Headroom and human trampling: cave ceiling-height determines the spatial patterning of stone artefacts at Petzkes Cave, northern New South Wales.
Theunissen, Robert ; Balme, Jane ; Beck, Wendy 等
Going into a cave or shelter, one walks where one can stand upright
or has to crouch less. That affects which zones objects are trampled on,
which zones they may be kicked out of, which zones they may be kicked
into. And those effects interact with the usual spatial order - with its
activity zones and drop zones - that develops through occupation of the
enclosed cave or shelter.
As has been widely recognized (for example, by Schiffer 1983;
Hivernal & Hodder 1984; Nash & Petragila 1987; Frankel 1989;
Holdaway & Irwin 1993), identification of activity areas from maps
of spatial artefact distribution is difficult because materials at
archaeological sites are rarely recovered from their original position.
Disturbance of the materials through postdepositional processes may
alter the original spatial distribution and deliberate site maintenance
at the time of occupation may also move artefacts horizontally. Human
trampling, animal activity, water and wind disturbance are the main
post-depositional processes, with human trampling regarded as a major
disturbance agent in sandy rock-shelter deposits in Australia and
elsewhere (Stockton 1973; Hughes & Lamperr 1977; Villa & Courtin
1983).
This paper investigates the effects of human trampling on the
horizontal distribution of stone artefacts at Petzkes Cave, a large
sandstone rockshelter in northern New South Wales. Stone artefacts were
chosen for study; less subject to animal activity than organic
artefacts, they are resistant to decay. Petzkes Cave [ILLUSTRATION FOR
FIGURE 1 OMITTED]) was chosen for excavation because it is a large (25 m
long) and relatively deep (9 m) rock-shelter. Its size makes it likely
that a variety of activities took place at the site.
In this paper we find that trampling effects on the artefact
distribution at Petzkes Cave can be predicted from ceiling-height. This
allows us to focus on the artefacts and the cave zones which may be
least affected by trampling, when investigating the original areas of
activity. We predicted a generalized artefact distribution from previous
work, and refined it for Petzkes Cave by a trampling experiment. When
this 'hypothetical' distribution was compared with the actual
distribution of artefacts in the top 3 cm of the deposit, we found good
agreement between the expected and observed distribution of artefacts,
suggesting that trampling was a major agent of disturbance.
Predicting the distribution of artefacts after trampling
Existing models for the effects of human trampling
Human trampling has long been recognized as a significant source of
post-depositional disturbance in archaeological sites, especially in
periodically re-occupied sites such as caves and rock-shelters (Hughes
& Lampert 1977). In some caves, particularly sandstone caves such as
Petzkes Cave, sediment may only accumulate at a rate of a few
centimetres per century; materials near the surface are subjected to the
combined trampling disturbance of all the occupations occurring in that
time-span (Hughes & Lainpert 1977; 136; Villa & Courtin 1983:
270). Most experimental studies on the effects of human trampling on the
distribution of archaeological materials in archaeological sites - for
example, Gifford-Gonzalez et al, (1985), Stockton (1973) and Villa &
Courtin (1983) - have mainly dealt with vertical displacement of
artefacts and its implications for the temporal association of artefacts
with archaeological strata.
[TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE 1 OMITTED]
The little previous research on the effects of trampling on the
horizontal distribution of artefacts may suggest that larger artefacts
should be displaced further than smaller with a positive but
statistically insignificant relationship between horizontal movement and
artefact size (Nielson 1991: 490-93); the incidental results of Villa
& Courtin (1983: 277) show an inverse relationship (also not
significant) between lateral movement and artefact size.
Nielson (1991: 492-3) differentiates 'traffic zones', such
as paths where people walk (and so scuff artefacts), from 'marginal
zones' along walls or in the corners of rooms, where people
generally don't walk. Nielson hypothesizes that the larger and
medium-sized artefacts displaced by trampling in the traffic zones will
eventually collect in the marginal zones where they are protected (in
some cases by physical barriers) from further trampling.
The main aspect of rock-shelters which controls traffic flow is the
available headroom (ceiling-height). High ceiling areas might be the
place where people undertook most activities; perhaps tossing and
caching artefacts into the low ceiling zones (e.g. Morwood 1981).
