New views on old hands: the context of stencils in El Castillo and La Garma caves (Cantabria, Spain).
Pettitt, Paul ; Castillejo, Alfredo Maximiano ; Arias, Pablo 等
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
Introduction
Hand stencils form one of the most recognisable categories of
European Palaeolithic 'cave art. Since their initial study by
Cartailhac (1906-1909), Capitan (1911) and Breuil (Alcalde del Rio et
al. 1911; Breuil 1952), research into their nature and meaning,
particularly over the last half century, has essentially repeated
questions as to what the age and gender of the stencilled individuals
were and why some fingers or parts of them appear missing. We take here
a contextual approach to the production and location of hand stencils in
the caves of El Castillo and La Garma in Cantabria (henceforth
'stencils'--which are far more numerous than positive prints).
These caves are in relatively close proximity and contain a relatively
large number of stencils. Our rationale is to look past questions about
the possible identity of those stencilled and reformulate our approach
around their context in the caves. What follows is the first detailed
examination of the physical context of stencils. Our resulting
interpretations are preliminary, but reveal the potential of a new
concept--palpation--for understanding cave art more widely.
Hand stencils in Palaeolithic cave art
In many societies the hand serves as a cultural icon. Although
modern Homo sapiens societies tend to prioritise spoken language above
hand-based communication, enough use is made of hand communication by
small-scale societies to suggest that this has a considerable antiquity,
and thus that it may have played an important role for Palaeolithic
hominins capable of creating and understanding symbolic systems (e.g.
Capitan 1911; Leroi-Gourhan 1967). Examples of hand stencils (outlines
created by projecting fluid pigment over a hand placed against a cave
surface) and prints (made by pressing a pigment-covered hand against a
surface) are known in a variety of countries and areas, notably
Argentina, Australia, Borneo, Mexico, Peru, the Sahara and the USA
(Arizona, California and particularly Texas), all apparently of Late
Pleistocene age or younger. European Upper Palaeolithic examples--the
focus of this study--are known in France, Spain and Italy. No precise
quantification of the number of stencils/prints and the number of caves
in which they occur is known to us: we count them in 44 caves, of which
30 are in France, 13 in Spain and 1 in Italy (for details and references
see online supplementary material). Among these the greater majority are
in southern France (Dordogne, Lot, Ariege) and northern Spain
(Cantabria), although isolated examples are known as far north as the
Grande Grotte at Arcy-sur-Cure, Burgundy (Baffler & Girard 2007) and
as far south as Ardales near Malaga (Espejo Herrerias & Cantalejo
Duarte 2006).
Breuil (1952: 38) assigned prints and stencils to his
Aurignacian-Perigordian cycle on stratigraphic grounds and their lack of
association with anything other than dots or simple animal outlines, and
although Leroi-Gourhan occasionally assigned them to a later phase (e.g.
his Style III/Solutrean-Early Magdalenian for Tibiran; Leroi-Gourhan
1968: 321) most subsequent researchers, basing their conclusions on
radiocarbon measurements and U-series measurements on stalactites
overlying stencils support the view that they are Gravettian or earlier
(e.g. Lorblanchet 1995: 245-46; Ripoll Lopez et al. 1999a: 13; Foucher
et al. 2007: 83; Pike et al. 2012.). For the purposes of this paper,
however, the only assumption one needs to make is that the stencils in
each cave are broadly contemporary and thus can be interpreted as a
thematic whole.
In only a few cases have stencils been meticulously studied and
published, notably at Gargas (Sahly 1966; Barriere & Sueres 1993;
Foucher & Rumeau 2007), Cosquer (Clottes et al. 1992; Clottes &
Courtin 1996: 69-79) and Maltravieso (Ripoll Lopez et al. 1999a &
b). Wider research has concentrated entirely on the identity of hand
stencils rather than their context, i.e. the possible gender and age of
the people whose hands were depicted, whether the left or right hand was
shown (e.g. Faurie & Raymond 2004), and why in some caves fingers or
parts of them appear attenuated, i.e. missing or bent back (in the last
half century, for example, Janssens 1957; Sahly 1966; Hooper 1980;
Wildgoose et al. 1982; Barriere & Sueres 1993; Ripoll Lopez et al.
1999a; Guthrie 2005: 114-32; Rouillon 2006). Most researchers agree that
the left hand was overwhelmingly stencilled; that the ratio of lengths
of ring and index fingers--which indicate gender from the age of two
(Manning et al. 1998)--is often consistent with female hands, and that
there is no reason to assume more than a small number of individuals are
represented in each cave. Attenuated (usually described as
'missing') fingers in fact belong to stencils in only a small
number of the caves, and the focus on these presumably resulted simply
from their abundance in Gargas and neighbouring Tibiran. Given the
relative rarity of attenuated fingers (a term we favour due to its
neutrality) among this relatively rare category of parietal art one
really should not be preoccupied with them.
