Estimating trajectories of colonisation to the Mariana Islands, western Pacific.
Fitzpatrick, Scott M. ; Callaghan, Richard T.
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
Introduction
The Marianas island chain, located in the north-western tropical
Pacific, is one of the most remote island groups in the western Pacific
Basin (Figure 1). The archipelago stretches for over 800km in a
north-south configuration between 13[degrees] and 20[degrees] N. It is
1800km from New Guinea and the Bismarcks to the south, and up to 2300km
away from the northern Philippines and Taiwan to the west. Current
archaeological research suggests that the Marianas island chain was
settled by at least 3200 cal BP, and possibly as early as 3500 cal BP
(see Carson 2008, 2011; Clark et al. 2010; Hung et al. 2011; Carson
& Kurashina 2012). Early radiocarbon dates from Ritidian on Guam and
Unai Bapot on Saipan (Carson 2008, 2011; Clark et al. 2010; Hung et al.
2011) are associated with thin, calcareous sand Tempered (CST),
red-slipped pottery known as Marianas Red Ware (first described by
Spoehr 1957). Similar pottery has been found at other sites, including
Achugao on Saipan (Butler 1994) and the House of Taga site on Tinian
(Pellett & Spoehr 1961), though the latter lacks rigorous dating
that could chronologically anchor the site and associated artefacts.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
This chronology is contemporaneous with, or even slightly earlier
than, that of Lapita groups who began moving eastward from Melanesia
into West Polynesia c. 3400-3300 BP, an observation that makes this an
astonishing feat in the history of Pacific island colonisation. Not only
do the Marianas lie between 1800 and 2300km from the nearest mainland
source--thought to be somewhere in Island Southeast Asia (ISEA)--but
there are very few intervening islands to use as stepping stones,
regardless of which mainland source was the original departure point.
That would give the Marianas chain the distinction of being the most
remote, and earliest, island chain to have been settled in Remote
Oceania, involving a sea crossing of some 2000km. This is in stark
contrast to Lapita peoples who initially crossed gaps of only hundreds
of kilometres.
While the Marianas may represent the longest sea crossing known in
Pacific island prehistory, the origin of these initial colonisers is
still debated. Hung et al. (2011), for example, have postulated that the
first settlers originated from the northern Philippines. That is based
on typological similarities and the chronological overlap of Marianas
Red Ware with red-slipped, incised and stamped pottery found at the
Nagsabaran site in the Cagayan Valley of north-eastern Luzon in the
Philippines, which dates to 2000-1000 BC. There are also linguistic and
genetic affiliations.
In a rejoinder to Hung et al, (2011), Winter et al. (2012) instead
proposed that the ceramics, linguistics and oceanographic conditions
point to an origin further south in ISEA. They note that it would have
been extremely difficult, if not impossible, to drift or sail eastward
from the Philippines to the Marianas given prevailing currents and winds
and without an adequate sailing technology like the double-hulled canoe
with fixed mast and standing rigging. Instead, they note that the area
between Mindanao in the southern Philippines and the Bird's Head of
New Guinea serves as a more suitable corridor for venturing into western
Micronesia given the fairly consistent south-west winds, particularly
between July and September.
Winter et al. (2012) also conducted analysis of ceramics from
Nagsabaran and Unai Bapot and noted a number of important technological
differences in pottery manufacture, including the dominance of
calcareous sand temper (CST) in the Marianas assemblages and the use of
paddles and anvils, versus the natural volcanic sand temper and coiling
that were preferred at Nagsabaran. It should be noted, however, that the
sample size was fairly small (eight vessels from the Philippines and
nine from the Unai Bapot site in the Marianas; Winter et al. 2012).
Furthermore, additional analysis of the Marianas pottery manufacturing
sequence (M. Carson pers. comm.) has identified a combination of
slab-built and coil-built primary forming, followed by paddle-anvil and
trimming as a secondary stage. Finally, Winter et al. (2012) argue from
the corpus of linguistic evidence that Chamorro (the indigenous language of the Marianas) does not seem to have had a northern Philippine origin,
but instead to have dispersed from ISEA where Malayo-Polynesian was
spoken, though Blust (2000) has noted a number of similarities between
Chamorro and languages found in the central Philippines (or perhaps
elsewhere in the archipelago).