Adapting Nielson's hypothesis to a rock-shelter context, areas of
the shelter with ceiling above average human height would have been
cleared of most larger artefacts by trampling while areas with low
ceilings would have collected these artefacts. Smaller artefacts would
remain in the traffic areas.
While these high and low ceiling-height zones - corresponding to
Nielson's traffic and marginal zone respectively - are roughly
defined around average adult human height, we do not know at just what
height the expected changes in artefact size and frequency should occur,
or exactly how much clearance is needed for comfortable walking. A
trampling experiment was conducted (by RT) to clarify this. When studies
in post-depositional movement of artefacts have gone straight from a
generalized model to testing in the archaeological data, the temptation
is to keep varying the model's parameters until a fit with the
archaeology is found. Meaningful patterns can be obscured and/or weak
patterns emphasized by uncritically selecting divisions between
continuous variables (Frankel 1989; Blankholm 1991). Our experiment was
designed to define precisely ceilingheight zones relevant to human
trampling in Petzkes Cave and to identify the distances artefacts move
horizontally.
An experiment
For this experiment a 3-m wide section of the cave was selected
[ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 2 OMITTED]) with the greatest variety of
ceiling-heights; 147 flakes, flaked pieces and cores made on basalt were
selected for the experiment, divided into size classes by weight - when
all artefacts are of the same material their weights are directly
proportional to their size. We attempted to match the size distribution
and range with that perceived during excavation.
Seven groups, each containing 21 experimental artefacts with a
similar size distribution were created (TABLE 1). One group was placed
in each of the seven excavation squares (shown in [ILLUSTRATION FOR
FIGURE 3 OMITTED]).
The seven groups of artefacts were laid on a back-filled surface
which covered the full range of ceiling-heights recorded at the site.
Each group was allocated to a 1-m strip [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 3
OMITTED]) within which individual artefacts were spaced at 5-cm
intervals along the measured line, alternating each side of the line
also by 5 cm. The position of each artefact was then recorded.
Once placed, the artefacts were left to be trampled incidentally by
the 15 excavation team members, wearing fiat-soled soft shoes or bare
feet, for a period of three weeks. Team members were not aware of the
full details of the experiment or of its expectations and for the most
part were unaware of the presence of the experimental artefacts. They
went through the study area for a number of reasons, to access equipment
and the theodolite station at the far end of the cave, and to excavate
adjacent squares. Although these activities are not those of the
original occupants of the cave, human movement through the cave should
have been similarly constrained; the shape of cave topography has not
changed much since the surface sediments were deposited.
After three weeks the positions of the experimental artefacts were
individually recorded with the Electronic Distance Measurer. Eventually,
all but one (from size class 2) of the experimental artefacts were
recovered, yielding a final experimental sample of 146 artefacts moved
as a result of trampling.
The effects of ceiling-height and of barriers on the extent of
movement of stone artefacts of different sizes was then assessed. The
outline of the cave's walls and ceiling-height was recorded. The
GIS program IDRISI was used to interpolate a contour map of the
ceiling-heights from these point measurements and then to overlay trajectories of the experimental artefacts on to the map for direct
comparison. The GIS was then used to quantify the movement, frequency
and size distribution of artefacts.
Experiment results
FIGURE 4 provides striking evidence that ceiling-height influences
the horizontal movement artefacts experience through human trampling.
Artefacts in higher ceiling zones moved further than artefacts in low
ceiling zones. The largest change occurs at a ceiling-height of
[approximately] 2 metres (clearly related to adult human height), with
average movement of artefacts (9.3 cm) from areas with ceilig [greater
than] [greater than] 2.0 metres about two and a half times ]hat of
artefacts from areas with ceilings [less than] 2.0 metres (3.9 cm).
The experiment also shows that at Petzkes Cave larger stone artefacts
are horizontally displaced longer distances by trampling than smaller
artefacts. FIGURE 5 shows the average movement of artefacts in each of
the six size classes.
How likely is it that these measured differences result by chance?
The significance was tested using the Monte Carlo technique (Kintigh
1990: 172), with average movements for each size class compared with
four randomly generated sets of artefact movement data. Each random set
contains the same quantity of artefacts and the same length
trajectories, with the size classes randomly assigned in correct
proportion (for example, four size class 1 artefacts) among the artefact
trajectories. The experimental observations are significant if the real
experimental artefact movements show stronger or more consistent trends
than the randomly generated movement files.
