Towards a prehistory of primates.
Haslam, Michael
Introduction
The social and technological traits of living non-human primates
(henceforth 'primates') have contributed significantly to
reconstructions of the activities of their past common ancestors with
humans (van Schaik et al. 2003; Haslam et al. 2009; McGrew 2010; Silk
2011; Whiten 2011; Wynn et al. 2011). Customary tool-use by wild
primates, a key trait of interest to archaeologists, has been recorded
in tropical Africa, South America and Southeast Asia, including by
chimpanzees (Figure 1), orangutans and bearded capuchin monkeys (McGrew
1992; van Schaik et al. 1996; Spagnoletti et al. 2011). Less well
studied or less prevalent wild tool-use has also been noted among
gorillas, bonobos, long-tailed macaques and both yellow-breasted and
blonde capuchins (Hohmann & Fruth 2003; Breuer et al. 2005;
Malaivijimond et al. 2007; Canale et al. 2009; Souto et al. 2011). Yet
there is a great disparity in the observational time span for human and
primate behaviour, because the archaeological record of hominins (human
ancestors back to our split with the Pan lineage) has unusual
chronological depth.
At present, hominin technical activities have been detected back
~3.4 million years (McPherron et al. 2010), with a well-studied and
possibly continuous stone tool record since 2.6 million years ago (mya)
(Semaw et al. 2003). This record is augmented by non-stone prehistoric
technologies for tens, and in some cases hundreds, of thousands of years
(e.g. Thieme 1997). In contrast, we have around half a century of
systematic chimpanzee behavioural data, and even less for other
primates, although anecdotal reports from West Africa extend back around
400 years (Sept & Brooks 1994). When extinct hominin behaviours are
compared with those of living primates (e.g. Joulian 1996; Gowlett 2009;
Pruetz & Bertolani 2009; Toth & Schick 2009; de la Torte 2010;
Ungar & Sponheimer 2011), we need to ask whether extant primate
behaviour is representative of the several million years of behavioural
evolution that preceded it. In other words, when did apes, monkeys and
other primates become 'behaviourally modern', and how would a
more complete primate archaeological record change our perspective on
human evolution?
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Behavioural modernity
Significant behavioural changes have occurred in the hominin
lineage during the past few million years, and archaeologists studying
Homo sapiens have concentrated on the issue of 'behavioural
modernity' (Minugh-Purvis 1995; McBrearty & Brooks 2000; Wadley
2001; d'Errico 2003; Henshilwood & Marean 2003; Renfrew 2007;
Conard 2008). At the core of this concept is a search for features that
are unique to our species, an attempt to identify the process by which
the habits and capacities of past humans came to be recognisably similar
to those of humans living today (Klein 2000). While this may be a useful
driver of debate (d'Errico & Stringer 2011; Shea 2011), the
proposal of a radical behavioural shift, or 'human revolution'
(Mellars & Stringer 1989; Bar-Yosef 2002) part way through hominin
evolution is unusual when placed in a broader zoological context. There
is, for example, no literature on the timing and character of the
emergence of 'modern Macaca behaviour', or international
conferences discussing a possible 'Papio behavioural
revolution'.
From the mid twentieth century onwards, the combination of
increasing numbers of hominin fossil discoveries and the establishment
of long-term field primatology sites opened up new debates over the
similarities between hominins and other African primates (e.g. Leakey
1961; Washburn & DeVore 1961; Holloway 1969; Jolly 1970; Foley
1987). Archaeologists explored the ways that the emerging primate
literature could inform topics such as early Pleistocene site creation
and social structure (Clark 1960; Isaac 1969), but it was modern,
twentieth-century primates that were the point of comparison. Little or
no attention was given to possible pre-Anthropocene (Steffen et al.
2011) primate behavioural characteristics, except those hypothesised for
common human-chimpanzee ancestors (e.g. Parker & Gibson 1979).
The lack of comparative Pan fossils can promote the notion of a
static phenotype for all chimpanzees, past and present. For example,
whereas hominins are represented by a variety of species on charts of
brain volume evolution, chimpanzees can only be represented by a modern
value (e.g. Aiello & Dean 1990; Gamble et al. 2011). Yet a number of
ancestral panins (the distinct bonobo-chimpanzee lineage) must have
existed during their separate evolution over the past 5-7 million years
(Kumar et al. 2005). The average time from speciation to extinction for
large African mammal species is 2.33 million years (Vrba 2000), while
Foley (1993) calculated that African terrestrial primates have a species
longevity close to one million years. The one known panin speciation,
splitting chimpanzees from bonobos, occurred in the Early Pleistocene
(Stone et al. 2010; Wegmann & Excoffier 2010). We can therefore
posit a minimum of two (and very likely more) extinct direct panin
ancestor species since the split from hominins, with an unknown number
of dead-end side branches and additional genera (Wood & Harrison
2011).
Cultural variation observed in living chimpanzees and orangutans
(Whiten et al. 2001; van Schaik et al. 2003) suggests that we should not
expect the primate archaeological record to be homogeneous in time or
space, even when produced by a single species. Extant wild tool-using
primates live in a wide range of habitats and have varied social systems
(e.g. van Schaik et al. 1999; McGrew 2004; Mannu & Ottoni 2009;
Gumert et al. 2011; Shumaker et al. 2011), factors that influence the
transmission of technological activities (Biro et al. 2010). The
capacity for cultural variation means that, depending on available
materials, non-hominin primates likely experimented with a greater range
of tool types than those seen today, including a range of non-stone
tools (Panger et al. 2002; Byrne 2005; Haslam et al. 2009).
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
A full treatment of these topics lies outside the scope of this
review, and I therefore concentrate here on three broad and interlinked
categories for which a long-term perspective is important for inter- and
intra-species behavioural comparisons: geographic patterning, diet and
innovation. These three categories also reflect key adaptive shifts in
Pliocene hominin evolution (Potts 2007), and their investigation will
assist in understanding the causes of hominin divergence from other
primates. My focus is on the most intensively studied living close
relative to humans, chimpanzees (Nishida et al. 2010), but the concepts
are not naturally limited to any one primate (or even non-primate)
species.
Towards a prehistory of primates
Geographic patterning
Modern chimpanzees are behaviourally flexible and occupy a wider
variety of environments than most primate taxa, including lowland and
montane forests, woodlands and drier grasslands or savannah. This
adaptability suggests that the current geographic distribution of
chimpanzees (Figure 2) is unlikely to directly reflect past
distribution. We know that the chimpanzee range is currently shrinking
(Butynski 2003), and a significant earlier shift is indicated by the
finding of putative chimpanzee fossils in Kenya, ~600km east of the
species' current range (McBrearty & Jablonski 2005). The
question here is, how did past primate populations spread, split and
contract, and how did this affect cultural variation?