While additional archaeological, linguistic and genetic data are
needed to resolve the origin of early Marianas colonists, one important
avenue of research that has not yet been fully addressed, and which can
complement other lines of evidence, is the use of computer simulations
of seafaring. The issue hinges on whether the islands could have been
settled purposefully versus accidentally given the prevailing
oceanographic conditions (winds, currents). We approach this problem
using a computer simulation program to estimate the likelihood that
prehistoric seafarers reached Guam or other islands in the Marianas
chain from different points around the western Pacific. These
simulations have been used by various researchers around the world for
both prehistoric and historic voyages to estimate the probability that
seafarers (downwind/drift or intentional) leaving one location would
teach another, the trajectories they would take, and the likelihood of
crew survival (e.g. Levison et al. 1973; Irwin 1992, 2010; Di Piazza
& Pearthree 1999; Montenegro et al. 2006, 2008; Avis et al. 2007; Di
Piazza et al. 2007). In this paper, we use the seafaring simulation
program known as Seascape, which was developed by one of the authors
(R.C.) and has been used in numerous other successful projects (e.g.
Callaghan 2001, 2003a, b & c, 2008; Callaghan & Bray 2007;
Callaghan & Fitzpatrick 2007, 2008; Fitzpatrick & Callaghan
2008a & b, 2009). Our goal is not to review the archaeological,
linguistic or genetic data to decide whether one part of Island
Southeast Asia or Melanesia was a more likely origin for Chamorro
groups. We seek instead to provide additional data on seafaring
probabilities to estimate the most likely trajectory, and thereby to
complement other lines of evidence.
Below we detail the functions and parameters of the Seascape
simulation program and discuss the findings of both drifting (downwind)
and intentional (directed) voyaging scenarios for each month of the year
for Taiwan, Luzon, Mindanao, Halmahera, western New Guinea, central New
Guinea, eastern New Guinea and the Bismarck Archipelago. These represent
the most likely points of departure for colonists to the Marianas. We
also simulated directed voyages from Guam to the Philippines. The
results suggest that there is 0% probability during any time of the year
of either downwind or directed voyages being able to colonise the
Marianas from Taiwan or the Philippines. In general, the highest success
rates for directed voyages were from Halmahera, the Bismarcks or points
along the northern New Guinea coastline. Overall, the only successful
downwind or directed voyages from any location we tested were those
undertaken between July and October, indicating that there was a narrow
window of time during the year when colonisation is likely to have
occurred.
Methods
The Seascape simulation program uses wind and current data for each
month of the year from the CD-ROM version of the U.S. Navy's Marine
climatic atlas of the world (Version 1.1; U.S. Navy 1995) (see Callaghan
2001, 2003a, b & c, 2008; Callaghan & Bray 2007; Callaghan &
Fitzpatrick 2007, 2008; Fitzpatrick & Callaghan 2008a & b, 2009;
Callaghan & Scarre 2009). With the exception of Arctic regions, the
data in the Atlas (U.S. Navy 1995) include all of the world's
oceans and seas.
The actual mechanics of the program (how the simulations are
structured) includes ways in which the data are organised and selected,
how success is calculated as a percentage of the total of voyages, and a
number of variables. Four main variables are considered when conducting
these seafaring simulations: 1) vessel type; 2) propulsion; 3) wind
patterns; and 4) current patterns.
The data are organised in a resolution of one-degree Marsden
squares (one degree of latitude x one degree of longitude). The program
randomly selects wind and current data that are frequency-weighted
according to the compiled observations of the Marine Climatic Atlas.
These forces are then allowed to operate on vessels for a 24-hour period
before a new selection is made (see Levison et al. 1973 for a
justification of the period length). Success is simply the percentage of
voyages reaching a selected target within a survivable time.
Vessel type and propulsion are important variables of any
simulation, as the shape of an object both above and below the waterline
affects its response to the wind. Polar diagrams can be used to
determine performance characteristics (e.g. Di Piazza et al. 2007:
1221). These diagrams, which give the ratio of vessel velocity to true
wind velocity, are available for a number of Polynesian sailing vessel
types. Performance characteristics can also be determined by held
experimentation (e.g. Gladwin 1970; Finney 1979, 2003; Lewis 1994) which
is particularly useful for non-sailing craft.