New research in La Garma and El Castillo
We have undertaken an informed analysis of the context and
positioning of stencils in La Garma and El Castillo, taking a number of
observations and measurements of each visible stencil. By focusing on
context we hope that we will widen our understanding of stencils and by
so doing widen the appreciation of 'cave art' through
exploring objectively processes of individual decision-making made in a
very specific and constrained environment. Given that the stencils
appear to be relatively early in the chronology of European Palaeolithic
art we hope that this new perspective will contribute to current debates
as to the origins and development of cognitively 'modern'
behaviour and art in general.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
El Castillo, in the village of Puente Yiesgo, and La Garma, just
north of the village of Omono, lie around 29km apart, to the south-west
and east of Santander respectively (Figure 1). The c. 300m-long cave of
El Castillo can be divided into two areas; a large entrance chamber (the
Gran Sala), and a subsequent labyrinth of narrow galleries totalling
almost 1km in length. Much of its art is figurative and can be
attributed to the Solutrean or the Magdalenian, although its hand
stencils belong to an earlier group which includes dots and lines, the
antiquity of which was recently attested by the U-series dating noted
above (Pike et al. 2012). Most of the stencils--at least 44 of which 38
are clear--cluster in a panel in a narrow gallery beyond the Gran Sala
(the Gallery of the Hands) although isolated examples exist further into
the cave's depth (Alcalde del Rio et al. 1911; Leroi-Gourhan 1968:
333-34; Figure 2) and as many surviving examples are faint one cannot
rule out that their absence from the Gran Sala is due to their
disappearance due to atmospheric erosion (Alcalde del Rio et al. 1911:
117). The c. 300m-long Lower Gallery of La Garma cave, 12km from
Santander, can be divided into nine zones on the basis of the
cave's topography. Contrasting patterns of spatial distribution
distinguish the early (Pre-Magdalenian) and late (Middle Magdalenian)
stages of Palaeolithic activity so far identified in the cave (Gonzalez
Sainz 2003; Ontanon 2003; Arias et al. 2011). The former stage
(including the stencils, series of red dots and other simple paintings,
as well as animal representations also in red) occurs along the entire
gallery; the later stage (comprising the remains of habitation
structures and numerous paintings and engravings of Magdalenian style)
is concentrated in the areas that are close to the cave's original
entrance (zones I-IV, spanning roughly one third of the gallery's
length). Recent pigment analyses of a number of examples of the
cave's art has reinforced this division, demonstrating that the
hand stencils are linked to non-figurative dots and lines rather than to
the more diverse figurative images of the Magdalenian (Arias et al.
2011).
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
We examined as many hand stencils as were clearly visible in La
Garma (n = 27) and El Castillo (n = 38) (Figure 3), taking a series of
observations on each, notably: colour (in both caves always red); left
or right hand (assuming a palm down position); the length (mm) of each
observable digit; the width of palm (ram); the orientation of the hand
(expressed as a point in a 360[degrees] circle); the angle between the
thumb and the index finger; and the height above the cave floor. In
addition to these we recorded contextual information about the location
and associations of each hand stencil, specifically: the type of surface
(e.g. flat, smooth, modified, bright, dull); the presence/absence of
specific features (notably stalactite, concave or convex surface,
cracks) and relation of these to the stencil; any artistic association
(i.e. whether other stencils or other art is located close by); the
wider positioning of the stencils (i.e. chamber type, proximity to
shafts, stalactite columns and other features); and the specific
position of stencils in relation to localised cave wall morphology, e.g.
convex or concave surfaces, cracks and the like. In this paper we
concentrate on these contextual associations.
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
Stencils: context and associations
Through the experimental replication of stencils we created a coded
scheme reflecting the ease or difficulty of producing stencils. This
confirmed that the most comfortable position in which to produce
stencils was at 45[degrees] to the left (of vertical) when producing a
stencil of the right hand, and 45[degrees] to the right (of vertical)
when producing a stencil of the left hand. Table 1 compares coded
ease/difficulty with the orientation of stencils in La Garma and El
Castillo. Although most stencils in these caves were created within the
most practical and comfortable range of 45[degrees] (left) to
135[degrees] (right), a small number of exceptions were identified (one
in La Garma and five in El Castillo), which were created in
uncomfortable and impractical conditions even though more comfortable
and practical locations were available in their immediate vicinity (e.g.
flat surfaces around head height). In four of these cases the stencils
are associated with features of the cave walls, to which we return
below. Choices about the placement of stencils were not simply about
practicality, therefore; some were created at chosen locations
irrespective of whether they were comfortable or practical or neither.
