Our own engendered species.
Hurcombe, Linda
The study of gender in ancient societies seems inseparable from the
place of gender in our own society - and therefore inseparable from the
particular attitudes and expectations those contemporary manners create.
This 'BIG' problem is explored, and some approaches to its
resolution are developed.
Walcott, following our primal biases of gender, found two specimens
that appeared to lack the frontal nozzle. ... On one specimen, Walcott
found a slender, two-pronged structure in the same location as the
nozzle. Walcott therefore concluded that he had discovered sexual
dimorphism in Opabinia: the strong and stout nozzle belonging to the
male (naturally), and the slender structure to the more delicate female.
He wrote that these supposed females 'differ from the male... in
having a slender, bifid frontal appendage instead of the strong
appendage of the male'. He even foisted the stereotypes of active
and passive upon his fictitious distinctions, arguing that the nozzle
'was probably used by the male to seize the female'.
GOULD (1989: 128) writing about Walcott (1912: 169)
Viable human populations need at least two biological sexes,
sufficient numbers of both, and their interaction. The archaeological
literature shows a past peopled mainly by men (e.g. 'Early
man', 'Neolithic man', 'craftsmen', 'big
men') and with little consideration for the interaction between
genders. As it is represented, the human species simply does not have a
viable prehistoric population. The reasons are rooted in the notions of
gender prevailing in the time and society in which the authors wrote,
and write, the archaeological accounts. The more ancient the period
under discussion, the more fragmentary the evidence and the more open
the interpretation of the scant fossil and cultural remains.
Walcott's view, quoted at the start of this article, refers to the
fossil remains of Opabinia, a 530 million-year-old worm with five eyes
and tail fins! If 1912 perceptions of contemporary human gender roles
have been imposed on that early invertebrate, how much more have
contemporary attitudes to gender coloured academic discussions on human
evolution and the history of our own species!
The gender roles prevailing in the archaeologist's time have
pervaded so much of the body of 'knowledge' of prehistoric
people that it is difficult to see how such an unhealthy past could have
gone unnoticed for so long. As the evidence for gender roles is woefully incomplete, contemporary social influences push 'comfortable'
interpretations. In academic writing to discuss new evidence and
theories, and in museum displays and reconstruction drawings, the
present is re-created for the past. There are thus two key areas: the
present attitudes to gender (a problem affecting all our society, and
hence not an archaeological concern per se), and the reflection of our
own society back on the past. This second aspect is clearly a concern
for archaeologists who need to discern and combat the problem. As the
past is used to explain the present, a tautology develops which leads to
a legitimization of the present by the past. The issue can be termed the
BIG (Biased Interpretation of Gender) problem in archaeology. This
article does not claim the discovery of the problem, as many people have
drawn attention to the invisibility of women and how we judge the past
by the contemporary present (e.g. Burtt 1987; Conkey & Spector 1984;
Engelstad 1991; Gilchrist 1991; Jones & Pay 1990; Moser 1992a;
1992b). Instead, this article aims to present the BIG problem clearly,
to group responses to it and to suggest ways of counteracting gender
bias. Medical metaphors show the complexity of the problem, its
propagation and the effectiveness of potential responses.
The disease - the BIG problem
Academic reactions to gender theory vary widely. One is that no
action is necessary because now that the problem has been recognized,
there is no longer any gender bias. The examples are mostly drawn from
the last five years in order to show that this complacency is completely
wrong. Gender roles are too crucial to the design of the social fabric
to be assumed, rather than critically interpreted. There are at least
two sexes in our past, and I am more interested in how these interact
than in favouring either one. There are sex differences which could
enable one sex to perform certain tasks or roles more efficiently, yet
the relative value attached to activities is a completely different
issue. Difference is not synonymous with inequality. It is the
contemporary value placed on activities which has caused most of the BIG
problem. As with the medical analogy, the symptoms are the first
indication of a problem. Once the disease has been discovered, more
subtle indications may become apparent and a study of cause, effect and
propagation will follow. For the BIG problem there are two main sets of
symptoms: visual and written.
Visual symptoms
Archaeology is not often taught in schools, so most people gain their
impressions of the subject from popular accounts, exhibitions and
television programmes. These media feature visual reconstructions giving
an immediate and influential impression of male and female roles in the
past, namely that women are less numerous in the past than men; that
males define the species and changes of evolutionary history; and that
throughout prehistory, males and females fulfilled the same roles as
they commonly do now.