Objects caught within a current are affected by a 1:1 ratio (i.e.
unless other forces are also operating, the object has the same set and
speed as the current). Unless the current is exceptionally strong, any
object floating with an appreciable part above the waterline will be
more affected by wind than by currents. Traditional navigators in
Kiribati, for example, used the effect of wind on objects floating high
in the water to determine the direction of land when recent winds
differed from the flow of the current (Lewis 1994).
The actual distance and direction travelled are based on the wind
and current data combined with the speed of vessels in relation to the
wind (from Levison et al. 1973) and parameters selected by the program
operator. Parameters of the simulation are choices made by the program
operator in order to set up the simulation to answer a particular
question. This includes the following information: 1) point of origin
and destination; 2) crew strategy; 3) performance characteristics; 4)
duration of voyages; 5) time of year; and 6) number of simulations.
In this study, multiple points of origin were used: Taiwan; Luzon;
Mindanao; Halmahera; western New Guinea; central New Guinea; eastern New
Guinea; and the Bismarck Archipelago. We also simulated directed voyages
from Guam to the Philippines. Vessels were positioned as close to the
centres of Marsden squares as possible, but in some cases were moved
further away, as vessels cannot be started on land. This was an issue
discovered by Levison et al. (1973), who noted that vessels starting too
close to shore tended to return to the same coast.
Two crew strategies were simulated. The first was downwind sailing,
where the crew simply tries to cover as much distance as possible. This
is a strategy often used when the vessel's position has been lost
due to storms (Dening 1963: 138-53). The second strategy was directed
sailing where the crew selects a heading. Here, no intent was assumed
for the crew other than to travel in the direction of the Marianas or,
in the case of Guam, to the Philippines due west.
In this instance, we do not know what types of vessels were used,
although some form of outrigger canoe is most likely. Given the overall
uncertainty of vessel type, we have chosen to follow Levison et al.
(1973: 19, 27) and use more generalised figures that are reasonable for
a variety of watercraft types. Levison et al. (1973: 27), basing their
estimation for windward sailing on experiments by Horvath and Finney
(1969) and Bechtol (1963: 100), and observations of modern canoes by
Gladwin (1970), assumed that vessels could only sail on a teach
(perpendicular to the wind) at 90[degrees]. This is more conservative
than actual experiments and observations suggest. It is also assumed
that when winds are on the beam or aft, the vessels could maintain
speeds equivalent to those adapted for downwind sailing.
The duration of voyages is a critical factor in crew survival. Some
indication of realistic maximum durations for voyages can be derived
from lengthy drift voyages in open boats, due, for example, to shipwreck or other misfortune. These are well known in the Pacific both
historically and in modern times (Howay 1944; Levison et al. 1973:
20-21). The maximum drift voyage seems to be of the order of seven to
eight months. Several voyages recorded covered distances of nearly
5000km over a period of six to ten weeks, and a great number covered
shorter distances. Given that intentional voyages with the supplies and
equipment necessary for long durations at sea (rather than drift
voyages) are also considered here, a maximum duration of 200 days was
set for the simulations.
Time of year is important, as in most regions of the world there is
significant seasonal variation in wind and current patterns. Vessels
were started from all points of origin for all 12 months of the year.
The program automatically shifts to the database for the following month
after the month originally selected has expired, reflecting the changing
wind and current conditions.
The number of simulations from each origin point is 100 for each
month. Earlier work (Callaghan 1999) conducted 1000 simulations for each
origin point. Only very rarely, however, did an outlier occur different
from those given by the smaller sample of 100, prompting the use of a
sample size of 100 in subsequent analyses.
