Sex, symmetry and silliness in the bifacial world.
Hayden, Brian ; Villeneuve, Suzanne
After 10 years of pursuing sexy handaxes it is probably time to put
these coquettish creatures to bed. Readers wishing to continue the
debate are courteously directed to our Project Gallery
As lithic technologists, we were at first amused by Kohn and
Mithen's (1999) arguments to the effect that handaxes developed in
order to enhance sexual selection among early hominins. However,
contributors to Antiquity continue to take their suggestions seriously
and even to extend them to new heights (Machin 2008; Mithen 2008;
Hodgson 2009). We would like to bring the issues back down to the
ground, and discuss the practical and technological aspects of biface
manufacture which these authors do not seem to have appreciated. This is
perhaps not surprising since none of the authors appear to have much
experience in manufacturing stone tools. We do agree with them that a
socially-oriented analysis of stone tools is desirable, but this must be
founded on a solid understanding of lithic technology.
Kohn and Mithen's (1999: 520) original argument, as with
subsequent versions published in Antiquity, is based on the fundamental
assertion that 'in the majority of artefacts a specific symmetrical
form was imposed ... an imposed symmetry beyond functional
requirements.' This assertion was supported by a study of the
effectiveness of symmetrical versus asymmetrical handaxe performance in
butchering (Machin et al. 2006). Mithen (2008: 766-9) continues to claim
that his sexual selection explanation is the only theory that can
account for the features typical of handaxes. One hesitates to accept
these claims at face value when also confronted with highly questionable
assertions to the effect that bones were used as billets and handaxes
were used for chopping up vegetables. The same fundamental premise has
been uncritically adopted by subsequent commentators who propose
variously that consistency of form in handaxes was the product of
natural selection (for unstated reasons--Machin 2008: 763); or that
symmetry evolved among animals in general as a means of identifying
things in the world and that handaxe symmetry evolved to create
reassurance that an unspecified critical aspect of the human perceptual
system was working appropriately (Hodgson 2009). Machin subsequently
presented cogent arguments from a Darwinian viewpoint as to why handaxes
would make poor candidates for use in sexual selection.
What Kohn and Mithen fail to acknowledge is that there is a
practical technological logic behind the symmetry and other
characteristics of bifaces. This does not appear to be very evident from
cognitive evolutionary perspectives. What we suggest is that analysing
handaxe symmetry from a design theory viewpoint can be much more
insightful. Design theory involves: 1) the identification of a specific
problem to be solved; 2) the identification of relevant constraints
(typically factors such as material costs and availability, performance
characteristics, required skill, processing volumes, time availability,
existing technology, mobility and transport limitations, and available
labour); and 3) identifying the trade-offs in advantages and
disadvantages of certain constraints (such as material procurement
costs) against the advantages and disadvantages in other constraints
(such as effectiveness). These factors are then used to develop a suite
of design solutions (Pye 1964, 1968; Horsfall 1987; Hayden 1998; Hayden
et al. 2000).
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
In order to demonstrate the potential utility of design theory, the
issue of handaxe symmetry can be considered in the broader context of
general trends throughout the Stone Age toward greater economy in the
use of lithic material employed in high-consumption activities. Greater
material economy is manifested by changes in core reduction techniques
and resharpening techniques; and these, in turn, arguably reflect
changes in major design constraints. Oldowan choppers/cores, for
example, produce a minimal number of flakes or resharpenings per
kilogram of tool stone due to the hard hammer techniques employed. The
large striking platform remnants that result rapidly reduce the volume
of core material and increase edge angles (Figure 1a & b). Handaxes,
on the other hand, are made with billets which enable knappers to
resharpen tools many more times and to remove many more flakes from a
given mass of raw material due to the much smaller striking platform
remnants in relation to flake sizes (Figure 1c). Reduction of cores
using blade techniques extends the amount of cutting edge per kilogram
of material even further (Sheets & Muto 1972) as does the production
and use of microliths. Pressure flaking, edge grinding, and the use of
resharpenable metals all extend the lives of cutting tools into the
realms of curated tools that can last for years or even generations.
Each of the technological developments mentioned exhibits symmetry,
requires skill, training, and often special raw materials or auxiliary
tools that go far beyond the simple cutting requirements of daily
routines. Are these developments all to be viewed as sexual or costly
signalling rather than basic technological adaptations to underlying
material and performance constraints?