Genetic analyses have begun to reveal the population structure and
history of the four commonly recognised chimpanzee subspecies: the
western (Pan troglodytes verus), Nigeria-Cameroon (P. t. ellioti),
central (P. t. troglodytes) and eastern (P. t. schweinfurthii)
populations (Goldberg 1998; Gagneux et al. 2001; Yu et al. 2003; Won
& Hey 2005; Caswell et al. 2008; Hey 2010; Stone et al. 2010;
Wegmann & Excoffier 2010; Bjork et al. 2011). These studies suggest
that major rivers in Central Africa (in particular the Niger, Sanaga and
Ubangi rivers) act as rarely permeated boundaries to the various
subspecies ranges, in addition to isolating the bonobo (Pan paniscus)
south of the Congo River (Gagneux et al. 2001; Bjork et al. 2011).
Within these regions, the potential importance of Pleistocene refugia in
promoting allopatric differentiation has been considered (Lehman &
Fleagle 2006), but at least for eastern chimpanzees there is little
correlation between posited forest refugia and present-day diversity
(Goldberg 1998).
Pounding stone use has been included in the list of cultural traits
charted in multi-site chimpanzee studies (Whiten et al. 1999, 2001).
These studies recognise clear geographic variation, with wild
chimpanzees from different study sites using different combinations of
stone and wood hammers and anvils, or alternatively not using stones to
crack nuts even when both are available. Explanations offered for this
variation include the variety and hardness of available nut species
(Boesch & Boesch 1990; McGrew et al. 1997) and limited spatial
diffusion of nut-cracking knowledge between sites (McGrew 1992).
However, causal mechanisms are difficult to infer over short time spans,
and differentiating between cases in which primate cultural variants are
absent because knowledge has been lost from a particular site or region,
or absent because they were never invented by the resident groups
(Wrangham 2006; Whiten et al. 2009), is very difficult without a
referent chronological sequence. Experimental evidence from captive
primates (e.g. Hayashi et al. 2005; Marshall-Pescini & Whiten 2008b)
has limited ability to address this issue, and studies that seek to
generalise findings from captive primate subjects may have to
acknowledge the limited scope of those findings for understanding
primate behaviour before the twentieth century (Boesch 2007; Leavens et
al. 2010; Whiten 2011).
It would be particularly interesting to see if chimpanzee use of
pounding stones has persisted throughout periods where shifting climates
altered the available nut and fruit resources (Dupont et al. 2010), or
whether such tool-use disappears and re-appears in tune with
environmental changes. In this regard, it is important to note that
chimpanzee pounding stone use is currently localised to west Africa (and
P. t. verus), which may limit the archaeological study of such
traditions and their correlates to this part of the modern chimpanzee
range, although recent anecdotal evidence for stone tool-using
Nigeria-Cameroon chimpanzees (Morgan & Abwe 2006) suggests that this
behaviour might be more widespread. Nonetheless, as a starting point we
can hypothesise that stone tool-use as a behaviourally modern trait in
Pan is limited by the time of genetic divergence (as a proxy for
physical separation) of the western chimpanzees from the central-eastern
clade. Present consensus places this process ~1-0.5mya (Becquet et al.
2007; Caswell et al. 2008; Stone et al. 2010; Wegmann & Excoffier
2010), and more likely towards the younger end of that spectrum. Viewed
as a specific West African innovation, and not as a remnant behaviour of
the hominin-panin last common ancestor, the most plausible hypothesis is
that chimpanzee stone tool-use began some time closely prior to 500 000
years ago. While tool-use traits do not always align with genetic
divisions in chimpanzees (Lycett et al. 2010), stone tool-use currently
does, and excavation of the African primate archaeological record is the
only way to test this hypothesis.
Emerging genetic data for the capuchin monkeys of Central and South
America (Lynch Alfaro et al. 2011) similarly provide targets for future
archaeological investigation. The capuchin Cebus genus has recently been
reclassified into two genera (the gracile Cebus and the more robust
Sapajus), the result of a late Miocene split potentially related to the
establishment of the Amazon River (Hoorn et al. 2010). Use of pounding
stones to open palm nuts in the wild is currently only known among
Sapajus (Figure 3), in the drier cerrado (grassland to scrub woodland)
and caatinga (dry forest and thorn scrub) environments that separate the
Brazilian Atlantic and Amazon forests (Moura & Lee 2004; Visalberghi
et al. 2007; Canale et al. 2009). Sapajus began to diversify from an
origin zone somewhere in the Atlantic Forest around 3-2.5mya, before
expanding west into the drier habitats, a movement postulated to have
occurred around 0.7mya (Casado et al. 2010; Lynch Alfaro et al. 2011).
As forest dwelling capuchins have not been observed to use stone tools,
the genetic data therefore provide an initial prospective date for the
origins of lithic technology among the capuchins, with the intriguing
possibility that it was this specific technological innovation that
permitted their dispersal into the previously uninhabited areas. The
data further suggest that the north-western members of these dispersing
groups subsequently moved into the Amazon rainforest (already occupied
by Cebus) after 0.4mya (Lynch Alfaro et al. 2011). We can only speculate
at present, but the absence of stone pounding tools among modern
Amazonian Sapajus populations may well result from loss of a
technological adaptation that was strongly tied to the palm nuts found
in the cerrado and caatinga. In this scenario, testable through
archaeological exploration, the lithic technologies of both
'behaviourally modern' capuchins and chimpanzees therefore
have a Middle Pleistocene origin.
Beyond genetic reconstructions, the recovery of stone (or other)
tools used by primates is strong proxy evidence of the presence of those
primates at a given time and place. Fortunately, chimpanzees and
capuchins transport and accumulate stone materials (Boesch & Boesch
1984; Visalberghi et al. 2007; Canale et al. 2009; Carvalho et al. 2009;
Spagnoletti et al. 2011), creating recognisable sites and constructing
technological niches that may structure subsequent activity (Figure 3).
Redundant patterning has also been documented for chimpanzee nesting
sites (Sept 1998; Hernandez-Aguilar 2009), further concentrating group
behaviour.
The archaeological study of chimpanzee activity areas is currently
in its infancy (Mercader et al. 2002; Carvalho et al. 2008), and a
primary difficulty is in distinguishing primate tools from naturally
occurring stones, or from hominin behavioural debris. Primate preference
for stones of particular density, size and material assist in their
discernment (although this approach is less useful once outside the
range of preferences seen among living primates), as can the careful
documentation of characteristic wear patterns and adhering residues
(Mercader et al. 2007). In the Americas, the task is aided by the
absence of hominins prior to the terminal Pleistocene (Goebel et al.
2008), meaning that stone pounding tools dated prior to this time may in
theory be attributed to non-human primates.