The results of this comparison are shown in FIGURE 6. In the
experiment the smallest size classes i and 2 moved less and the largest
size classes 5 and 6 moved more than the same size classes in any of the
randomly generated data sets. We conclude that the experimentally
observed trend of increasing artefact movement with increasing artefact
size is significant. The experiment helps to define for a rock-shelter
site the two distributional zones of artefacts generally predicted by
Nielson (1991): a 'traffic' zone in areas with high ceilings
[greater than]2.0 m) will have fewer and smaller artefacts than the
non-traffic or 'marginal' zone [les than] 2 m). However, our
results suggest a further breakdown of these zones may be useful.
At 9.3 cm the average horizontal movement of experimental artefacts
in the traffic zone, though greater than elsewhere, is quite small.
Rather than collecting against the cave walls, this suggests artefacts
will mainly collect in a narrow band occurring at a ceiling-height
similar to, and just below, average adult human standing height. This
comparatively rich 'transitional' zone is made up of the lower
ceiling-height areas of the traffic zone and the higher ceiling-height
areas of the 'marginal' zone suggested above.
The remaining 'marginal' zone can be further divided into
the area immediately adjacent to cave walls and the area which is not,
to separate out effects on the horizontal distribution of artefacts from
behaviours other than trampling. Tossing (Binford 1983: 152-4), sweeping
and dumping are likely to move larger artefacts well away from activity
areas (O'Connell 1987: 91-2), far into low ceiling zones against
cave walls. Larger artefacts may also be stored for later use in deep,
low ceiling-height recesses (Morwood 1981: 35).
To summarise, this experiment, in conjunction with previous studies,
predicts four artefact distributional zones from human trampling:
* The traffic zone, with a ceiling-height greater than 2.0 m,
(nominally [greater than]2.15 m)
* The transitional zone 1.25 m-2.15 m which covers a range of human
heights including older children
* The final two categories are marginal zones: 0.75-1.25 m and
[less than] ]0.75 m
The predicted distribution of artefacts is modelled in FIGURE 7.
This simple model assumes that most knapping activities were carried
out in high ceiling areas and that tossing and caching behaviour placed
some artefacts in low ceiling areas. The original distribution of the
artefacts is unknown, but assuming that there were originally more
artefacts within the high-ceiling zones than low-ceiling zones,
subsequent trampling should affect the distribution pattern of the
artefacts in the way in which the model describes.
Testing the model at Petzkes Cave
To test this model we used the sample of artefacts excavated from the
surface 3 cm from the same part of the shelter in which the experiment
was carried out. As artefacts were found during excavation, their
position was recorded using an electronic distance measurer. The
analysis of the artefacts addressed firstly how closely artefact
frequency and secondly artefact size fit our predictions.
Distribution of artefacts across ceiling-height zones
A plan [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 8 OMITTED]) shows the distribution of
1998 artefacts from this area whose co-ordinates were recorded during
excavation as well as contours of the ceiling-height; the patterns
appear to match predictions.
The most striking feature of the distribution is an extremely dense
cluster of artefacts in the centre of the study area. Its almost certain
explanation is the effect of the cave's drip-line which at this
point particularly concentrates water during rain. At the site today
water from the cave's drip-line washes fine sediments away down the
slope at the front of the cave, removing all but the largest particles
and exposing the artefacts. The tell-tale evidence is the lack of fine
sediment around the exposed artefacts and their location immediately
below the line of the cave's overhang (Morwood 1981: 34).
This 'drip-line' disturbance complicates the identification
of the pattern resulting from human trampling under the control of cave
ceiling-height. On a reasonably level surface it is not likely to affect
the average weight of artefacts present (as even larger particles of
sediment remain), but it does create a local peak in spatial artefact
frequency. The small drip-line cluster has been removed from the
following visual and quantitative analysis.
This drip-line cluster apart, the spatial patterning of artefacts
closely matches the modelled pattern [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 7
OMITTED]): the high ceiling 'traffic' zone contains far fewer
artefacts than are present in the low ceiling areas (transitional and
marginal zones) to either side. Artefact frequency (in general) declines
gradually on moving deeper into the lowest ceiling areas (marginal zone)
on the extreme left and right.