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
We suggest that the handaxe can be viewed as a specialised tool
design ultimately responding to increased processing and time
constraints. Torrence (1989) and Bleed (1986) have argued that time
constraints (the need to complete certain activities within a limited
amount of time) may explain why specialised tools were developed. Under
conditions of time stress, such as the need to butcher and dry thousands
of salmon during short spawning runs, or the need to butcher and remove
meat from large game before packs of predators arrive, one cannot afford
to interrupt harvesting or cutting activities because of tool exhaustion
and wander off in search of materials (often several kilometres distant)
to replace what has been used up. From a design theory perspective, it
can also be argued that as processing volumes increase, the development
of specialised tools becomes more cost-effective even though they
usually involve increased procurement/manufacturing costs, skills,
and special manufacturing equipment (Hayden & Gargett 1988; Hayden
1989). This is a relationship that has been well-established by
engineers for industrial machinery (Figure 2; see also Bell 1972; Zeyher
1977; Gold 1979) but has also been documented in technologies as simple
as fish butchering knives (Figure 3; see also Hayden & Gargett
1988). And it was most likely processing volume constraints that were
changing most significantly throughout the Palaeolithic and Neolithic
(Hayden 1989). At this basic level, the same fundamental technological
relationships may be as applicable to the Palaeolithic as they are to
recent centuries.
During the Palaeolithic and Neolithic, the greater the processing
volumes the more inefficient it would have been to discontinue a suite
of processing activities in order to obtain replacement raw materials
for exhausted tools. In addition, like the industrial lathes and fish
butchering knives in Figures 2 and 3, the greater the processing volumes
were, the more cost effective it would have been to increase the
efficiency of the tool by creating a specialised tool that would perform
tasks with maximal efficiency and maximal longevity even though it might
be more costly to make or maintain. These relationships especially
obtain when the transport of lithic material is constrained by human
mobility, i.e. the need for individuals to carry all of their tools and
replacement materials with themselves during moves.
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
To return to the Palaeolithic, the scenario of increasing cutting
requirements over time seems especially plausible if we assume that,
during Oldowan times, the use of spears may have been relatively
infrequent and limited (Hayden 2008). Chopping tools and the flakes
derived from them appear to have been entirely adequate to cope with
rare butchering or spear making/sharpening events between suitable
lithic sources. With the advent of Acheulean technology, however, it is
generally thought that hunting became increasingly perfected and that
hunted meat played a much larger role in the human diet (Shipman &
Walker 1989; Aiello & Wheeler 1995; Bramble & Lieberman 2004).
If so, this implies that spears and butchering tools were being made and
used far more frequently than in the Oldowan, and involved much greater
processing volumes of both wood (for spears) and meat. Given these new
processing requirements, together with the constraints of transporting
all tools by hand, it would have been important to develop more
specialised tools and to make those tools last significantly longer than
Oldowan choppers. It is in this context that the development of handaxes
makes good technological sense, despite the increased costs of training,
making and transporting specialised billets, and procuring better
quality raw materials.
As illustrated in Figure 1c, billet-produced bifaces can be
resharpened many more times than any core or core tool reduced by
hard-hammer techniques. Moreover, the resulting billet flakes typically
have very acute edge angles making them ideal for cutting through hides
of animals. Such tools dull quickly when cutting through dirty or muddy
hair and they may need to be replaced frequently, thereby making
production of these flakes in quantity an important consideration in
some contexts. In this respect, one of the most important functions of
handaxes in some situations may have been as sources of thin billet
flakes as well as bifacial butchering tools. It seems evident that
handaxes would have made very useful tools for removing skins and for
other aspects of butchering, as demonstrated in a number of experiments
and through use-wear analyses (Jones 1980; Keeley 1980: 169, 170, 175;
Schick & Toth 1993; Machin et al. 2006).
If this much can be established with some certainty, it is critical
to realise that the much-touted symmetry of handaxes is also a product
of the soft-hammer reduction technique. In effect, it makes no
technological sense to use billets to reduce lithic materials unless one
is trying to maximise the number of resharpenings or to produce thin
billet flakes. In both cases, this can only be systematically and
reliably accomplished by creating a symmetrical, lenticular cross-sectioned core tool. In fact, the more lenticular and symmetrical
the form, the easier it is to remove typical thin, flat, curved,
tear-drop shaped billet flakes that travel beyond the centre of the tool
and hence maintain the cross-sectional and edge angle morphology within
constant ratios or optimal ranges over the many flake removals typical
of soft-hammer reduction (Hayden & Hutchings 1989). As any
accomplished flint knapper knows, this is what underlies the symmetry of
handaxes whether people were trying to maximise the use-life (number of
resharpenings) of handaxes as tools, or trying to maximise the number of
sharp flakes removed from a core tool, or both. We argue that the
symmetrical and lenticular shape of general handaxes had no more to do
with their use in sexual selection than the symmetrical rounded shape of
a pencil point over the many resharpenings of its lifetime. As the adage
states: form follows function. We suggest that this is also the reason
why billet reduction everywhere in the world, whether Europe, Africa, or
the Americas, exhibits symmetrical, lenticular bifacial forms--or do
Kohn and Mithen and others want to argue that all bifaces everywhere in
the world were used as sexual selection or other symbolical strategies?