Some examples are extreme: Hand 28 at the far right of the Main Panel of
Hands in El Castillo--the only example of an uncomfortable or
impractical stencil without any obvious association to a feature of the
cave wall--was created at a low height on a steeply sloping flowstone
surface close to where it meets the cave wall. This is a remarkably
uncomfortable and impractical position to assume, strongly suggesting
that a second individual created the stencil, i.e. that stenciller and
stencilled were separate individuals (Figure 4).
Little attention has previously been paid to the context of
stencils. What information has been published is typically informal,
with formal associations generally restricted to those with other
examples of cave art, which may or may not be contemporary.
Leroi-Gourhan (1968: 148), for example, noted an association between
hand stencils and rows or clusters of dots and parallel strokes in
Gargas, Les Merveilles (Rocamadour), Pech Merle, El Castillo and
Bernifal, and their proximity to or location in the midst of
'central compositions' in Bayol, El Castillo, Pech Merle and
Bernifal. He interpreted them in terms of his gendered symbolism,
although he was probably grouping art of different ages into this
holistic view and thus they are not meaningful associations. Otherwise,
published associations with the topography of cave surfaces are few.
Lewis-Williams (2002: 219) noted a 'bulging' of the rock face
between two hand stencils of the dappled horses panel in Pech Merle, and
in the Grotte Cosquer eight stencils cluster on a stalagmite drapery and
another was placed in a natural niche (Clottes et al. 1992: figs. 6 and
7, respectively). Most importantly Gonzalez Garcia (1987) noted that the
stencils of El Castillo were created on concave surfaces in his wider
analysis of the context of the cave's art. It is obvious from
published photographs that stencils could be placed in or above niches
(e.g. in Gargas (Foucher et al. 2007: 41, fig. 37; Foucher & Rumeau
2007: figs. 49, 52, 67 & 68) and Cosquer (Clottes & Courtin
1996: 71, fig. 35)), in proximity to cracks (e.g. in Gargas (Bourges
2007: fig. 31)) and in Cosquer (Clottes & Courtin 1996: figs. 33
& 171)), atop stalactite draperies (e.g. in Cosquer (Clottes &
Courtin 1996: figs. 31 & 38)) and in apparent association with
cracks (Les Fieux (Lorblanchet 2010: 325)), but no systematic study has
to our knowledge been undertaken of stencils per se. Table 2 presents
hand stencils in El Castillo and La Garma that are found in obvious
association with natural features, notably fissures, convex bosses and
concave depressions, observed examples of which are defined in the
table. In total 80% of observable stencils at La Garma and 74% at El
Castillo have some kind of association, either with fissures or
undulations on the caves' surfaces. As areas of 'smooth'
rock were easily accessible in each cave and within close proximity to
stencils, such associations cannot be entirely fortuitous. Some stencils
seem to have been 'fitted' to subtle topographic features in
the wall, and some were positioned on bosses in the wall in such a
manner that they appear to be 'gripping' the wall in a similar
way that explorers use their hands to steady themselves when navigating
the caves (Figure 5).
[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]
From Table 2 it can be seen that a concern with
'gripping' convex rock surfaces is observable on around 27% of
stencils in La Garma and around 23% in El Castillo. The palms of a
smaller number of stencils--17% and 5% respectively--were
'economically' fitted to slight concavities on the cave
surfaces. Even using modern lighting these can only be seen very close
up, and under the lighting conditions in which they would have been
created, such fitting would have necessarily involved touch as well as
very close-up scrutiny of the surface. Overall there seems to have been
more of a concern with fitting stencils to surface morphology in La
Garma (44% have either gripping or ergonomic characters) than El
Castillo (28%). By contrast, an association with natural cracks or
fissures typically accounts for 39% of stencils in La Garma and 60% in
El Castillo. Details differ between the two: in La Garma the concern is
more with placing the stencil to the right of fissures (15%) or in
between two fissures (9%), whereas in contrast a concern with placing
stencils directly on top of fissures (29%) or with two stencils spanning
fissures (11%) is clear at El Castillo. It is of interest that the few
examples of stencils placed in uncomfortable and impractical positions
(with coded scores of 4 or 5 in each category) possess clear
associations: in El Castillo these are Hand 7 (placed atop a
stalactite), Hand 13 (associated with a crack), Hand 35 (placed in a
chimney accessed only up a steep slope) and Hand 36 (ergonomically
fitted to a palmar concavity, with fingers gripping a boss).