The exception is a concentration of artefacts against the rear wall
of the cave to the lower left of FIGURE 8. This area, well shielded from
wind, is out of the main thoroughfare of the cave yet has sufficient
ceiling-height for comfortable sitting; it may in the past have been
preferred for activities which allow people to sit.
To quantify these observations, the numbers of artefacts per square
metre in separate ceilingheight zones were counted using the IDRISI
program [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 9 OMITTED]). A 0.75 m diameter area
around the drip-line cluster was not included in the counts.
The trends displayed in FIGURE 9 closely match those predicted in the
model. To test whether or not this distribution of artefacts in space
could have occurred by chance we again used Monte Carlo principles, but
indirectly, through a K-means cluster analysis (Kintigh 1993). The
clustering recorded in the archaeological distribution was compared with
that of four false random distributions [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 10
OMITTED]). The per cent SSE value of the trace (y axis) is well below
the traces for random distributions; the archaeological sample's
distribution is clustered to an extent unlikely to have arisen from
random variation (Kintigh 1990: 172). Coupled with the doubling of
artefact frequency across the crucial traffic/transitional zone
boundary, this result suggests that the match between the frequency
distribution of artefacts at Petzkes Cave and the model is significant.
Distribution of different-sized artefacts in different ceiling-height
zones
Information on the distribution of sizes in each of these four
ceiling-height zones was obtained by applying IDRISI to a sub-sample
consisting of 857 point-plotted stone artefacts with attached size data
collected during the excavation of the two northern metre strips across
the study area (that is, not including the southern 1-m strip).
The artefacts were divided into the same six size classes used in the
experiment (artefacts in the drip-line disturbance area included as that
disturbance should not affect the size distribution of artefacts), and
the average size class in each of the four ceiling-height zones
determined [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 11 OMITTED]). As predicted average
artefact size increases across the traffic/transitional zone boundary
(around 2 m ceiling-height).
The Monte Carlo style significance test for these results involves a
comparison of average artefact size in different ceiling-height zones
between the excavated sample and four false data sets where size classes
were randomly allocated (in correct proportions) to real artefact
positions in space. Variation from the mean artefact size, between the
low [less than]2 m) and high [greater than]2 m) ceiling-height zones for
both original and random distributions, is presented graphically in
FIGURE 12.
The difference in average artefact size between high and low
ceiling-height zones [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 12 OMITTED])is far greater
for the original distribution than for any of the random size
distributions. This confirms the significance of the observed
size-distribution trend against ceiling-height in FIGURE 11 and its
correspondence with the trend predicted in the model.
Conclusions and discussion
The horizontal distribution of excavated archaeological artefacts and
their size distribution at Petzkes Cave closely conforms to the
predicted distribution derived from experimental results, and is shown
as unlikely to result from chance. We conclude that horizontal
patterning at Petzkes Cave has been strongly affected by human trampling
operating under the control of cave ceiling-height, with average
horizontal displacement of artefacts from human trampling increasing
with increasing artefact size. It matches our prediction (derived from
Nielson 1991) that cave ceiling-height influences the horizontal
distribution of artefacts by restricting human trampling disturbance to
areas where humans can comfortably walk. The distribution patterns
formed by human trampling in caves will be distinctive and relatively
easy to discern archaeologically as changes in the frequency and average
size of artefacts should correspond in space with areas where cave
ceiling-height is equal to average human walking height.
The least disturbed artefacts are the very smallest; although there
may be fewer stone artefacts remaining in the high ceiling sections of
the cave, they have probably not suffered excessive post-depositional
horizontal movement. Clusters of artefacts in these areas may represent
original concentrations of activities. Areas deep in the marginal zones
will have the most direct connection with purposeful human activity,
particularly storing or tossing.
Trampling is a major disturbance factor; but, because the horizontal
distribution of artefacts resulting from such disturbance correlates to
cave topography, it can be predicted. Although the chances are that
larger artefacts will move further than smaller it cannot be inferred
that all artefacts have moved. If clusters are found that cannot be
explained by trampling, it is possible that the clusters are part of the
original deposition pattern. Our future research intends to test this by
comparing the distribution of stone artefacts to that of other kinds of
archaeological evidence - hearths and materials such as charcoal and
starch which, from the principles identified here, are less likely to be
affected by trampling.