Results
Out simulations demonstrated that for downwind sailing (Table 1),
there was no possibility of reaching the Marianas at any time of the
year from Taiwan, Luzon or Mindanao. Overall success is highest from
central New Guinea, with 13% of vessels landing in the Marianas if
starting in August, 25% in September (Figure 2) and 2% in October
Durations of voyages ranged from 58 to 198 days. Success rates were the
second highest from the Bismarck Archipelago, with 1% of vessels
starting in July reaching the Marianas, 4% in August, 13% in September
(Figure 3) and 9% in October. Durations of voyages ranged from 54 to 191
days. Vessels starting from western New Guinea had the next highest
success rate. Vessels starting in July and August had 3% success in
reaching the Marianas, 7% in September and 8% in October. Durations of
voyages ranged from 56 to 184 days. From eastern New Guinea, success
rates were 6% in August and September and 3% in October. Durations of
voyages were from 84 to 192 days. Finally, from Halmahera, success was
9% in September and 2% in October. Durations were from 56 to 137 days.
As with downwind voyages, successful directed voyages were not
possible from Taiwan, Luzon or Mindanao (Table 2). From the remaining
origin points, it was possible to reach the Marianas from July to
October, with the exception of central New Guinea where success was
possible from August to October. Success rates did not follow the same
pattern as for downwind voyages. Vessels sailing from the Bismarck
Archipelago had the highest success, with 14% in July, 32% in August
(Figure 4), 24% in September and 18% in October. Durations of voyages
lasted from 30 to 98 days. From Halmahera, vessels had a 13% chance of
reaching the Marianas in July, 27% in August (Figure 5), 20% in
September and 10% in October. Durations of voyages were from 22 to 184
days. From western New Guinea, success rates were 5% in July, 10% in
August, 24% in September and 26% in October (Figure 6). Voyages lasted
from 37 to 144 days. From eastern New Guinea, voyages starting in July
had a 9% success rate, 17% in August and September and 14% in October.
Voyages lasted between 35 and 98 days. Interestingly, central New
Guinea, which had the highest downwind sailing success rate, had the
lowest rate for directed voyages. Those voyages beginning in August had
only a 2% rate of success, 20% in September and 18% in October. Voyages
lasted between 29 and 94 days.
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
Generally, if it is difficult to sail in a direction due to
contrary wind and current patterns, it will be easier to sail in the
opposite direction. This is the case for directed voyages from Guam to
the west (Table 3). However, in July and August, vessels attempting to
sail west towards the Philippines are all swept northwards by winds and
currents. With a June departure, only one vessel reached the
Philippines, while two reached Taiwan. The rest of the voyages were
swept northwards by the Kuroshio Current. The results for September were
similar, with only three vessels reaching the northern Philippines. The
rest of the voyages went north to China and Taiwan. Between October and
May, success rates for vessels reaching the Philippines were high and
nearly all vessels reached land. In October conditions, 96% of vessels
landed in the Philippines and the rest made landfall to the south. In
November, 91% of vessels made landfall in the Philippines, with 8%
landing on the New Guinea coast and 1% on Palau. Given a start with
December conditions, 77% of the vessels reached the Philippines, while
23% made landfall to the south. For a January start, success was 73%,
with 23% of vessels landing on Halmahera or the Moluccas, 2% on Palau,
1% in western New Guinea and 1% on Yap. With February starting
conditions, 33% of vessels landed in the Philippines, 58% in Halmahera
and the Moluccas, 3% in western New Guinea and 6% in Sulawesi. For
March, 52% of vessels reached the Philippines and 44% reached Halmahera
and the Moluccas, while 4% landed in New Guinea. With April starting
conditions, 73% of voyages ended in the Philippines, while the rest
moved north. Starting in May, 65% of vessels reached the Philippines and
the remaining voyages terminated in Taiwan and China.
[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]
An interesting observation is that at some times of the year, there
is a high probability that vessels sailing downwind from all of the
origin points other than Guam will end up in the Philippines. From
Halmahera in October, 20% of downwind voyages land in the Philippines.
From western New Guinea in September, 63% make the same landfall. For
central New Guinea, 63% of voyages land in the Philippines starting in
October. Starting in September, 26% of voyages make a Philippine
landfall, and with October start conditions, 34% of vessels from the
Bismarcks land in the Philippines.