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
Diet
Chimpanzees have a mixed and variable diet, including fruits,
herbaceous vegetation, insects, vertebrates and secondary products such
as honey (e.g. McGrew et al. 1988; Milton 1999; McGrew 2001; Tweheyo et
al. 2004; Laden & Wrangham 2005; Sanz & Morgan 2009). Ideally,
the recovery of archaeological evidence for past chimpanzee behaviour
would allow us to assess the representativeness of modern primate diets,
compared to their ancestors throughout the past few million years. As
discussed earlier, the adaptability of chimpanzees to a variety of
conditions and ability to range across wide areas may act as a buffer to
local climate-driven changes in resources; however, it is reasonable to
hypothesise that some of the now extinct panin species were more
specialised and/or localised than living populations, based on the
variation known from the hominin lineage (e.g. the hyper-robust
Paranthropus, or the island dwelling Homo floresiensis) and the
orangutan lineage (Ungar & Sponheimer 2011; Wood & Harrison
2011). At its simplest, the question that arises is to what extent the
range of potential chimpanzee dietary niches were actually exploited by
past panin species.
Fortunately, tool-use is pervasive in chimpanzee dietary foraging
(McGrew 1992), particularly in the extraction of embedded nutrient rich
resources such as nuts, termites, underground storage organs (USOs) and
honey (e.g. Boesch & Boesch-Achermann 2000; Deblauwe et al. 2006;
Hernandez-Aguilar et al. 2007; Sanz & Morgan 2009). The spearing
tools used by Fongoli chimpanzees to disable small prey hidden in tree
holes (Pruetz & Bertolani 2007) could similarly be seen as targeting
'embedded' food. Tool-use allows us to record diet-related
activities beyond direct observation for primates that are either
unhabituated (Sanz et al. 2004; Fowler & Sommer 2007; Canale et al.
2009; Koops et al. 2010) or even deceased (Mercader et al. 2002). Most
of the tools manufactured and used by chimpanzees are made of plant
materials, which will survive in the short-term (perhaps decades in the
case of nests made by modifying living trees; Stewart et al. 2011), but
are unlikely to enter the long-term archaeological record other than
through fortuitous discard in a waterlogged or hyper-arid setting.
Nonetheless, standardisation of the size and physical characteristics of
many of these tools (McGrew 2004; Boesch et al. 2009; Sanz et al. 2009)
means that they are both recognisable when encountered, and encode
information about the traditions and dietary habits of the group that
created them.
Even more promising for studying long-term changes in diet is the
use of stone and hard wood pounding tools for opening nuts and other
food items, as practised by chimpanzees, capuchin monkeys and
long-tailed macaques (Boesch & Boesch 1990; Ottoni & Izar 2008;
Canale et al. 2009; Carvalho et al. 2009; Gumert et al. 2009). The use
of anvil stones has been presented as a plausible mechanism for the
initial stage leading to deliberate stone tool fracture among hominins
(Marchant & McGrew 2005), and if this is the case then it is worth
exploring the reasons why present-day chimpanzees have not followed a
similar path to stone tool manufacture. Stone tools used by chimpanzees
have a demonstrated antiquity of over 4000 years in the Tai Forest of
Cote d'Ivoire (Mercader et al. 2007), and there is no reason to
suspect that the stone tool record left by past primates in Africa, Asia
and South America is not still present and awaiting study by
archaeologists and primatologists.
As pounding tools are currently recognised as primarily a dietary
adaptation, then at a minimum the antiquity of embedded food
exploitation can be investigated through systematic excavation and
analysis of these artefacts. However, there are also more complex
diet-related questions that may be addressed through the analysis of
stone artefacts used by chimpanzees and other primates. For example, the
relationship between the physical character (shape, size and density) of
selected stone tools and the food types exploited with them deserves
greater attention. Chimpanzees and capuchins choose stone tools based on
their performance characteristics and fitness for the task at hand
(Boesch & Boesch 1982; Visalberghi et al. 2009), an ability we may
reasonably expect panin ancestors to also have possessed. Along with
microscopic residues adhering to used tools (Mercader et al. 2007;
Haslam et al. 2010), the search for correlations of form and function
would allow assessment of the composition, size and hardness of the
foods targeted by past primates. With added chronological resolution,
such assessments will permit the reconstruction of a behavioural
component to primate diet evolution, which can then be further refined
through concurrent environmental reconstruction of artefact find spots
and consideration of dental morphologies and wear (Ungar 1998).
The exploitation of embedded foods provides a nutritional advantage
to tool-using primates (Wrangham 2006). While it is rarely considered
that either a chimpanzee direct ancestor or a side branch of the panin
lineage may have developed radically different or perhaps even more
complex technology than extant individuals, the selective impetus is
there, in the form of nutrient and calorie-rich nuts, as well as in
underground starchy foods (Hernandez-Aguilar et al. 2007). Each primate
lineage has had millions of years in which to discover, spread, lose and
re-discover the benefits of technology, and it is unlikely that the
forms of stone tool-use observed among living primates are the only ones
ever to have evolved. The increased energetic returns gained by
accessing embedded resources is a plausible reason to hypothesise that
tool-use traditions at least equivalent in complexity to modern
behaviour may have been repeatedly discovered by past primate, and
hominin, populations.
Innovation
Cumulative cultural change plays a key role in behavioural studies
attempting to differentiate primate and human learning processes
(Marshall-Pescini & Whiten 2008a), with investigation into
mechanisms such as imitation and emulation (Tennie et al. 2009). Studies
of human behavioural modernity have turned in recent years to the impact
of demography on permitting and sustaining behavioural innovations and
patterns (Shennan 2001; Powell et al. 2009; Richerson et al. 2009).
Chimpanzee research has also begun to explore these connections (Mitani
2006; Lycett et al. 2009), with a correlation shown between larger
female group sizes and the number of habitual and customary cultural
traits in several wild chimpanzee communities (Lind & Lindenfors
2010). This again raises the possibility that chimpanzee groups affected
by recent human activities may not be behaviourally representative of
past panins.
The same genetic studies that seek to identify past chimpanzee
population expansions and contractions (e.g. Won & Hey 2005; Caswell
et al. 2008) also provide spatio-temporal targets for investigating the
occurrence of periods of more and less rapid cultural innovation.
Mitochondrial studies that reconstruct female population size trends may
be of special interest in this regard, if females were a primary vector
for cultural transmission in the past (Lycett et al. 2010).
Unfortunately, genetic studies do not provide precise information on the
timing of past bottlenecks and expansions, with date estimates varying
widely depending on the model and calibration points used (Stone et al.
2010). However, archaeological evidence for origins and changes in
primate cultural traditions could assist in narrowing the error margins
of genetic studies, as it has done for calibrations of the human
mitochondrial 'clock' (Soares et al. 2009).
Each of the chimpanzee subspecies may have been genetically
distinct for longer than H. sapiens has existed as a species (Wegmann
& Excoffier 2010; but see Gonder et al. 2011), which leaves a
significant amount of time over which any cumulative effects of cultural
innovation may have operated among the panins. Analysis of material
culture left by primate populations that were larger and better
connected than those of today would provide a much needed comparison to
short-term laboratory-based studies that suggest chimpanzees do not
accumulate modifications (Tennie et al. 2009). In particular, the
long-term perspective provided by archaeology allows for assessment of
whether panin accumulative processes operate on a slower scale than has
been observable in the few decades of chimpanzee research.
