Fashion versus reason--then and now.
Bentley, R. Alexander
Analogies between modern practice and prehistoric material culture
are becoming increasingly useful for archaeologists, including those
interested in branding studies, for example (e.g. Wengrow, in press) and
at formal research centres such as the AHRC Centre for the Evolution of
Cultural Diversity and the Santa Fe Institute. Studies of modern
cultural change--at a level of detail that most archaeologists can only
dream about--can lead to related insights about prehistoric culture
change through time. Modern fashion analysis can be methodologically
similar to testing, for example, the degree to which certain prehistoric
transitions reflect demographic change (e.g. Shennan 2000; Henrich
2004). How much of the Upper Palaeolithic 'revolution' in cave
art is due to increases in population in western Europe? Although the
data are trickier to obtain, the goal is basically the same--subtract
what is considered background (e.g. population size) from what is of
interest to the researcher (e.g. instances of particular art motifs). In
Neolithic Germany, for example, pottery designs can be treated as the
'fashions' and numbers of longhouses are used to estimate
population size (e.g. Shennan & Wilkinson 2001; Bentley &
Shennan 2003).
Conceptualised this way, the study of material culture popularity
can take advantage of sophisticated tools from network theory (e.g.
Watts 2003) and population genetics (e.g. Cavalli-Sforza & Feldman
1981; Ammerman & Cavalli-Sforza 1984; Bentley et al. 2004). The
resulting culture evolution models, in all their variety (e.g. Mesoudi
et al. 2006; Shennan 2002 for reviews) can generally be divided into two
camps. The first treats individuals as independent decision-makers who
weigh the costs and benefits of their options, while subject to various
biases of influence (e.g. Winterhalder & Smith 2000; Henrich &
Gil-White 2001; Gintis 2007; McElreath & Boyd 2007). This applies
well to behaviours or technology that serve some adaptive purpose, i.e.
that matter to human survival, such as the conversion from foraging to
farming (e.g. Renfrew 1978), or the spread of a useful technology (e.g.
Rogers 1962; Henrich 2001). Even art, if it imparts some meaningful
signal (e.g. mating potential), can be governed by cost/benefit
decisions (e.g. Bliege Bird & Smith 2005; Geher & Miller 2007).
At the other end of the spectrum are behaviours that do not
inherently 'matter', and for which there is often a large,
maybe infinite, variety of options--decorative designs, musical motifs,
and word forms, for example. These choices can be considered
'neutral' traits, in that what is chosen has no inherent value
relative to other available options (Binford 1963; Koerper & Stickel
1980; Gillespie 1998). It assumes that whether a mother names her girl
'Jane' or 'Jamelia' depends on the current usage of
the name, rather than the name itself. This is formalised as the random
copying or neutral model, akin to the neutral-trait model of population
genetics, for popular culture change (e.g. Neiman 1995; Lipo et al.
1997; Shennan & Wilkinson 2001; Bentley & Shennan 2003; Hahn
& Bentley 2003).
Crucially, it is not proposed that people act randomly, but that
the statistics of all their choices, at the population level, are
comparable to random copying. It is in deliberate contrast to
independent decisions--actions under random copying depend entirely on
what others are doing. Applied to prehistoric studies, the model simply
allows us to ask, what if everyone simply copied each other, with
occasional innovation? Against this background 'canvas', more
interesting phenomena become visible (e.g. Herzog et al. 2004; Eerkens
& Lipo 2005). Shennan and Wilkinson (2001), for example, observed
that pottery design frequencies fit neutral model predictions for the
Early but not the Late Linearbandkeramik (LBK), which in turn suggests
that either people were becoming more creative or they were receiving
new ideas from outside communities. In any case, these new insights
about Late LBK society were made possible by use of the neutral (random
copying) model, just through analysing the frequencies of pottery
designs in one location.
Given the two extremes--random copying versus independent
decisions--often the question is where behaviours lie on the spectrum
between them (e.g. Collard et al. 2006). For example, with independent,
rational thinking, behaviours should converge upon the collective
priorities of individuals (Dunnell 1978; Surowiecki 2004). On the other
hand, random copying with occasional innovation leads our collective
tastes to drift continually, in directions that are unpredictable
(Salganik et al. 2006), but at a rate that is steady and predicted by
the level of innovation (Bentley et al. 2007). Crucially, we need not
decide beforehand what is subject to drift, as this is just what we aim
to find out empirically, using these contrasting models for the patterns
of change through time.
These observations apply equally to the debate about academic
writing, and were prompted by Stephen Chrisomalis (2007), who raises an
excellent point in response to Bentley (2006): in evaluating fashion
trends, one must take the background into account. In the case of
academic publishing today, the appearances of all keywords have
increased since 1990, due to a roughly fourfold increase in recorded
journal pages during that period (cf. Chrisomalis 2007: Figure 1).
However, both 'agency' and 'nuanced' still qualify
as buzzwords against this rising background: 'agency'
increasing tenfold since 1990 (Bentley 2006: Figure 1), and
'nuanced' increasing fourfold since 1997, after the expansion
of journals had already levelled off (cf. Chrisomalis 2007: Figure 1).
There are other ways to demonstrate this language copying--a quick
Google search for 'a more nuanced understanding of' (exact
phrase) receives fully a third as many hits as 'nuanced
understanding' and twice as many hits as anything 'less
nuanced' at all. Orwell (1946) was absolutely right about the
copying of strips of words. I have fretted about academic jargon because
it demonstrates the continual flux and empirical patterns of random
copying (Simkin & Roychowdhury 2003; Bentley 2006), which implies
that buzzwords do not matter in a meaningful, scientific sense. This is
a natural part of human interaction, however. The language copying that
Orwell (1946) was bemoaning reflects, for better or worse, our
remarkable ability to imitate--a prerequisite for culture itself.
