.43     3.74        0.48
                    2         0.62     0.34     3.20        0.43
                    3        -1.78     0.48   13.99 ***     0.62
                    4        -1.17     0.59    3.98 *       0.35
                    5        -2.40     0.59   16.48 ***     0.41
                    6         0.23     0.33     0.48        0.16
                    7         0.95     0.33    8.28 **      0.66
Engagement          1         2.69     1.86     2.10        0.37
                    2         1.18     0.45    6.97 **      0.66
                    3        -1.00     0.62     2.61        0.28
                    4         0.18     0.76     0.06        0.05
                    5        -2.19     0.77    8.08 **      0.61
                    6        -0.48     0.43     1.27        0.27
                    7        -0.01     0.43    < .001       0.01
Dominance           1         1.71     1.70     1.00        0.26
                    2        -0.22     0.41     0.28        0.12
                    3        -0.40     0.57     0.49        0.13
                    4        -0.62     0.70     0.78        0.19
                    5        -0.18     0.71     0.06        0.05
                    6        -0.52     0.39     1.74        0.31
                    7         0.98     0.39    6.25 *       0.59
Expected Price      1        20.86     3.12   44.61 ***     1.68
                    2         2.55     0.76   11.37 ***     0.82
                    3        -0.52     1.05     0.25        0.08
                    4         2.02     1.28     2.49        0.33
                    5        -3.07     1.30    5.61 *       0.49
                    6        -0.61     0.72     0.70        0.20
                    7         0.38     0.72     0.28        0.13

* p < .05; ** p< .01; *** p < .001

Table 5

Regression Results

           Dimension    [R.sup.2]   F(3, 271)   B

Expected                .07         6.821 **
  Price
           Goodness                             -.182
           Engagement                           .314
           Dominance                            .446

           SE     Beta    t(271)

Expected
  Price
           .150   -.080   -1.212
           .120   .161    2.623 **
           .139   .209    3.218 ***

** p < .01; *** p <.001
联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有