Far from being the only theory that explains handaxe symmetry or
other characteristics, Kohn and Mithen's scenario appears as a weak
supposition lacking grounding in technological realities. Handaxes do
not even make reliable indicators of reproductive merit since they could
be easily loaned out and may have circulated more as communal property
than individual property, as is typical of almost all material
possessions in simple hunter-gatherer societies. Kohn and Mithen's
suggestion strains credulity that handaxes were discarded shortly after
being made (sometimes in abundance) in order to show off skills by flint
knappers who were being watched by potential mates (Kohn & Mithen
1999: 522). This idea is not much better than the informal suggestion
that some archaeologists have expressed that handaxe makers lacked
memory to the extent that when they rested, they put down their bifaces
and then forgot that they had already made a handaxe and so started
over, and over, to make yet another.
It is far more plausible to suggest either that some localities
were being used as depots to stockpile tools like handaxes and raw
materials for easy access during transits that did not intersect good
quarry locations (much as suggested by Potts (1988: 305) for Oldowan
lithic accumulations), or that the main objective of handaxe production
at some sites was not the handaxes themselves but the production of
quantities of billet flakes for skinning or making bags or cloaks of
skin. This could account for the relatively visually
'pristine' appearance of many handaxes at some well-documented
butchering sites like Boxgrove, and the large numbers of plain,
unretouched flakes at some sites (the thin edges of most billet flakes
are not very suitable for direct percussion retouching, and even when
resharpened will never have as sharp an edge as an the unretouched
version).
Nor can much credence be given to the claim that 'Changes in
sexual selection criteria ... caused the Acheulean ta break dawn'
(Kohn & Mithen 1999: 523). It is far more likely that changes in
hunting, woodworking, hafting, or other economic constraints, were the
underlying factors leading to the abandonment of handaxe technology.
Curated, hafted Levallois flakes, in particular, may have replaced
expedient billet flakes as the skinning tool of choice in many areas.
All this is not to deny that objects requiring some skill to make
could have sometimes been used to impress others, especially when well
made--a point also made by Machin (2008: 765-6). But this is true of
virtually all crafts, including some lithic types. It seems almost
certain that same bifaces in later periods were used as prestige items.
Good examples include the very rare and exceptionally large and thin,
non-functional Solutrean laurel leafs, and the ceremonial bifaces (up to
750mm long) used by California Indians in rituals (Kroeber 1905: 690,
1953: 26-7; Rust 1905). However, in these cases, the prestige or ritual
examples were all exceptional variants of basic utilitarian bifaces.
While there are good ethnographic accounts of elaborate bifaces being
used to impress ritual specialists or high ranking individuals, there
are no ethnographic indications that bifaces were ever used as a
significant strategy to attract mates, or for that matter to project any
'important perceptual imperative' related to symmetry or other
abstractions or to trigger any sense of reassurance that the perceptual
system was working appropriately (Hodgson 2009: 196-7).
That a few handaxes, especially the very large examples, may have
been used to impress others, or even used as non-functional prestige
objects, is a possibility worth exploring and might provide insights
into Acheulean social dynamics. However, deriving the existence of
entire technological classes of objects on the basis of the extreme
characteristics of a few rare and exceptional cases is not sound
methodology. It is no more appropriate to derive the reason for handaxe
development from a few large examples than it is to try to argue that
automobiles in general developed primarily as a sexual selection or
prestige display tactic on the basis of the elongated and lavish
attributes of stretch limousines.
The basic symmetrical biface form makes far more sense as firmly
rooted in the practicalities of lithic inventorying, mobility,
processing volumes, and other fundamental design constraints. In order
to explain the appearance and demise of specific archaeological aspects,
it may be trendy to attempt to clothe old artefacts in the mantles of
new theories like sexual selection, signalling theory, or evolutionary
psychology of symmetry. However, without a firmly grounded understanding
of the basic technology involved and its constituent constraints, it
soon becomes apparent that the new clothes of such theoretical emperors
are embarrassingly lacking in substance.
Postscript
This comment complements the recent article by Nowell and Chang
(2009).