References
AMMERMAN, A.J. & L.L. CAVALLI-SFORZA. 1984. The Neolithic
Transition and the Genetics of Populations in Europe. Princeton (NJ):
Princeton University Press.
BENTLEY, R.A. 2006. Academic copying, archaeology and the English
language. Antiquity 80: 196-201.
BENTLEY, R.A. & S.J. SHENNAN. 2003. Cultural evolution and
stochastic network growth. American Antiquity 68: 459-85.
BENTLEY, R.A., M.W. HAHN & S.J. SHENNAN. 2004. Random drift and
culture change. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 271: 1443-50.
BENTLEY, R.A., C.P. LIPO, H.A. HERZOG & M.W. HAHN. 2007.
Regular rates of popular culture change reflect random copying.
Evolution and Human Behavior 28: 151-58.
BINFORD, L. 1963. "Red ochre" caches from the Michigan
area: a possible case of cultural drift. Southwestern Journal of
Anthropology 19: 89-108.
BLIEGE BIRD, R. & E. ALDEN SMITH. 2005. Signaling theory,
strategic interaction, and symbolic capital. Current Anthropology 46:
221-48.
CAVALLI-SFORZA, L.L. & M.W. FELDMAN (ed.) 1981. Cultural
Transmission and Evolution: a quantitative approach. Princeton (NJ):
Princeton University Press.
CHRISOMALIS, S. 2007. The perils of pseudo-Orwellianism. Antiquity
81: 204-7.
COLLARD M., S.J. SHENNAN & J.J. TEHRANI. 2006. Branching,
blending and the evolution of cultural similarities and differences
among human populations. Evolution and Human Behavior 27: 169-84.
DUNNELL, R.C. 1978. Style and function: a fundamental dichotomy.
American Antiquity 43: 192-202.
EERKENS, J. & C.P. LIPO. 2005. Cultural transmission and the
generation, maintenance, and propagation of variation in the
archaeological record. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24:
316-34.
GEHER, G. & G.F. MILLER (ed.) 2007. Mating Intelligence. Mahwah
(NJ): Erlbaum.
GILLESPIE, J.H. 1998. Population Genetics: a Concise Guide.
Baltimore (MD): Johns Hopkins University Press.
GINTIS, H. 2007. A framework for the unification of the behavioural
sciences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30: 1-61.
HAHN, M.W. & R.A. BENTLEY. 2003. Drift as a mechanism for
cultural change: an example from baby names. Proceedings of the Royal
Society B 270: S1-S4.
HENRICH, J. 2001. Cultural transmission and the diffusion of
innovations. American Anthropologist 103: 992-1013.
--2004. Demography and cultural evolution: why adaptive cultural
processes produced maladaptive losses in Tasmania. American Antiquity
69: 197-211.
HENRICH, J. & F.J. GIL-WHITE. 2001. The evolution of prestige:
freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of
cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior 22: 165-96.
HERZOG, H.A., R.A. BENTLEY & M.W. HAHN. 2004. Random drift and
large shifts in popularity of dog breeds. Proceedings of the Royal
Society B 271: S353-56.
KOERPER, H.C. & E.G. STICKEL 1980. Cultural Drift: A primary
process of culture change. Journal of Anthropological Research 36:
463-69.
LIPO, C.P., M.E. MADSEN, R.C. DUNNELL & T. HUNT 1997.
Population structure, cultural transmission and frequency seriation.
Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 16: 301-33.
McELREATH, R. & R. BOYD. 2007. Mathematical Models of Social
Evolution: a Guide for the Perplexed. Chicago (IL): University of
Chicago Press.
MESOUDI, A., A. WHITEN & K.N. LALAND. 2006. Towards a unified
science of cultural evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29: 329-47.
NEIMAN, F.D. 1995. Stylistic variation in evolutionary perspective.
American Antiquity 60: 7-36.
ORWELL, G. 1946. Politics and the English language, in The Penguin
Essays of George Orwell: 348-60. London: Penguin.
RENFREW, C. 1978. Trajectory, discontinuity and morphogenesis: the
implications of catastrophe theory for archaeology. American Antiquity
43: 203-22.
ROGERS, E.M. 1962. Diffusion of Innovations. New York: Free Press.
SALGANIK, M.J., P.S. DODDS & D.J. WATTS. 2006. Experimental
study of inequality and unpredictability in an artificial cultural
market. Science 311: 854-56.
SHENNAN, S.J. 2000. Population, culture history, and the dynamics
of change. Current Anthropology 41: 811-35.
--2002. Genes, Memes and Human History. London: Thames &
Hudson.
SHENNAN, S.J. & J.R. WILKINSON. 2001. Ceramic style change and
neutral evolution: a case study from Neolithic Europe. American
Antiquity 66: 577-94.
SIMKIN, M.V. & V.P. ROYCHOWDHURY. 2003. Read before you cite!
Complex Systems 14: 269.
SUROWIECKI, J. 2004. The Wisdom of Crowds: Why the Many Are Smarter
than the Few. London: Abacus.
WATTS, D.J. 2003. Six Degrees: The Science of a Connected Age.
London: Heinemann.
WENGROW, D. In press. Prehistoric commodity branding. Current
Anthropology.
WINTERHALDER, B. & E. ALDEN SMITH. 2000. Analyzing adaptive
strategies: human behavioral ecology at twenty-five. Evolutionary
Anthropology 9: 51-72.
R. Alexander Bentley, AHRC Centre for the Evolution of Cultural
Diversity, Department of Anthropology, Durham University, 43 Old Elvet,
Durham DH1 3HN, UK (Email: r.a.bentley@durham.ac.uk)