[FIGURE 5 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 6 OMITTED]
Specific examples illustrate our observations. Six stencils
comprise El Castillo Area 1, among which one was created from a
comfortable position on a relatively flat surface atop several fissures,
despite the 'availability' of unfissured rock nearby (Figure
6). In the Main Panel of Hands (our Area 3) one stencil was created over
a small concave depression, and thus was ergonomically placed (Figure
7). An isolated hand in the Gallery of Dots was similarly ergonomically
fitted to an otherwise smooth surface, the palm placed over a slight
concave depression and the fingers 'gripping' a boss in the
wall next to a natural fissure (our 'ergonomic,
'gripping' and 'right of fissure' positions) (Figure
8). In the Main Panel two stencils of a left hand (probably the same
hand) were created in very close proximity to each other to either side
of a natural crack (Figure 9). A more obvious pairing, again
'spanning' a crack, was created by left and right hands placed
on an otherwise smooth surface (Figure 10). We can be reasonably sure
that the present floor level approximates fairly closely the level of
the floor at the time these were created: the lack of excavation or
modification and the survival of stalagmite flows over the floor suggest
this is so. Assuming this is correct the height of these
stencils--directly above the head of the stenciller/stencilled--would
have rendered them difficult to create.
[FIGURE 7 OMITTED]
By comparing these associations with the specific morphologies of
the relevant hand stencils we hoped to establish a pattern of individual
choice in the placing of the stencils. In most cases stencils were too
faint to establish finger lengths and other measurements confidently.
Where stencils are clear, finger terminations may still be vague and
thus decisions about where to begin and end measurements probably
involve an error in the order of a few millimetres; thus we are cautious
not to over-interpret these results and accept that the little data we
do have may underestimate the actual number of individuals who left
stencils of their hands in the caves. Table 3 documents finger and palm
measurements for Stencils with the most common associations in the two
caves. At La Garma, the morphology of stencils in the gripping position
seems to reflect four separate individuals; the index/ring finger ratios
(sensu Snow 2006) of two are consistent with females (Hands 16 and 24)
and one with a male (Hand 23). At least two individuals placed stencils
to the right of fissures based on the length of their middle fingers
(Hands 14 and 16, the small length of the former possibly indicative of
a juvenile). Hand 16 was placed with the fingers in the gripping
position, the palm ergonomically fitted and to the right of a fissure.
At least two individuals placed stencils in positions bordered by
fissures (Hands 15 and 23 and 24 which may belong to the same individual
and are consistent with a male). At least three individuals left
stencils with no association visible today (Hands 2, 5 and 13).
In El Castillo it is possible that Hands 4 and 6 in the gripping
position were left by the same individual, although two other
individuals seems to have adopted this position too (Hands 32 and 38,
the latter of which has finger ratios consistent with a female). Little
can be said for stencils placed atop fissures except that three
individuals seem to be represented by Hands 1, 6 and 18, the finger
ratios of the latter consistent with a male. Three individuals placed
stencils spanning fissures (Hands 12, 19 and 20, the latter of which is
consistent with a female). This is of interest, as Hands 19 and 20 form
a left and right pair that otherwise one would assume to represent the
same individual, but the morphology suggests that one person placed
their left hand to the left of the fissure and a second individual
placed their right hand to the right of the fissure. Given the distinct
differences in ring, index and little finger lengths of these stencils
it is unlikely that these differences reflect measurement imprecision.
Three or four individuals left stencils with no association (Hands 15,
27 and 28, which may be the same individual, and 31) and of these the
finger ratios of Hand 28 are consistent with a female.
[FIGURE 8 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 9 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 10 OMITTED]
Given the indistinct nature of most stencils one should not make
too much of these data. It is of interest that few examples exist of the
same individual represented in different areas, but this may well be
masked by the indistinctiveness of most examples. What it does reveal,
at least, is that several individuals were making similar decisions
about the meaningful placement of stencils; it is not the work of one
individual, but represents similar decision-making among a group.
Palpation: a new understanding of hand stencils
Under the best of lighting conditions the exploration of caves is a
tactile process; hands are often placed on floors, slopes and walls to
steady the body. In the extremely low light conditions that pertained
for the stencillers they would be brought into very close proximity with
any surface they intended to mark. Hands would be placed on surfaces,
fingers and palms traced across them, and the undulations of the cave
walls and ceilings could be 'read' as much by touch as visual
inspection. We will probably never know exactly why decisions were made
to mark walls with pigments or engravings, although we have identified
here a plausible link between touch and marking. In order to fix
stencils, red pigments were presumably selected for their visual
'warmth', i.e. their visibility in low light conditions
(Groenen 1997). The use of cave topographies and shadows to place and
structure figurative art is well known (e.g. Lejeune 2004; Pigeaud 2004;
Remade 2004; Vialou 2004), and our results suggest that similar
processes were at work in the creation of stencils, irrespective of
whether the process was easy or difficult. To an extent these are visual
processes, but stencils in gripping positions and particularly with
palms fitted to very slight concavities indicate that visual stimuli
were not the only way that stencillers interacted with cave surfaces.