[FIGURE 5 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 6 OMITTED]
Discussion and conclusions
The foregoing simulations allow us to evaluate the competing models
for the colonisation of the Marianas from the northern Philippines (Hung
et al. 2011) or from further south (Winter et al. 2012). Our results
indicate that there is a 0% chance of either intentional (directed) or
unintentional (drift) voyaging to the Marianas from Taiwan or anywhere
in the northern Philippines (Tables 1 & 2). This casts doubt on the
model advocated by Hung et al. (2011). There is, however, a reasonable
chance of reaching the Marianas from Halmahera, the Bismarcks or
anywhere along the northern New Guinea littoral, especially by directed
voyages. The success rate is as high as 32% for intentional voyaging to
Guam from the Bismarcks and 27% from Halmahera during the month of
August (Table 2).
Interestingly, while it is impossible to reach Guam from Taiwan or
the northern Philippines under prevailing oceanographic conditions,
vessels leaving Guam generally have a high rate of success in reaching
the Philippines during the late autumn and early winter months (see
Table 1). It is theoretically possible that anomalous conditions such as
a storm event could have driven initial colonisers to the Marianas from
the northern Philippines--at which point they could much more easily
have voyaged back (intentionally or not) to their supposed homeland
during certain times of the year. This scenario appears unlikely,
however, from the simulation evidence, particularly since directed
voyages from the northern Philippines were never successful at any time
of year.
It is difficult to say with any degree of certainty whether the
initial voyages were undertaken by accident or design. Population
movements in other parts of ISEA testify to increasingly sophisticated
watercraft and navigational skills during the Late Holocene, however,
and it is not outside the realm of possibility that people consistently
pursued new islands to colonise, albeit with a low rate of initial
success. The further possibility exists that some islands were
'discovered' but not fully colonised until later in time.
The early radiocarbon dates from the Marianas, dating to as early
as c. 3500 BP, are indicative of a lengthy voyage from some point in
ISEA or the northern New Guinea region, even when intervening islands
are considered. From many lines of evidence, it appears that
communication between the Philippines and islands in western Micronesia
occurred periodically, though at different scales and levels of
intensity through time. While pottery similar to Marianas Red Ware has
not been found in Indonesia to date, the simulations we conducted
emphasise that additional linguistic, genetic and archaeological
research is needed to resolve the question of Chamorro origins, with a
departure point further south being a distinct possibility.
References
AVIS, C., A. MONTENEGRO & A. WEAVER. 2007. The discovery of
western Oceania: a new perspective. Journal of Island and Coastal
Archaeology 2: 197-209.
BECHTOL, C. 1963. Sailing characteristics of oceanic canoes, in J.
Golson (ed.) Polynesian navigation (Polynesian Society Memoir 34):
98-101. Auckland: Polynesian Society.
BLUST, R.A. 2000. Chamorro historical phonology. Oceanic
Linguistics 39: 83-122.
BUTLER, B.M. 1994. Early prehistoric settlement in the Mariana
Islands: new evidence from Saipan. Man and Culture in Oceania 10: 15-38.
CALLAGHAN, R.T. 1999. Computer simulations of ancient voyaging.
Northern Mariner 9: 11-22.
--2001. Ceramic age seafaring and interaction potential in the
Antilles: a computer simulation. Current Anthropology 42: 308-13.
--2003a. Comments on the mainland origins of the Preceramic
cultures of the Greater Antilles. Latin American Antiquity 14: 323-38.
--2003b. Prehistoric trade between Ecuador and west Mexico: a
computer simulation of coastal voyages. Antiquity 77: 796-804.
--2003c. The use of simulation models to estimate frequency and
location of Japanese Edo period wrecks along the Canadian Pacific Coast.
Canadian Journal of Archaeology 27: 62-82.
--2008. On the question of the absence of Archaic Age sites on
Jamaica. Journal of Island and Coastal Archaeology 3: 54-71.
CALLAGHAN, R.T & W. BRAY. 2007. Simulating prehistoric sea
contacts between Costa Rica and Colombia. Journal of Island and Coastal
Archaeology 2: 4-23.
CALLAGHAN, R.T. & S.M. FITZPATRICK. 2007. On the relative
isolation of a Micronesian archipelago during the historic period: the
Palau case study. International Journal of Nautical Archaeology 36:
353-64.
--2008. Examining prehistoric migration patterns in the Palauan
archipelago: a computer simulated analysis of drift voyaging. Asian
Perspectives 47: 28-44.