Genetic data show that Central African chimpanzee populations have
the largest current effective population size, and that unlike the
western and eastern subspecies they did not go through a substantial
population bottleneck in the Pleistocene (Wegmann & Excoffier 2010).
The strong bottleneck effects seen for the western and eastern
populations, as well as for the bonobos, might suggest that central
populations are the ones to look to for cultural continuities and
accumulation over the past few million years (Sanz & Morgan 2010).
However, P. t. troglodytes have received less intensive field study than
the western and eastern populations; for the 19 (out of 39) cultural
variants identified by Whiten et al. (2001) that involve tool-use, 11
have insufficient data for assessing their presence among one or both of
the Central African Goualougo and Lope communities (Lycett et al. 2010).
It is therefore not possible on present evidence to identify whether the
presence of larger populations over longer time periods has had an
impact on the innovation and cultural retention rates of modern
chimpanzees. Nevertheless, a reasonable prediction based on demography
would be that cultural innovation rates were consistently highest among
past Central African panins, with cultural founder effects active among
the diverging western and then eastern chimpanzee subspecies from around
500 000 years ago.
Discussion: a primate archaeological perspective
The three aspects of primate behaviour considered briefly here,
diet, geographic variation and innovation, were chosen to point up the
benefits of bringing an archaeological approach to primatology (Haslam
et al. 2009). This approach improves our understanding of primate
behavioural evolution in its own right, complementing studies of
psychological and manual skills (e.g. Byrne 2005), in addition to
generating comparative data for the study of human evolution. The
reviewed topics are not mutually exclusive, since geographical
expansions may stimulate or derive from innovations, and new innovations
that relate to diet may be preferentially taken up by other individuals
and groups (Nishida et al. 2009). It is also important to remember that,
even among humans, seemingly adaptive behaviours can be lost (Henrich
2004). Without knowing just how chequered the temporal pattern of
cultural variation may be (Wrangham 2006), we should not automatically
regard living tool-using primates (or indeed any cultural species) as
representative of their entire ancestral lineages. For example, based on
emerging genetic data, I have hypothesised that stone tool-use observed
in Brazilian capuchin monkeys may be no more than 700 000 years old, and
the same behaviour among West African chimpanzees perhaps little more
than 500 000 years old. But across these taxa (and others) there may
have been numerous instances of technological discovery, loss and
re-discovery over that time period, dependent on demography and the
availability of suitable tools and dietary targets. Now that the
conceptual constraint of a lone Homo-Pan stone tool-using clade is
abandoned, a key task is to examine the independent technological
trajectory of each tool-using group.
The idea of cross-referencing hominin and primate activities is not
new in archaeology, but directly investigating the activities of past
primates does involve a shift away from humans and our ancestors as the
sole focus of prehistoric study. In practice, the methods required
differ little from those used in Palaeolithic archaeology. Surveys are
necessary to determine the prevalence and distribution of tool
materials, and their geographical intersection with habitats suitable
for resources such as palm nuts and social insects, regardless of
whether these are currently used by extant primates. The Brazilian
cerrado and caatinga zones certainly hold promise in this regard, as
does the Dahomey Gap savannah that separates the Upper and Lower Guinea
rainforests (Mayr & O'Hara 1986) (Figure 2). Forest expansion
in the latter area is linked to an increase in the pioneer oil palm
Elaeis guineensis (Salzmann & Hoelzmann 2005), favoured for
nut-cracking and pestle-pounding by the extant Bossou chimpanzees of
Guinea (Sugiyama 1994).
Excavations are needed both within and outside the current range of
tool-using primate species, especially in areas where environmental
reconstructions indicate the previous existence of suitable habitats
(e.g. Dupont et al. 2010). Information on changes to major river
systems, as barriers to both gene and information flow, may assist in
setting temporal limits to cultural processes. Initial dating programs
also need to target durable, culturally relevant materials such as
broken nut shells. Use-wear and residue analyses will continue to be
valuable in distinguishing primate tools from natural objects, including
use-wear on concentrations of broken stone debris that may reveal tool
re-use behaviours akin to the early stages of hominin stone knapping.
Key plant residues include starches found in nuts and USOs, the lipids
and resins found in these same sources, and fragments of nut shells and
structural tissue pounded into tool surfaces. Animal residues may
include hair (such as the bushbaby hair found adhering to a chimpanzee
spear in Senegal: Pruetz & Bertolani 2007), termite and ant parts,
fats and marrow (Boesch & Boesch 1989), blood and by-products such
as honey (Boesch et al. 2009; Sanz & Morgan 2009).
Realistically, the initial age estimates provided in this review
for the origins of extant primate stone tool-use will require revision.
These estimates are partly based on genetic dates, which have wide error
margins, and in any case population genetic divergence cannot by default
be assumed to correlate with material culture changes in any animal
(including hominins). Finally, we should remember that hominins have
filled the niche of cognitively advanced, large bodied, tool-using
primate for the past few million years, which probably had a negative
impact on the evolution of similar roles among our close relatives.
Debatably, this impact may have included intermittent hominin occupation
of African forests from the Middle Pleistocene onwards (Mercader 2002).
It is possible that we and our ancestors are the reason modern
chimpanzees display limited rates of cultural evolution, and it is
likely that the survival of extant primate species in Africa is to some
extent a function of their ability to co-exist with hominins (Jablonski
2002). Outside Africa, the Asian macaques also have a long shared
history with Homo, while in the Americas a lack of Pleistocene hominins
may have permitted greater primate technological experimentation. Our
ability to address issues such as these will strongly benefit from an
archaeological approach, and an ability to see beyond behaviourally
modern primates will offer new perspectives on both the relevance of
primatological referents to human evolution, and on the variability and
time depth of non-human primate behaviour.
Acknowledgements
I am grateful to Dora Biro, Bill McGrew, Susana Carvalho,
Elisabetta Visalberghi, Dorothy Fragaszy and three anonymous reviewers
for discussions and helpful comments on earlier drafts. I would also
like to thank all the researchers working on the evolution of primate
technology, on whose insightful work this review draws. The author
received funding from a UK Arts and Humanities Research Council Early
Career Fellowship, and European Research Council Starting Grant No.
283959 (PRIMARCH).
References
AIELLO, L. & C. DEAN. 1990. An introduction to human
evolutionary anatomy. London: Academic Press.
BAR-YOSEF, O. 2002. The Upper Palaeolithic revolution. Annual
Review of Anthropology 31: 363-93.
BECQUET, C., N. PATTERSON, A. STONE, M. PRZEWORSKI & D. REICH.