These reveal that a close-up scrutiny of cave surfaces must have
preceded the creation of stencils in the majority of cases in La Garma
and El Castillo. For this, we suggest the term palpation, deriving from
the surgical term for examination by touch. Not that palpation governed
only the production of hand stencils; finger dots and lines connect the
artist's hands with the cave wall, and finger flutings--that appear
to have been created irrespective of discomfort (Sharpe & Van Gelder
2006)--could be interpreted as the visible record of the act of
palpation. Furthermore, Lorblanchet (2009) has noted the gradation
between bear claw marks and finger 'rubbings' and traces in
the caves of the Quercy, which he interprets as a ritual interaction
with the cave walls. We have, so to speak, barely scratched the surface
here, but hope that we have demonstrated the heuristic potential of
examining touch and context in Palaeolithic art. Far more formal
examination of the context of parietal art is necessary, as is the
investigation of levels of illumination and its effects on visibility,
topography and placement of art. There is clearly much more to
'cave art' than meets the eye.
Supplementary material is published online at
http://antiquity.ac.uk/projgall/pettitt339/
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to the Gobierno de Cantabria for authorisation to
conduct this research and for their kind assistance. It was funded by a
Leverhulme Trust grant to PP which employed RH as Research Associate. We
are grateful to Marc Groenen for his support for the project. Paul Bahn
and Joao Zilhao kindly commented on a previous draft. Beatrice
Greenfield, Ellie Walton and the students of the Department of
Archaeology at Sheffield University gave up much of their time
reproducing hand stencils, for which PP is grateful. Luis Teira provided
the photographs.
References
ALCALDE DEL RIO, H., H. BREUIL & L. SIERRA. 1911. Les cavernes
de la region Cantabrique (Espagne). Monaco: Imprimerie A. Chene.
ARIAS, P., E. LAVAL, M. MENU, C. GONZALEZ SAINZ & R. ONTANON
PEREDO. 2011. Les colourants dans l'art parietal et mobilier
paleolithique de La Garma (Cantabrie, Espagne). L'Anthropologie
115: 425-45.
BAFFIER, D. & M. GIRARD. 2007. La Grand Grotte
d'Arcy-sur-Cure. Les Dossiers d'Archeologie 324: 74-85.
BARRIERE, C. & M. SUERES. 1993. Les mains de Gargas. Dossiers
d'Archeologie 178: 46-54.
BOURGES, F. 2007. Les peintures prehistoriques des Grottes, les
raisons d'une conservation exceptionelle, in P. Foucher, C. San
Juan-Foucher & Y. Rumeau (ed.) La Grotte de Gargas. Un siecle de
decouvertes-, 53-57. Mazeres de Neste: Communaute de Communes du Canton
de Saint-Laurant de Neste.
BREUIL, H. 1952. Four hundred centuries of cave art. Paris: Sappho.
CAPITAN, L. 1911. Les empreintes de mains sur les parois de las
Grotte de Gargas. Bulletin Anthropologique 1911: 87-88.
CARTAILHAC, E. 1906-1909. Les mains rouges et noire et les dessins
paleolithiques de la Grotte de Gargas. Bulletin de la Societe
d'Archeologie du Midi de la France 37:140-41.
CLOTTES, J. & J. COURTIN. 1996. The cave beneath the sea.
Palaeolithic images at Cosquer. New York: Harry N. Abrams.
CLOTTES, J., J. COURTIN, H. VALLADAS, H. CACHIER, N. MERCIER &
M. ARNOLD. 1992. La Grotte Cosquer datee. Bulletin de la Societe
Prehistorique Francaise 89: 230-34.
ESPEJO HERRERIAS, M. & P. CANTALEJO DUARTE. 2006. Cueva de
Ardales (Malaga). Reproduction digital del arte rupestre paleolitico.
Guadalteba: Comarca del Guadalteba.
FAURIE, C. & M. RAYMOND. 2004. Handedness frequency over more
than ten thousand years. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London
Series B 271: S43-S45.
FOUCHER, P. & Y. RUMEAU. 2007. Les galleries ornees de Gargas,
in P. Foucher, C. San Juan-Foucher & Y. Rumeau (ed.) La Grotte de
Gargas. Un siecle de decouvertes: 61-86. Mazeres de Neste: Communaute de
Communes du Canton de Saint-Laurant de Neste.
FOUCHER, P, C. SAN JUAN-FOUCHER & Y. RUMEAU (ed.). 2007. La
Grotte de Gargas. Un siecle de decouvertes. Mazeres de Neste: Communaute
de Communes du Canton de Saint-Laurant de Neste.
GONZALEZ GARCIA, R. 1987. Organization, distribution and typology
of the cave art of Monte del Castillo, Spain. Rock Art Research A:
127-36.
GONZALEZ Sainz, C. 2003. El conjuncto parietal paleolitico de la
galena inferior de La Garma (Cantabria): avance de su organizacion
interna, in R. de Balbln Behrmann & P. Bueno Ramirez (ed.) El arte
prehistorico desde los inicios del siglo XXL: Primer Symposium
Internacional de Arte Prehistorico di Ribadasella: 201-22. Ribadasella:
Association Cultural Amigos de Ribadasella.
