Hunter-gatherers: an interdisciplinary perspective.
KELLY, ROBERT L.
CATHERINE PANTER-BRICK, ROBERT H. LAYTON & PETER ROWLEY-CONWY
(ed.). Hunter-gatherers: an interdisciplinary perspective, xii+341
pages, 22 figures, 16 tables. 2001. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press; 0-521-77210-9 hardback 60 [pounds sterling]& US$90,
0-521-77672-4 paperback [pounds sterling] 20.95 & US$29.95.
The editors' purpose in this volume is to re-establish the
lines of communication between analysts of the ecology, biology and
society of foraging societies which have, in their opinion, broken down
in recent years. Accordingly, the volume contains papers covering
archaeology, optimal foraging, technology, linguistics, biology, health,
demography, art and foragers' relations with nation-states.
The editors begin by claiming that foraging's most salient
characteristic is the absence of control over the reproduction and gene
pool of exploited species. However, that definition is at odds with
foragers who often alter their environments, especially through burning.
Although the editors find this definition useful, it is not followed up
in the volume. Instead, there are papers by Winterhalder on optimal
foraging approaches; Rowley-Conwy, Torrence, Kuhn and Stiner on
archaeology; McConvell on linguistics; Pennington, Jenike and Froment on
human biology; Conkey on art; and Layton on the status of modern
foragers in the nation-state.
The papers are all good, but uneven in their accessibility.
Winterhalder, for example, provides an easy review of optimal foraging,
finishing with a succinct discussion of foragers' tendency to
underproduce and criticisms of the optimization assumption. Likewise,
the papers by Rowley-Conwy and Torrence are readable, speaking to
`big' issues such as the antiquity of the `original affluent
society' and concepts with which to understand forager technology.
Kuhn and Stiner use ethnographic data to compare Mousterian and Upper
Paleolithic archaeology, showing that Neanderthals were not human in the
way we expect humans to behave. In a way, Conkey contributes to this
position by pointing out that `art' -- which first definitively
appears in the Upper Paleolithic -- is a cultural manifestation and
culturally defined -- all evidence suggesting that humans were not human
as we know them until late in human evolution. Although Kuhn, Stiner,
Winterhalder and others show that ethnographic data can be usefully
employed by archaeology, Layton reminds us that we need to remember the
historical context of living foragers. If anything, this makes
cross-cultural studies more important, since such studies are more
likely to ferret out cases where the social context is more strongly
influencing behaviour than, say, the natural environment. But other
papers are less accessible. If this volume seeks to establish a
dialogue, then the authors have to talk to more general points, and in
an accessible language. McConnell's paper on linguistics is more
concerned with a particular problem in Australian linguistic prehistory.
The biological papers are often hard going, using measures that are not
common in the foraging literature, with extensive discussion of biology
that the uninitiated will find difficult.
These papers are intriguing, but they are also selective.
Pennington, for example, focuses on a critique of Blurton-Jones'
argument that nomadic !Kung women maximized their reproduction with a
four-year birth spacing. He argues that there is no difference between
the fertility of nomadic and sedentary !Kung, that long birth spacing is
a function of venereal disease, and that past foragers should have had
high fertility rates punctuated by periods of venereal disease-induced
declines in fertility or high mortality. But he nearly ignores the
effects of breast-feeding, aerobic activity, diet, and seasonality in
diet quality on fertility. The other biological papers rely most heavily
on African foragers, with ethnographic examples thrown in, in the sort
of off-the-cuff use that Layton warns against.
Undergraduates will find the text dense. Graduate students will
find it more useful, but will be surprised at what is excluded:
mobility, `complex' hunter-gatherers, or social and political
organization are virtually untouched.
Another area that is left aside is evolutionary psychology (EP). EP
argues that humans are a product of genetic selection during the
evolutionary environment of selection (EES) -- when everyone was a
forager. Thus we have hunter-gatherer minds, with selection accounting
for everything from male-female differences in spatial reckoning to
morality. EP has never said when or where the EES existed -- just `back
there'; but Kuhn and Stiner show that humans have been cultural
beings for a short period of time, evolutionarily speaking. EP is one
effort at synthesizing forager biology, behaviour and prehistory -- so
why does not this volume, with the same purpose, comment on it?
Although individual papers are useful, some even intriguing
(Pennington makes me rethink demography), the volume falls short of
re-establishing an interdisciplinary debate. Unfortunately, most edited
volumes do; there is no substitute for hard work by an ambitious
individual to read, digest and synthesize the ever-growing number of
focused studies. I was glad to read this book, but I still look forward
to the synthesis, dialogue and debate at which this volume took aim.
ROBERT L. KELLY
Department of Anthropology
University of Wyoming
rlkelly@uwyo.edu