2007. Genetic structure of chimpanzee populations. PLoS Genetics 3: e66.
BIRO, D., S. CARVALHO & T. MATSUZAWA. 2010. Tools, traditions,
and technologies: interdisciplinary approaches to chimpanzee nut
cracking, in E. Lonsdorf, S. Ross & T. Matsuzawa (ed.) The mind of
the chimpanzee: ecological and experimental perspectives: 141-55.
Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.
BJORK, A., W. LIU, J. WERTHEIM, B. HAHN & M. WOROBEY. 2011.
Evolutionary history of chimpanzees inferred from complete mitochondrial
genomes. Molecular Biology and Evolution 28: 615-23.
BOESCH, C. 2007. What makes us human (Homo sapiens)? The challenge
of cognitive cross-species comparison. Journal of Comparative Psychology
121 : 227-40.
BOESCH, C. & H. BOESCH. 1982. Optimization of nut-cracking with
natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 83: 265-86.
--1984. Mental map in wild chimpanzees: an analysis of hammer
transports for nut cracking. Primates 25: 160-70.
--1989. Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Tai National
Park. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 78: 547-73.
--1990. Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica 54: 86-99.
BOESCH, C. & H. BOESCH-ACHERMANN. 2000. The chimpanzees of the
Tai Forest. Behavioural ecology and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
BOESCH, C., J. HEAD & M. ROBBINS. 2009. Complex tool sets for
honey extraction among chimpanzees in Loango National Park, Gabon.
Journal of Human Evolution 56: 560-69.
BREUER, T., M. NDOUNDOU-HOCKEMBA & V. FISHLOCK. 2005. First
observation of tool use in wild gorillas. PLoS Biology 3(11): e380.
BUTYNSKI, T. 2003. The robust chimpanzee Pan troglodytes: taxonomy,
distribution, abundance, and conservation status, in R. Kormos, C.
Boesch, M. Bakarr & T. Butynski (ed.) West African chimpanzees.
Status survey and conservation action plan: 5-12. Cambridge: IUCN.
BYRNE, R. 2005. The maker not the tool: the cognitive significance
of great ape manual skills, in V. Roux & B. Bril (ed.) Stone
knapping: the necessary conditions for a uniquely hominid behaviour.
159-69. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.
CANALE, G.R., C.E. GUIDORIZZI, M.C.M. KIERULFF & C.A.F.R.
GATTO. 2009. First record of tool use by wild populations of the
yellow-breasted capuchin monkey (Cebus xanthosternos) and new records
for the bearded capuchin (Cebus libidinosus). American Journal of
Primatology 71 : 366-72.
CARVALHO, S., E. CUNHO, C. SOUSA & T. MATSUZAWA. 2008. Chaines
operatoires and resource-exploitation strategies in chimpanzee (Pan
troglodytes) nut cracking. Journal of Human Evolution 55: 148-63.
CARVALHO, S., D. BIRO, W. C. McGREW & T. MATSUZAWA. 2009.
Tool-composite reuse in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes):
archaeologically invisible steps in the technological evolution of early
hominins? Animal Cognition 12: S103-S114.
CASADO, F., C. BONVINCINO, C. NAGLE, B. COMAS, T. MANZUR, M. LAHOS
& H. SEUANEZ. 2010. Mitochondrial divergence between 2 populations
of the hooded capuchin, Cebus (Sapajus) cay (Platyrrhini, Primates).
Journal of Heredity 101: 261-69.
CASWELL, J., S. MALLICK, D. RICHTER, J. NEUBAUER, C. SCHIRMER, S.
GNERRE & D. REICH. 2008. Analysis of chimpanzee history based on
genome sequence alignments. PLoS Genetics 4(4): el 000057.
CLARK, J.D. 1960. Human ecology during Pleistocene and later times
in Africa south of the Sahara. Current Anthropology 1:307-324.
CONARD, N. 2008. A critical view of the evidence for a southern
African origin of behavioural modernity. South African Archaeological
Society Goodwin Series 10: 175-79.
D'ERRICO, F. 2003. The invisible frontier. A multiple species
model for the origin of behavioral modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology
12: 188-202.
D'ERRICO, F. & C. STRINGER. 2011. Evolution, revolution or
saltation scenario for the emergence of modern cultures? Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society B 366: 1060-69.
DE LA TORRE, I. 2010. Insights on the technical competence of the
early Oldowan, in A. Nowell & I. Davidson (ed.) Stone took and the
evolution of human cognition: 45-65. Boulder (CO): University Press of
Colorado.
DEBLAUWE, I., P. GUISLAIN, J. DUPAIN & L. VAN ELSACKER. 2006.
Use of a tool-set by Pan troglodytes troglodytes to obtain termites
(Macrotermes) in the periphery of the Dja Biosphere Reserve, southeast
Cameroon. American Journal of Primatology 68: 1191-96.
DUPONT, L., S. JAHNS, F. MARRET & S. NING. 2010. Vegetation
change in equatorial West Africa: time-slices for the last 150 ka.
Palaeogeography, Palaeoclimatology, Palaeoecology 155:95-122.
FOLEY, R. 1987. Another unique species. New York: Longman
Scientific & Technical.
--1993. African terrestrial primates: the comparative evolutionary
biology of Theropithecus and hominids, in N. Jahlonski (ed.)
Theropithecus--the rise and fall of a primate genus: 245-70. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
FOWLER, A. & V. SOMMER. 2007. Subsistence technology of
Nigerian chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology 28: 997-1023.
GAGNEUX, P., M. GONDER, T. GOLDBERG & P. MORIN. 2001. Gene flow
in wild chimpanzee populations: what genetic data tell us about
chimpanzee movement over space and time. Philosophical Transactions of
the Royal Society B 356: 889-97.
GAMBLE, C., J.A.J. GOWLETT & R. DUNBAR. 2011. The social brain
and the shape of the Palaeolithic. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 21 :
115-35.
GOEBEL, T., M.R. WATERS & D. O'ROURKE. 2008. The Late
Pleistocene dispersal of modern humans in the Americas. Science 319:
1497-502.
GOLDBERG, T. 1998. Biogeographic predictors of genetic diversity in
populations of eastern African chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes
schweinfurthi). International Journal of Primatology 19: 237-54.
GONDER, M., S. LOCATELLI, L. GHOBRIAL, M. MITCHELL, J. KUJAWSKI, F.
LANKESTER, C.-B. STEWART & S. TISHKOFF. 2011. Evidence from Cameroon
reveals differences in the genetic structure and histories of chimpanzee
populations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA
108: 4766-71.
GOWLETT, J.A.J. 2009. Artefacts of apes, humans, and others:
towards comparative assessment and analysis. Journal of Human Evolution
57:401-410.
GUMERT, M., M. KLUCK & S. MALAIVIJITNOND. 2009. The physical
characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers
used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand.
American Journal of Primatology 71: 594-608.