GROENEN, M. 1997. Ombres et lumieres dans l'art des grottes.
Bruxelles: ULB.
GUTHRIE, R.D. 2005. The nature of Paleolithic art. Chicago (IL):
University of Chicago Press.
HOOPER, A. 1980. Further information on the prehistoric
representations of human hands in the cave of Gargas. Medical History
24: 214-16.
JANSSENS, P.A. 1957. Medical views on prehistoric representations
of human hands. Medical History 1: 318-22.
LEJEUNE, M. 2004. Quelques reflexions sur le role de la paroi
rocheuse dans l'art du Paleolithique Superieur, in M. Lej'eune
(ed.) L'Art parietal paleolithique dans son contexte naturel (ERAUL
107): 15-19. Liege: Universite de Liege.
LEROI-GOURHAN, A. 1967. Les mains de Gargas. Essaie pour une etude
d'ensemble. Bulletin de la Societe Prehistorique Francaise 64:
107--22.
--1968. The art of prehistoric man in Western Europe. London:
Thames & Hudson.
LEWIS-WILLIAMS, J.D. 2002. The mind in the cave: consciousness and
the origins of art. London: Thames & Hudson.
LORBLANCHET, M. 1995. Les grottes ornees de la prehistoire:
nouveaux regards. Paris: Errance.
--2009. Claw marks and ritual traces in the Paleolithic sanctuaries
of the Quercy, in P.G. Bahn (ed.) An enquiring mind: studies in honour
of Alexander Marshack: 165-70. Oxford: Oxbow.
--2010. Art parietal. Grottes ornees du Quercy. Parc-Saint-Joseph:
Rouergue.
MANNING, J.T., D. SCUTT, J. WILSON & D.I. LEWIS-JONES. 1998.
The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and
concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen. Human
Reproduction 13: 3000-3004.
ONTANON, R. 2003. Sols et structures d'habitat du
Paleolithique superieur, nouvelles donnes depuis les Cantabres: la
Gallerie Inferieure de La Garma (Cantabrie, Espagne).
L'Anthropologie 107: 333-63.
PIGEAUD, R. 2004. Dialogue avec la paroi: cas des representations
paleolithiques de la Grotte ornee Mayenne-Sciences (Thorigne-en-Charnie,
Mayenne), in M. Lejeune (ed.) L'Art parietal paleolithique dans son
contexte naturel (ERAUL 107): 21-43. Liege: Universite de Liege.
PIKE, A.W.G., D.L. HOFFMANN, M. GARC(A-DIEZ, P.B. PETTITT, J.
ALCOLEA, R. DE BALBIN, C. GONZALEZ SAINZ, C. DE LAS HERAS, J.A.
LASHERAS, R. MONTES & J. ZILHAO. 2012. U-Series dating of
Paleolithic art in 11 caves in Spain. Science 336: 1409-13.
REMACLE, L. 2004. De la grotte au(x) mythe(s). Phenomenes
d'interaction et lecture de l'image paleolithique, in M. Otte,
L. Oosterbeek, D. Seglie, L. Remade & V. Bertollo (ed.) Art du
Paleolithique Superieur et du Mesolithique (British Archaeological
Reports international series 1311): 7-14. Oxford: Archaeopress.
RIPOLL LOPEZ, S., E. RIPOLL PERELLO & H. COLLADO Giraldo.
1999a. Maltravieso. El santuario extremeno de las manos. Caceres: Museo
de Caceres.
RIPOLL LOPEZ, S., E. RIPOLL PERELLO, H. COLLADO GIRALDO, M. MAS
CORNELLA & J.F. JORDA PARDO. 1999b. Maltravieso. El santuario
extremeno de las manos. Trabajos de Prehistoria 56: 59-84.
ROUILLON, A. 2006. Au Gravettien, dans la Grotte Cosquer
(Marseille, Bouches-du-Rhone), l'homme a-t-il compte sur ses
doigts? L'Anthropologie 110: 500-509.
SAHLY, A. 1966. Les mains mutilees dans l'art prehistorique.
Toulouse: Privat.
SHARPE, K. & L. VAN GELDER. 2006. The study of finger flutings.
Cambridge Archaeological Journal 16: 281-95.
SNOW, D.R. 2006. Sexual dimorphism in Upper Palaeolithic hand
stencils. Antiquity 80: 390-404.
VIALOU, D. 2004. Architecture de l'art parietal paleolithique,
in M. Lejeune (ed.) L'Art parietal paleolithique dam son contexte
naturel (ERAUL 107): 7-14. Liege: Universite de Liege.
WILDGOOSE, M., E. HADINGHAM & A. HOOPER. 1982. The prehistoric
hand pictures at Gargas: attempts at simulation. Medical History 26:
205-207.