Acknowledgements
We would like to express our deep gratitude for the lithic insights
and flint knapping training that Francois Bordes provided, the
suggestions from Maxine Kleindienst to pursue design theory applications
in lithic studies, and the cogent comments provided by Rick Schulting on
an earlier draft.
References
AIELLO, L. & P. WHEELER. 1995. The expensive-tissue hypothesis.
Current Anthropology 36: 199-221.
BELL, R. 1972. Changing technology and manpower requirements in the
engineering industry (Science Policy Research Unit, Research Report 3).
Brighton: Sussex University Press.
BLEED, P. 1986. The optimal design of hunting weapons:
maintainability or reliability. American Antiquity 51: 737-47.
BRAMBLE, D. & D. LIEBERMAN. 2004. Endurance running and the
evolution of Homo. Nature 432: 345-52.
GOLD, B. 1979. Productivity, technology and capital Lexington (MA):
Heath.
HAYDEN, B. 1989. From chopper to Celt: the evolution of
resharpening techniques, in R. Torrence (ed.) Time, energy and stone
tools: 7-16. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
--1998. Practical and prestige technologies. Journal of
Archaeological Method and Theory 5: 1-55.
--2008. What were they doing in the Oldowan? Lithic Technology 33:
105-39.
HAYDEN, B. & R. GARGETT. 1988. Specialization in the
Paleolithic. Lithic Technology 17(1): 12-18.
HAYDEN, B. & K. HUTCHINGS. 1989. Whither the billet flake?, in
D. Amick & R. Mauldin (ed.) Experiments in lithic technology
(British Archaeological Reports International Series 528): 235-57.
Oxford: British Archaeological Reports.
HAYDEN, B., N. FRANCO & J. SPAFFORD. 2000. Keatley Creek lithic
strategies and design, in B. Hayden (ed.) The ancient past of Keatley
Creek (Volume 1): 185-212. Burnaby (B.C.): Archaeology Press.
HODGSON, D. 2009. Symmetry and humans: reply to Mithen's
'sexy handaxe theory.' Antiquity 83: 195-8.
HORSFALL, G. 1987. Design theory and grinding stones, in B. Hayden
(ed.) Lithic studies among the contemporary highland Maya: 332-77.
Tucson (AZ): University of Arizona Press.
JONES, P. 1980. Experimental butchery with modern stone tools and
its relevance for Palaeolithic archaeology. World Archaeology 12:
153-65.
KEELEY, L. 1980. Experimental determination of stone tool uses: a
microwear analysis. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.
KOHN, M. & S. MITHEN. 1999. Handaxes: products of sexual
selection? Antiquity 73:518-26.
KROEBER, A. 1905. Notes by A.L. Kroeber. American Anthropologist 7:
690-95.
--1953. Handbook of the Indians of California. Berkeley (CA):
California Book Co.
MACHIN, A. 2008. Why handaxes just aren't that sexy: a
response to Kohn & Mithen. Antiquity 82: 761-6.
MACHIN, A., R. HOSHELD & S. MITHEN. 2006. Why are some handaxes
symmetrical? Testing the influence of handaxe morphology on butchery
effectiveness. Journal of Archaeological Science 34: 883-93.
MITHEN, S. 2008. 'Whatever turns you on': a response to
Anna Machin, 'Why handaxes just aren't that sexy'.
Antiquity 82: 766-9.
NOWELL, A. & M.L. CHANG. 2009. The case against sexual
selection as an explanation of handaxe morphology. PaleoAnthropology
2009: 77-88.
POTTS, R. 1988. Early hominid activities at Olduvai. New York:
Aldine de Gruyter.
PYE, D. 1964. The nature of design. London: Studio Vista.
--1968. The nature of art of workmanship. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
RUST, H. 1905. The obsidian blades of California. American
Anthropologist 7: 688-9.
SCHICK, K. & N. TOTH. 1993. Making silent stones speak: human
evolution and the dawn of technology. New York: Simon & Schuster.
SHEETS, P. & G. MUTO. 1972. Pressure blades and total cutting
edge: an experiment in lithic technology. Science 175: 632-4.
SHIPMAN, P. & A. WALKER. 1989. The costs of becoming a
predator. Journal of Human Evolution 18: 373-92.
TORRENCE, R. 1989. Re-tooling: towards a behavioral theory of stone
tools, in R. Torrence (ed.) Time, energy and stone tools: 57-66.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
ZEYHER, L. 1977. Zeyher's new guide to cost reduction in plant
operations. Englewood Cliffs (NJ): Prentice-Hall.
Brian Hayden & Suzanne Villeneuve *
* Archaeology Department, Simon Fraser University Burnaby, British
Columbia, Canada V5A 1S6 (Email: bhayden@sfu.ca)