GUMERT, M., L.K. HOONG & S. MALAIVIJITNOND. 2011. Sex
differences in the stone tool-use behavior of a wild population of
Burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea). American
Journal of Primatology 73:1-11.
HASLAM, M., A. HERNANDEZ-AGUILAR, V. LING, S. CARVALHO, I. DE LA
TORRE, A. DESTEFANO, A. DU, B.L. HARDY, J. HARRIS, L. MARCHANT, T.
MATSUZAWA, W. McGREW, J. MERCARDER, R. MORA, M. PETRAGLIA, H. ROCHE, E.
VISALBERGHI & R. WARREN. 2009. Primate archaeology. Nature 460:
339-44.
HASLAM, M., S. CARVALHO, A. CROWTHER & T. MATSUZAWA. 2010.
Residue analysis of modern and ancient chimpanzee pounding tools from
Bossou and Diecke, Guinea: defining a primate archaeological signature.
Paper presented at the 23rd International Primatological Society
Congress, Kyoto.
HAYASHI, M., Y. MIZUNO & T. MATSUZAWA. 2005. How does
stone-tool use emerge? Introduction of stones and nuts to naive
chimpanzees in captivity. Primates 46: 91-102.
HENRICH, J. 2004. Demography and cultural evolution: how adaptive
cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses: the Tasmanian case.
American Antiquity 69: 197-214.
HENSHILWOOD, C.S. & C.W. MAREAN. 2003. The origin of modern
human behavior. Current Anthropology 44: 627-51.
HERNANDEZ-AGUILAR, A. 2009. Chimpanzee nest distribution and site
reuse in a dry habitat: implications for early hominin ranging. Journal
of Human Evolution 57: 350-64.
HERNANDEZ-AGUILAR, A., J. MOORE & T. PICKERING. 2007. Savanna chimpanzees use tools to harvest the underground storage organs of
plants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 104:
19210-13.
HEY, J. 2010. The divergence of chimpanzee species and subspecies
as revealed in multipopulation isolation-with-migration analyses.
Molecular Biology and Evolution 27: 921-33.
HOHMANN, G. & B. FRUTH. 2003. Culture in bonobos?
Between-species and within-species variation in behavior. Current
Anthropology 44: 563-71.
HOLLOWAY, R. 1969. Culture: a human domain. Current Anthropology
10: 395-412.
HOORN, C., F. WESSELINGH, H. TER STEEGE, M. BERMUDEZ, A. MORA, J.
SEVINK, I. SANMARTIN, A. SANCHEZ-MESEGUER, C. ANDERSON, J. FIGUEIREDO,
C. JARAMILLO, D. RIFF, F. NEGRI, H. HOOGHIEMSTRA, J. LUNDBERG, T.
STADLER, T. SARKINEN & A. ANTONELLI. 2010. Amazonia through time:
Andean uplift, climate change, landscape evolution, and biodiversity.
Science 330: 927-31.
ISAAC, G.L. 1969. Studies of early culture in East Africa. World
Archaeology 1: 1-28.
JABLONSKI, N. 2002. The fossil record of Old World monkeys: the
Late Neogene radiation, in W. Hartwig (ed.) The primate fossil record:
255-99. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
JOLLY, C. 1970. The seed-eaters: a new model of hominid
differentiation based on a baboon analogy. Man 5: 5-26.
JOULIAN, F. 1996. Comparing chimpanzee and early hominid
techniques: some contributions to cultural and cognitive questions, in
P. Mellars & K. Gibson (ed.) Modelling the early human mind: 173-89.
Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.
KLEIN, R.G. 2000. Archeology and the evolution of human behavior.
Evolutionary Anthropology 9: 17-36.
KOOPS, K., WC. McGREW & T. MATSUZAWA. 2010. Do chimpanzees (Pan
troglodytes) use cleavers and anvils to fracture Treculia africana
fruits? Preliminary data on a new form of percussive technology.
Primates 51: 175-78.
KUMAR, S., A. FILIPSKI, V. SWARNA, A. WALKER & S.B. HEDGES.
2005. Placing confidence limits on the molecular age of the
human-chimpanzee divergence. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences of the USA 102: 18842-47.
LADEN, G. & R. WRANGHAM. 2005. The rise of the hominids as an
adaptive shift in fallback foods: plant underground storage organs
(USOs) and australopith origins. Journal of Human Evolution 49: 482-98.
LEAKEY, L. 1961. The progress and evolution of Man in Africa.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
LEAVENS, D., K. BARD & W. HOPKINS. 2010. BIZARRE chimpanzees do
not represent 'the chimpanzee'. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33: 100-101.
LEHMAN, S. & J. FLEAGLE. 2006. Biogeography and primates: a
review, in S. Lehman & J. Fleagle (ed.) Primate biogeography: 1-58.
New York: Springer.
LIND, J. & P. LINDENFORS. 2010. The number of cultural traits
is correlated with female group size but not with male group size in
chimpanzee communities. PLoS One 5:e9241.
LYCETT, S., M. COLLARD & W. C. McGREW. 2009. Cladistic analyses
of behavioural variation in wild Pan troglodytes: exploring the
chimpanzee culture hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution 57: 339-47.
LYCETT, S., M. COLLARD & W.C. McGREW. 2010. Are behavioral
differences among wild chimpanzee communities genetic or cultural? An
assessment using tool-use data and phylogenetic methods. American
Journal of Physical Anthropology 142: 461-67.
LYNCH ALFARO, J., J. BOUBLI, L. OLSON, A. DI FIORE, B. WILSON, G.
GUTIERREZ-ESPELETA, K. CHIOU, M. SCHULTE, S. NEITZEL, V. ROSS, D.
SCHWOCHOW, M. NGUYEN, I. FARIAS, C. JANSON & M. ALFARO. 2011.
Explosive Pleistocene range expansion leads to widespread Amazonian
sympatry between robust and gracile capuchin monkeys. Journal of
Biogeography. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2699.2011.02609.x.
MALAIVIJITNOND, S., C. LEKPRAYOON, N. TANDAVANITTJ, S. PANHA, C.
CHEEWATHAM & Y. HAMADA. 2007. Stone-tool usage by Thai long-tailed
macaques (Macaca fascicularis). American Journal of Primatology 69:
227-33.
MANNU, M. & E. OTTONI. 2009. The enhanced tool-kit of two
groups of wild bearded capuchin monkeys in the Caatinga: tool making,
associative use, and secondary tools. American Journal of Primatology 71
: 242-51.
MARCHANT, L.F. & W.C. McGREW. 2005. Percussive technology:
chimpanzee baobab smashing and the evolutionary modeling of hominid
knapping, in V. Roux & B. Bril (ed.) Stone knapping: the necessary
conditions for a uniquely hominin behaviour 341-50. Cambridge: McDonald
Institute for Archaeological Research.