Received: 23 April 2013; Accepted: 20 June 2013: Revised: 3 July
2013
Paul Pettitt (1), Alfredo Maximiano Castillejo (2), Pablo Arias
(2), Roberto Ontanon Peredo (2,3,4) & Rebecca Harrison (5)
(1) Department of Archaeology, Durham University, South Road,
Durham DH1 3LE, UK (Author for correspondence, email:
paul.pettitt@durham.ac.uk)
(2) The Cantabria International Institute for Prehistoric Research,
University of Cantabria, Edificio Interfacultativo, Avda. los Castros
sin, 39005 Santander, Spain
(3) Museo de Prehistoria y Arqueologia de Cantabria, Avda. los
Castros 65-67, 39005 Santander, Spain
(4) Cuevas Prehistoricas de Cantabria, 39670 Puenta Viesgo, Spain
(5) Department of Archaeology, University of Sheffield, Northgate
House, West Street, Sheffield S1 4ET, UK
Table 1. Orientation practicality and comfort levels of stencils at
La Garma and El Castillo
Number (% of total)
of stencils
Orientation of La Garma El Castillo Practicality
stencil (1) (N = 21) (N = 37) Comfort (2) (3)
10[degrees] 1 (2.7%) 4 4
45-145[degrees] 20 (95%) 32 (86.5%) 1 1
150[degrees] 1 (2.7%) 2 1
170[degrees] 1 (5%) 3 2
190[degrees] 1 (2.7%) 4 3
200[degrees] 1 (2.7%) 4 4
280[degrees] 1 (2.7%) 5 5
(1) from an angle of 0[degrees] which is horizontal and
pointing towards the left (9 o'clock on the clock face)
(2) levels defined from experimental work:
1 = very comfortable; 5 = very uncomfortable
(3) levels denned from experimental work:
1 = very practical; 5 = very impractical
Table 2. The context and association of stencils at
El Castillo and La Garma with natural features
of the cave surfaces.
La Garma hand
stencils (recorded
by our stencil El Castillo hand
numbers). (N = 29, stencils (recorded
includes two thumb our stencil
Category of association stencils) numbers). (N = 38)
No observable 6 (Hands 2; 3; 5; 10 (Hands 15; 17;
associations 11; 13; 19) 21; 22; 23; 25; 27;
28; 31; 34)
Ergonomic: the stencil is 5 (Hands 1;9; 16; 2 (Hands 26; 37)
matched to undulations/ 18; 26)
curves of wall (typically
the palm is placed over
a minor concave
depression)
Gripping: the fingers of 6 plus 2 thumbs 9 (Hands 3; 4; 5;
a stencil placed over (Hands 6; 10; 16; 6; 8; 32; 35;
('grip') a convex boss 23; 24; 27; Thumb 36; 38)
or ridge 1; Thumb 2)
Below fissure: the 1 (Hand 9) 0
stencil is located below
a natural fissure/crack
when viewed upright (i.e.
fingers to top)
Bordered by fissures: the 3 (Hands 1 (Hand 24)
stencil is located 15; 23; 24)
between two natural
fissures/cracks, i.e. to
its left and right
Within multiple fissures: 2 (Hands 4; 17) 3 (Hands 9; 13; 14)
the stencil is in close
proximity to multiple
natural fissures/
cracks (>2)
Above fissure: the 1 (Hand 20) 1 (Hand 32)
stencil is located above
a natural fissure/crack
when viewed vertically
Atop fissure(s): the 1 (Hand 25) 11 (Hands 1; 2; 3;
stencil is placed 5; 6; 9; 10; 16;
directly on top of 18; 29; 33)
fissure(s)
Atop stalactite: the 0 1 (Hand 7)
stencil is placed on top
of a stalactite
(flowstone)
Spanning fissure: a pair 0 4 (Hands 11 & 12;
of stencils spans a 19 & 20)
natural fissure/crack,
i.e. one to either side
Percentage of
recorded stencils
by site (rounded
Category of association up/down)
No observable La Garma 20%
associations El Castillo 26%
Ergonomic: the stencil is La Garma 17%
matched to undulations/ El Castillo 5%
curves of wall (typically
the palm is placed over
a minor concave
depression)
Gripping: the fingers of La Garma 27%
a stencil placed over (includes
('grip') a convex boss 2 thumbs)
or ridge El Castillo 23%
Below fissure: the La Garma 3%
stencil is located below
a natural fissure/crack
when viewed upright (i.e.
fingers to top)
Bordered by fissures: the La Garma 9%
stencil is located El Castillo 3%
between two natural
fissures/cracks, i.e. to
its left and right
Within multiple fissures: La Garma 6%
the stencil is in close El Castillo 8%
proximity to multiple
natural fissures/
cracks (>2)
Above fissure: the La Garma 3%
stencil is located above El Castillo 3%
a natural fissure/crack
when viewed vertically
Atop fissure(s): the La Garma 3%
stencil is placed El Castillo 29%
directly on top of
fissure(s)
Atop stalactite: the El Castillo 3%
stencil is placed on top
of a stalactite
(flowstone)
Spanning fissure: a pair El Castillo 11%
of stencils spans a
natural fissure/crack,
i.e. one to either side
Table 3. Finger lengths, palm widths and angles between thumb and first
finger for stencils in La Garma and El Castillo, ordered by apparent
association. It will be noticed that most are 'indistinct' i.e.
too faint to record measurements with confidence, but these are
included here for completeness. Shaded measurements of index fingers
indicate hands with finger ratios consistent with (but not necessarily
indicative of) females; shaded measurements of ring fingers are male.