CALLAGHAN, R.T. & C. SCARRE. 2009. Simulating the western
seaways. Oxford Journal of Archaeology 28: 357-72.
CARSON, M.T. 2008. Refining earliest settlements in Remote Oceania:
renewed archaeological investigations at Unai Bapot, Saipan. Journal of
Island and Coastal Archaeology 3: 115-39.
--2011. Paleohabitat of first settlement sites 1500-1000 BC in
Guam, Mariana Islands, western Pacific. Journal of Archaeological
Science 38: 2207-21.
CARSON, M.T. & H. KURASHINA. 2012. Re-envisioning long-distance
oceanic migration: early dates in the Mariana Islands. World Archaeology
44: 409-35.
CLARK, G., F. PETCHEY, O. WINTER, M. CARSON & P. O'DAY.
2010. New radiocarbon dates from the Bapot-1 site in Saipan and
Neolithic dispersal by stratified diffusion. Journal of Pacific
Archaeology 1: 21-35.
DENING, CM. 1963. The geographical knowledge of the Polynesians and
the nature of inter-island contact, in J. Golson (ed.) Polynesian
navigation (Polynesian Society Memoir 34): 138-53. Auckland: Polynesian
Society.
DI PIAZZA, A. & E. PEARTHREE. 1999. The spread of the
'Lapita people': a demographic simulation. Journal of
Artificial Societies and Social Simulation 2(3). Available at:
http://www.soc.surrey.ac.Uk/JASSS/2/3/4.html (accessed 16 April 2013).
DI PIAZZA, A., P. DI PIAZZA & E. PEARTHREE. 2007. Sailing
virtual canoes across Oceania: revisiting island accessibility. Journal
of Archaeological Science 344: 1219-25.
FINNEY, B. 1979. Hokule'a, the way to Tahiti. New York: Dodd,
Mead & Co.
--2003. Sailing in the wake of the ancestors: reviving Polynesian
voyaging. Honolulu: Bishop Museum.
FITZPATRICK, S.M. & R.T. CALLAGHAN. 2008a. Magellan's
crossing of the Pacific: using computer simulations to examine how
oceanographic conditions influenced one of the world's greatest
voyages. Journal of Pacific History 48(2): 145-65.
--2008b. Seafaring simulations and the origin of prehistoric
settlers to Madagascar, in G. Clark & S. O'Connor (ed.) Islands
of inquiry: papers in honour of Atholl Anderson (Terra Australis 29):
55-66. Canberra: Australian National University Press.
--2009. Examining dispersal mechanisms for the translocation of
chicken (Gallus gallus) from Polynesia to South America. Journal of
Archaeological Science 36: 214-23.
GLADWIN, T. 1970. East is a big bird: navigation and logic on
Puluwat Atoll. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.
HORVATH, S.M. & B. FINNEY. 1969. Paddling experiments and the
question of Polynesian voyaging. American Anthropologist 71: 271-76.
HOWAY, F.W. 1944. Some lengthy open-boat voyages in the Pacific
Ocean. American Neptune 4: 53-57.
HUNG, H-C., M.T. Carson, P. Bellwood, F.Z. CAMPOS, P.J. PIPER, E.
DIZON, M.J.L.A. BOLUNIA, M. OXENHAM & C. ZHANG. 2011. The first
settlement of Remote Oceania: the Philippines to the Marianas. Antiquity
85: 909-26.
IRWIN, G.J. 1992. The prehistoric exploration and colonisation of
the Pacific. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
--2010. Pacific voyaging and settlement: issues of biogeography and
archaeology, canoe performance and computer simulation, in A. Anderson,
J.H. Barrett & K.V. Boyle (ed.) The global origins and development
of seafaring. 131-41. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological
Research.
LEVISON, M., R.G. WARD & J.W. WEBB. 1973. The settlement of
Polynesia, a computer simulation. Canberra: The Australian National
University.
LEWIS, D. 1994. We, the navigators. Honolulu: University Press of
Hawai'i.
MONTENEGRO, A., R. HETHERINGTON, M. EBY & A.J. WEAVER. 2006.
Modelling pre-historic transoceanic crossings into the Americas.