MARSHALL-PESCINI, S. & A. WHITEN. 2008a. Chimpanzees (Pan
troglodytes) and the question of cumulative culture: an experimental
approach. Animal Cognition 11: 449-56.
--2008b. Social learning of nut-cracking behavior in East African
sanctuary-living chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Journal
of Comparative Psychology 122: 186-94.
MAYR, E. & R.J. O'HARA. 1986. The biogeographic evidence
supporting the Pleistocene forest refuge hypothesis. Evolution 40:
55-67.
McBREARTY, S. & A. BROOKS. 2000. The revolution that
wasn't: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human
behavior. Journal of Human Evolution 39: 453-563.
McBREARTY, S. & N. JABLONSKI. 2005. First fossil chimpanzee.
Nature 437: 105-108.
McGREW, W.C. 1992. Chimpanzee material culture: implications for
human evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
--2001. The other faunivory: primate insectivory and early human
diet, in C. Stanford & H. Bunn (ed.) Meat-eating and human
evolution: 160-78. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
--2004. The cultured chimpanzee: reflections on cultural
primatology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
--2010. In search of the last common ancestor: new findings on wild
chimpanzees. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 365:
3267-76.
McGREW, W.C., P. BALDWIN & C. TUTIN. 1988. Diet of wild
chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Mt. Assirik, Senegal: I.
Composition. American Journal of Primatology 16:213-26.
McGREW, W.C., R.M. HAM, L.J.T. WHITE, C. TUTIN & M. FERNANDEZ.
1997. Why don't chimpanzees in Gabon crack nuts? International
Journal of Primatology 18: 353-74.
McPHERRON, S.P., Z. ALEMSEGED, C.W. MAREAN, J. WYNN, D. REED, D.
GERAADS, R. BOBE & H. BHARAT. 2010. Evidence for stone-tool-assisted
consumption of animal tissues before 3.39 million years ago at Dikika,
Ethiopia. Nature 466: 857-60.
MELLARS, P. & C. STRINGER (ed.). 1989. The human revolution:
behavioral and biological perspectives on the origins of modern humans.
Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
MERCADER, J. 2002. Forest people: the role of African rainforests
in human evolution and dispersal. Evolutionary Anthropology 11:117-24.
MERCADER, J., M. PANGER & C. BOESCH. 2002. Excavation of a
chimpanzee stone tool site in the African rainforest. Science 296:
1452-55.
MERCADER, J., H. BARTON, J. GILLESPIE, J. HARRIS, S. KUHN, R.T.
TYLER & C. BOESCH. 2007. 4,300-year-old chimpanzee sites and the
origins of percussive stone technology. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the USA 104(9): 3043-48.
MILTON, K. 1999. Nutritional characteristics of wild primate foods:
do the diets of our closest living relatives have lessons for us?
Nutrition 15: 488-98.
MINUGH-PURVIS, N. 1995. The modern human origins controversy:
1984-1994. Evolutionary Anthropology 4: 140-47.
MITANI, J. 2006. Demographic influences on the behavior of
chimpanzees. Primates 47:6-13.
MORGAN, B. & E. ABWE. 2006. Chimpanzees use stone hammers in
Cameroon. Current Biology 16: R632-R33.
MOURA, A. & P. LEE. 2004. Capuchin stone tool use in caatinga
dry forest. Science 306:1909.
NISHIDA, T., T. MATSUSAKA & W.C. McGREW. 2009. Emergence,
propagation or disappearance of novel behavioral patterns in the
habituated chimpanzees of Mahale: a review. Primates 50: 23-36.
NISHIDA, T., K. ZAMMA, T. MATSUSAKA, A. INABA & W.C. McGREW.
2010. Chimpanzee behavior in the wild: an audio-visual encyclopedia.
Tokyo: Springer.
OTTONI, E. & P. IZAR. 2008. Capuchin monkey tool use: overview
and implications. Evolutionary Anthropology 17: 171-78.
PANGER, M., A. BROOKS, B.C. RICHMOND & B. WOOD. 2002. Older
than the Oldowan? Rethinking the emergence of hominin tool use.
Evolutionary Anthropology 11: 235-45.
PARKER, S. & K. GIBSON. 1979. A developmental model for the
evolution of language and intelligence in early hominids. Behavioral and
Brain Sciences 2: 367-408.
POTTS, R. 2007. Environmental hypotheses of Pliocene human
evolution, in R. Bobe, Z. Alemseged & A. K. Behrensmeyer (ed.)
Hominin environments in the East African Pliocene: an assessment of the
faunal evidence: 25-49. Dordrecht: Springer.
POWELL, A., S. SHENNAN & M. THOMAS. 2009. Late Pleistocene
demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324:
1298-301.
PRUETZ, J. & P. BERTOLANI. 2007. Savanna chimpanzees, Pan
troglodytes verus, hunt with tools. Current Biology 17: 412-17.
--2009. Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) behavioral responses to
stresses associated with living in a savanna-mosaic environment:
implications for hominin adaptations to open habitats. PaleoAnthropology
2009: 252-62.
RENFREW, C. 2007. Prehistory: making of the human mind. London:
Weidenfeld & Nicolson.
RICHERSON, P.J., R. BOYD & R.L. BETTINGER. 2009. Cultural
innovations and demographic change. Human Biology 81:211-35.
SALZMANN, U. & P. HOELZMANN. 2005. The Dahomey Gap: an abrupt
climatically induced rain forest fragmentation in West Africa during the
late Holocene. The Holocene 15: 190-99.
SANZ, C. & D. MORGAN. 2009. Flexible and persistent tool-using
strategies in honey-gathering by wild chimpanzees. International Journal
of Primatology 30:411-27.
--2010. The complexity of chimpanzee tool-use behaviors, in E.
Lonsdorf, S. Ross, T. Matsuzawa & J. Goodall (ed.) The mind of the
chimpanzee: ecological and experimental perspectives: 127-40. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
SANZ, C., D. MORGAN & S. GULICK. 2004. New insights into
chimpanzees, tools and termites from the Congo Basin. American
Naturalist 164:567-81.
SANZ, C., J. CALL & D. MORGAN. 2009. Design complexity in
termite-fishing tools of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Biology Letters
5: 293-96.
SEMAW, S., M.J. ROGERS, J. QUADE, P. RENNE, R.F. BUTLER, M.
DOMINGUEZ-RODRIGO, D. STOUT, W.S. HART, T. PICKERING & S.W. SIMPSON.
2003. 2.6-million-year-old stone tools and associated bones from OGS-6
and OGS-7, Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. Journal of Human Evolution 45: 169-77.
SEPT, J. 1998. Shadows on a changing landscape: comparing nesting
patterns of hominids and chimpanzees since their last common ancestor.
American Journal of Primatology 46: 85-101.
SEPT, J. & G.E. BROOKS. 1994. Reports of chimpanzee natural
history, including tool use, in 16th- and 17th-century Sierra Leone.