Thumb Index Middle Ring
(mm) (mm) (mm) (mm)
La Garma
No association
Hand 2 Indistinct 70 Indistinct 70
Hand 3 Indistinct Indistinct 73 68
Hand 5 50 78 65 77
Hand 11 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 13 Indistinct 58 >48 Indistinct
Hand 19 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Gripping
Hand 6 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 10 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 16 Indistinct 75 68 59
Hand 23 Indistinct 58 75 72
Hand 24 Indistinct 74 76 69
Hand 27 Indistinct 58 61 56
Ergonomic
Hand 1 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 9 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 16 Indistinct 75 68 59
Hand 18 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 26 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Right of fissure
Hand 8 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 12 Indistinct 71 Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 14 Indistinct Indistinct 44 Indistinct
Hand 16 Indistinct 75 68 59
Bordered by fissures
Hand 15 Indistinct Indistinct >72 74
Hand 23 Indistinct 58 75 72
Hand 24 Indistinct 60 Indistinct 65
Within multiple fissures
Hand 4 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 17 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
El Castille1
No association
Hand 15 Indistinct 65 85 66
Hand 17 Indistinct Indistinct 76 80
Hand 21 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 22 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 23 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 25 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 27 Indistinct 70 80 70
Hand 28 Indistinct 75 80 70
Hand 31 Indistinct 68 76 Indistinct
Hand 34 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Gripping
Hand 3 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 4 Indistinct 68 72 65
Hand 5 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 6 Indistinct 65 70 66
Hand 8 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 32 Indistinct Indistinct 75 66
Hand 35 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 36 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 38 Indistinct 60 65 55
Atop fissure
Hand 1 Indistinct 75 71 Indistinct
Hand 2 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 3 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 5 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 6 Indistinct 65 70 66
Hand 9 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 10 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 16 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct 60
Hand 18 Indistinct 70 80 75
Hand 29 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 33 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Spanning fissure
Hand 11 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 12 Indistinct 70 66 Indistinct
Hand 19 Indistinct 66 Indistinct 67
Hand 20 55 90 90 80
Within multiple fissures
Hand 9 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 13 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 14 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Little Angle between Palm
(mm) thumb & index width (mm)
La Garma
No association
Hand 2 60 60 95
Hand 3 Indistinct 47 81
Hand 5 69 62 82
Hand 11 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 13 51 Indistinct 94
Hand 19 Indistinct Indistinct 90
Gripping
Hand 6 Indistinct Indistinct 82
Hand 10 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 16 Indistinct Indistinct 90
Hand 23 50 45 90
Hand 24 62 45 90
Hand 27 Indistinct 35 85
Ergonomic
Hand 1 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 9 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 16 Indistinct Indistinct 90
Hand 18 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 26 52 Indistinct Indistinct
Right of fissure
Hand 8 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 12 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 14 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 16 Indistinct Indistinct 90
Bordered by fissures
Hand 15 V v-n 00 Indistinct 76
Hand 23 50 45 90
Hand 24 50 55 89
Within multiple fissures
Hand 4 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 17 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
El Castille
No association
Hand 15 60 35 94
Hand 17 70 Indistinct 84
Hand 21 Indistinct Indistinct 85
Hand 22 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 23 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 25 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 27 60 40 80
Hand 28 Indistinct 45 85
Hand 31 63 35 79
Hand 34 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Gripping
Hand 3 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 4 Indistinct 36 84
Hand 5 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 6 60 35 84
Hand 8 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 32 50 40 87
Hand 35 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 36 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 38 60 Indistinct 85
Atop fissure
Hand 1 53 25 95
Hand 2 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 3 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 5 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 6 60 35 84
Hand 9 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 10 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 16 Indistinct 35 83
Hand 18 70 30 85
Hand 29 Indistinct Indistinct 80
Hand 33 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Spanning fissure
Hand 11 Indistinct 27 Indistinct
Hand 12 Indistinct Indistinct 90
Hand 19 58 30 Indistinct
Hand 20 65 Indistinct 90
Within multiple fissures
Hand 9 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 13 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct
Hand 14 Indistinct Indistinct Indistinct