Quaternary Science Reviews 25: 1323-38.
MONTENEGRO, A., C. AVIS & A.J. WEAVER. 2008. Modelling the
pre-historic arrival of the sweet potato in Polynesia. Journal of
Archaeological Science 35: 355-67.
PELLETT, M. & A. SPOEHR. 1961. Marianas archaeology: report on
an excavation on Tinian. Journal of the Polynesia Society 70: 321-25.
SPOEHR, A. 1957. Marianas prehistory: archaeological survey and
excavations on Saipan, Tinian and Rota (Fieldiana: Anthropology 48).
Chicago (IL): Chicago Natural History Museum.
U.S. Navy. 1995. Marine climatic atlas of the world. Asheville
(NC): National Climatic Data Center.
WINTER, O., G. CLARK, A. ANDERSON & A. LINDAHL. 2012.
Austronesian sailing to the northern Marianas, a comment on Hung et al.
(2011). Antiquity 86: 898-910.
Received: 24 July 2012; Accepted: 13 September 2012; Revised: 20
November 2012
Scott M. Fitzpatrick (1) & Richard T. Callaghan (2)
(1) Department of Anthropology, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR
97403, USA
(2) Department of Archaeology, University of Calgary, 2500
University Dr. NW, Alberta, T2N1N4, Canada
Table 1. Success rates for downwind voyages to the Mariana Islands
and duration in days (in parentheses).
Code Taiwan Luzon Mindanao Halmahera
January 0% 0% 0% 0%
February 0% 0% 0% 0%
March 0% 0% 0% 0%
April 0% 0% 0% 0%
May 0% 0% 0% 0%
June 0% 0% 0% 0%
July 0% 0% 0% 0%
August 0% 0% 0% 0%
September 0% 0% 0% 9% (56-137)
October 0% 0% 0% 2% (57-91)
November 0% 0% 0% 0%
December 0% 0% 0% 0%
Code W. New Guinea C. New Guinea
January 0% 0%
February 0% 0%
March 0% 0%
April 0% 0%
May 0% 0%
June 0% 0%
July 3% (77-181) 0%
August 3% (56-184) 13% (60-175)
September 7% (101-141) 25% (58-198)
October 8% (65-178) 2% (63-116)
November 0% 0%
December 0% 0%
Code E. New Guinea Bismarck Archipelago
January 0% 0%
February 0% 0%
March 0% 0%
April 0% 0%
May 0% 0%
June 0% 0%
July 0% 1% (90)
August 6% (84-192) 4% (137-176)
September 6% (106-138) 13% (54-158)
October 3% (166-175) 9% (76-191)
November 0% 0%
December 0% 0%
Table 2. Success rates for directed voyages to the Mariana Islands
and duration in days (in parentheses).
Taiwan Luzon Mindanao Halmahera W. New Guinea
January 0% 0% 0% 0% 0%
February 0% 0% 0% 0% 0%
March 0% 0% 0% 0% 0%
April 0% 0% 0% 0% 0%
May 0% 0% 0% 0% 0%
June 0% 0% 0% 0% 0%
July 0% 0% 0% 13% (38-146) 5% (49-109)
August 0% 0% 0% 27% (22-184) 10% (37-71)
September 0% 0% 0% 20% (30-160) 24% (39-101)
October 0% 0% 0% 10% (39-160) 26% (42-144)
November 0% 0% 0% 0% 0%
December 0% 0% 0% 0% 0%
C. New Guinea E. New Guinea Bismarck Archipelago
January 0% 0% 0%
February 0% 0% 0%
March 0% 0% 0%
April 0% 0% 0%
May 0% 0% 0%
June 0% 0% 0%
July 0% 9% (35-98) 14% (35-82)
August 2% (29-53) 17% (39-93) 32% (39-82)
September 20% (40-46) 17% (39-74) 24% (34-76)
October 18% (41-94) 14% (41-76) 18% (30-98)
November 0% 0% 0%
December 0% 0% 0%
Table 3. Success rates for directed voyages to the Philippines
from Guam.
January 73%
February 33%
March 52%
April 73%
May 65%
June 1%
July 0%
August 0%
September 3%
October 96%
November 91%
December 77%