International Journal of Primatology 15: 867-78.
SHEA, J.J. 2011. Homo sapiens is as Homo sapiens was. Behavioral
variability versus 'behavioral modernity' in Paleolithic
archaeology. Current Anthropology 52: 1-35.
SHENNAN, S. 2001. Demography and cultural innovation: a model and
its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Cambridge
Archaeological Journal 11: 5-16.
SHUMAKER, R., K. WALKUP & B. BECK. 2011. Animal tool behavior:
the use and manufacture of tools by animals. Baltimore (MD): John
Hopkins University Press.
SILK, J. 2011. The path to sociality. Nature 479: 182-83.
SOARES, P., L. ERMINI, N. THOMSON, M. MORMINA, T. RITO, A. ROHL, A.
SALAS, S. OPPENHEIMER & M. RICHARDS. 2009. Correcting for purifying
selection: an improved human mitochondrial molecular clock. American
Journal of Human Genetics 84: 740-59.
SOUTO, A., C. BIONE, M. BASTOS, B. BEZERRA, D. FRAGASZY & N.
SCHIEL. 2011. Critically endangered blonde capuchins fish for termites
and use new techniques to accomplish the task. Biology Letters. doi:
10.1098/rsbl.2011.0034.
SPAGNOLETTI, N., E. VISALBERGHI, E. OTTONI, P. IZAR & D.
FRAGASZY. 2011. Stone tool use by adult wild bearded capuchin monkeys
(Cebus libidinosus). Frequency, efficiency and tool selectivity. Journal
of Human Evolution 61: 97-107.
STEFFEN, W., J. GRINEVALD, P. CRUTZEN & J. McNEILL. 2011. The
Anthropocene: conceptual and historical perspectives. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society A 369: 842-67.
STEWART, F., A. PIEL & W.C. McGREW. 2011. Living archaeology:
artefacts of specific nest site fidelity in wild chimpanzees. Journal of
Human Evolution 61 : 388-95.
STONE, A., F. BATTISTUZZI, L. KUBATKO, G.H. PERRY, E. TRUDEAU, H.
LIN & S. KUMAR. 2010. More reliable estimates of divergence times in
Pan using complete mtDNA sequences and accounting for population
structure. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 365:
3277-88.
SUGIYAMA, Y. 1994. Tool use by wild chimpanzees. Nature 367: 327.
TENNIE, C., J. CALL & M. TOMASELLO. 2009. Ratcheting up the
ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society B 364:2405-15.
THIEME, H. 1997. Lower Palaeolithic hunting spears from Germany.
Nature 385: 807-10.
TOTE, N. & K. SCHICK. 2009. The Oldowan: the tool making of
early hominins and chimpanzees compared. Annual Review of Anthropology
38: 289-305.
TWEHEYO, M., K. LYE & R. WELADJI. 2004. Chimpanzee diet and
habitat selection in the Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda. Forest Ecology
and Management 188: 267-78.
UNGAR, P. 1998. Dental allometry, morphology, and wear as evidence
for diet in fossil primates. Evolutionary Anthropology 6: 205-17.
UNGAR, P. & M. SPONHEIMER. 2011. The diets of early hominins.
Science 334:190-93.
VAN SCHAIK, C., E. Fox & A. SITOMPUL. 1996. Manufacture and use
of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans. Naturwissenschaften 83: 186-88.
VAN SCHAIK, C., R. DEANER & M. MERRILL. 1999. The conditions
for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material
culture. Journal of Human Evolution 36: 719-41.
VAN SCHAIK, C., M. ANCRENAZ, G. BORGEN, B. GALDIKAS, C. KNOTT, I.
SINGLETON, A. SUZUKI, S.S. UTAMI & M. MERRILL. 2003. Orangutan
cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science 299: 102-105.
VISALBERGHI, E., D. FRAGASZY, E. OTTONI, P. IZAR, M. DE OLIVEIRA
& F. ANDRADE. 2007. Characteristics of hammer stones and anvils used
by wild bearded capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) to crack open palm
nuts. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 132: 426-44.
VISALBERGHI, E., E. ADDESSI, V. TRUPPA, N. SPAGNOLETTI, E. OTFONI,
P. IZAR & D. FRAGASZY. 2009. Selection of effective stone tools by
wild bearded capuchin monkeys. Current Biology 19: 213-17.
VRBA, E. 2000. Major features of neogene mammalian evolution in
Africa, in T. Partridge & R. Maud (ed.) Cenozoic geology of southern
Africa: 277-304. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
WADLEY, L. 2001. What is cultural modernity? A general view and a
South African perspective from Rose Cottage Cave. Cambridge
Archaeological Journal 11: 201-21.
WASHBURN, S. & I. DEVORE. 1961. Social behavior of baboons and
early man, in S. Washburn (ed.) Social life of early Man: 91-105. New
York: Wenner-Gren Foundation for Anthropological Research.
WEGMANN, D. & L. EXCOFFIER. 2010. Bayesian inference of the
demographic history of chimpanzees. Molecular Biology and Evolution 27:
1425-35.
WHITEN, A. 2011. The scope of culture in chimpanzees, humans and
ancestral apes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B
366:997-1007.
WHITEN, A., J. GOODALL, W.C. McGREW, T. NISHIDA, V. REYNLDS, Y.
SUGIYAMA, C. TUTIN, R. WRANGHAM & C. BOESCH. 1999. Cultures in
chimpanzees. Nature 399: 682-85.
--2001. Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour 138:
1481-516.
WHITEN, A., K. SCHICK & N. TOTH. 2009. The evolution and
cultural transmission of percussive technology: integrating evidence
from palaeoanthropology and primatology. Journal of Human Evolution 57:
420-35.
WON, Y.-J. & J. HEY. 2005. Divergence population genetics of
chimpanzees. Molecular Biology and Evolution 22: 297-307.
WOOD, B. & T. HARRISON. 2011. The evolutionary context of the
first hominins. Nature 470: 347-52.
WRANGHAM, R. 2006. Chimpanzees: the culture-zone concept becomes
untidy. Current Biology 16: R634-R635.
WYNN, T.G., A. HERNANDEZ-AGUILAR, L.F. MARCHANT & W.C. McGREW.
2011. 'An ape's view of the Oldowan' revisited.
Evolutionary Anthropology 20: 181-97.
YU, N., M. JENSEN-SEAMAN, L. CHEMNICK, J. KIDD, A. DEINARD, O.
RYDER, K. KIDD & W.-H. LI. 2003. Low nucleotide diversity in
chimpanzees and bonobos. Genetics 164:1511-18.
Received: 6 May 2011; Accepted: 6 October 2011; Revised: 8 December
2011
Michael Haslam, Research Laboratory for Archaeology and the History
of Art, University of Oxford, Dyson Perrins Building, South Parks Road,
Oxford OX1 3QY, UK (Email: michael.haslam@rlaha.ox.ac.uk)