Acknowledgements. We would like to thank the Australian Research
Council for funding the Petzkes Cave project, the volunteers who helped
excavate the site and Doug Hobbs who helped set up our recording system
and drew FIGURES 1, 9, 10 & 11.
References
BINFORD, L.R. 1983. In pursuit of the past: decoding the
archaeological record. London: Thames & Hudson.
BISKOWSKI, M. 1994. GIS & the study of grinding tools in the
Teotihuacan Valley, Mexico, in I. Johnson (ed.), Methods in the
mountains: proceedings of UISPP Commission IV Meeting Mt Victoria,
Australia, August 1993: 115-34. Sydney: Sydney University.
Archaeological Methods Series 2.
BLANKHOLM, H.P. 1991. Intra-site spatial analysis in theory and
practice. Arhus: Aarhus University Press.
CARR, C. 1991. Left in the dust: contextual information in
modelfocused archaeology, in Kroll & Price (ed.): 221-56.
FRANKEL, D. 1989. Koongine Cave excavations 1986-7: investigating
spatial patterning, Australian Archaeology 28: 3-13.
GIFFORD-GONZALEZ, D.P., D.B DAMROSCH, D.R. DAMROSCH, J. PRYOR &
R.L. THUNIN. 1985. The third dimension in site structure: an experiment
in trampling and vertical dispersal, American Antiquity 50(4): 803-18.
HIVERNAL, F. & I. HODDER. 1984. Analysis of artefact distribution
at Ngena (Kenya) depositlanai and post-depositional effects, in H.J.
Hietala (ed.), Intra-site spatial analysis in archaeology: 97-115.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
HOLDAWAY, S. & G. IRWIN. 1993. Electronic recording systems used
during the excavation of Sll/20 Ponuii Island, Hauraki Gulf, Archaeology
in New Zealand 36:27-37.
HUGHES, P.J. & R.J. LAMPERT. 1977. Occupational disturbance and
types of archaeological deposit, Journal of Archaeological Science 4:
135-40.
KINTIGH, K. 1990. Intrasite spatial analysis: a commentary on major
methods, in A. Voorrips (ed.), Mathematics and information science in
archaeology: a flexible framework: 165-200. Bonn: Halos. Studies in
Modern Archaeology 3.
1993. Tools for quantitative archaeology. Unpublished brochure
describing the software package of the same name. Tempe (AZ):
Anthropology Department, Arizona State University.
KROLL, E.M & T.D. PRICE (ed.) 1991. The interpretation of
archaeological spatial patterning. New York (NY): Plenum Press.
MORWOOD, M.J. 1981. Archaeology in the Central Queensland Highlands:
the stone component, Archaeology in Oceania 16(1): 1-53.
NASH, D. & M.D. PETRAGLIA, 1987a. Natural formation processes and
the archaeological record: present problems and future requisites, in
Nash & Petraglia (ed.): 186-203.
(ed.) 1987b. Natural formation processes and the archaeological
record. Oxford: British Archaeological Reports. International series
352.
NIELSON, A.E. 1991. Trampling the archaeological record: an
experimental study, American Antiquity 56(3): 483-503.
O'CONNELL, J. 1987. Alyawara site structure and its
archaeological implications, American Antiquity 51(1): 74-108.
RIGAUD, J. & J.F. SIMEK. 1991. Interpreting spatial patterns at
the Grotte XV: a multiple-method approach, in Kroll & Price (ed.):
199-220.
SCHIFFER, M.B. 1983. Toward the identification of formation
processes, American Antiquity, 48: 675-706.
SIMEK, J. 1984. Integrating pattern and context in spatial
archaeology, Journal of Archaeological Science 11: 405-20.
SIMEK, J & R.R. LARICK. 1983. The recognition of multiple spatial
patterns: a case study from the French Upper Palaeolithic, Journal of
Archaeological Science 10: 165-80.
STOCKTON, E. 1973. Shaw's Creek Shelter: human displacement of
artefacts and its significance, Mankind 9: 112-17.
VILLA, P. & J. COURTIN. 1983. The interpretation of stratified sites: a view from underground, Journal of Archaeological Science 10:
267-81.
WANDSNIDER, L. 1987. Natural formation process experimentation and
archaeological analysis, in Nash & Petraglia (ed.): 151-85.