Reconstructions, both 2D and 3D, often show more men (e.g. see TABLES
1 & 3). Women seem particularly disadvantaged where the evidence is
most fragmentary, and the developments portrayed are the most crucial in
defining our human qualities (Moser 1992a; 1992b; in press; Tanner
1981). The illustrations of different species of Australopithecus and
Homo are almost always male (e.g. Lambert 1987; Dunbar & Foley
1989:39 & 40). When these illustrations are strung together to show
our evolutionary development, the absence of one half of our species has
no justification. Sexual dimorphism is an evolutionary development, so
species should be defined by both sexes. Instead, we are left with the
impression of immaculate conception or hermaphrodite qualities! The
problem of gender bias is not solved simply by putting more females in
illustrations because where women are portrayed, their position,
expression and activity are often different from that of males, giving
visual impressions of gender roles. Gender bias has been recognized in
reconstruction drawings and museum displays (Jones & Pay 1990; Moser
1992a; 1992b; Stringer & Gamble 1993: chapter 1). The two examples
that follow show how very recent reconstructions perpetuate the BIG
problem. In the first, great care has been taken to base the museum
tableaux on firm evidence, and to include both sexes. Despite this,
stereotyped views of male and female roles are chosen again.
Tattersall (1992) describes problems and choices in creating figures
for an exhibition on Human Biology and Evolution in the American Museum
of Natural History in 1993. Three figures were to depict the Neanderthal
scene (1992: 65):
The first choice to be made - what the three individuals in the scene
should be doing - was the easiest because archaeologists have learned a
fair amount about how Neanderthals lived. For example, the
characteristic wear on their stone tools tells us that Neanderthals used
flints to cut wood and scrape hides. We settled on showing a male
sharpening a wooden spear while a young female scraped a hide and an
older female offered advice. Because Neanderthal front teeth are usually
very heavily worn we felt safe showing the young woman with one end of
the hide held in her teeth.
From the evidence, it would be equally valid to have shown the female
sharpening the spear and the male working the skin; the wear on the
teeth of Neanderthals occurs for males and females (Trinkaus 1983), and
there is no way of knowing if both sexes, or one, made and used pointed
sticks. Reconstructions and illustrations are limited by expense and
speculative in many respects. My quarrel is not that they are
individually inaccurate, but that these speculative reconstructions
usually portray gender roles according to contemporary attitudes: he
makes weapons, she makes clothes. Tattersall (1992) is aware of gender
activity and image, and one of the exhibition's female figures is
depicted warding off a scavenging bird, but his comments on the figures
relating to the closely-spaced Laetoli footprints reveal the commercial
attraction of an image which is easily understood by modern society
(Tattersall 1992: 67):
For visual interest we opted once more for an adult male and female,
the male with one arm draped over the female's shoulder. Perhaps
that pose is too anthropomorphic for some tastes, but the gifted English
sculptor John Holmes produced such a vivacious result for us in his
finished Laetoli figures that we frankly didn't care.
Thus this recent exhibition reconstruction - despite an awareness of
the BIG problem - shows some very stereotyped gender roles for our
remote past for which there is no archaeological basis.
The recent publication by the British Museum (Natural History) is a
good case to demonstrate similar collective impressions in 2D
illustrations. The text (which is discussed later) is by Andrews &
Stringer (1989) with an illustration on each facing page. These
paintings are as educational as the authors' words. Indeed, the
back cover states their validity:
Each description is brought to life with a beautiful painting by
Maurice Wilson reconstructed from fossil evidence not only of the group
concerned but also of the plants and other animals alive at that time.
Written by two of the leading scientists in the field, this book will
appeal to anyone interested in learning more about the fascinating
origins of our own species.
The illustrations are all by Maurice Wilson and, indeed, are striking
images. An analysis of the numbers of males and females and the tasks
being performed was conducted using the scene illustrations from
Australopithecus afarensis (1989: 29) onwards [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 1
OMITTED]. The BIG problem stands out clearly. TABLE 1 compares the
number of female, male and indeterminate figures in each scene. Of the
56 figures, 24 are male whilst only 10 are female. This would not make a
viable population. Turning to the activities of the gender-specific
figures, TEBLE 2 shows the female activities are digging and eating
plants, putting sticks on the fire, scraping skins and running in a
hunt,whilst males are portrayed holding and making stone tools, carrying
sticks and spears, using sticks as weapons and killing game. Four out of
10 females are inactive, but for males this drops to only 3 out of 24.
In the scene of Homo labilis gathering [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 1G
OMITTED], the female:male ratio is nearly equal, whilst the scenes of
confrontation or hunting had the worst ratios, i.e. females are only
adequately represented when 'female activities' are the
purpose of the scene. The illustrations show aspects of changing
lifestyles through our evolutionary past: collectively they imply that
these changes were in male, not female, activities. There is a further
difference in the position of female and male figures. In all except one
scene [ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 1I OMITTED], the foremost figure is male.
The collective impression gained from the illustrations in this
introductory view of human evolution is that males are active, to the
fore and they make and use stone tools to hunt, while females are
passive, look after the children, make the clothes and tend the hearth.
There is no direct archaeological evidence whatsoever for such sharp
division of gender roles.
With these examples from 1992 and 1989, both created by national
museums, it is obvious that the visual symptoms of gender bias are very
much in evidence.
Written symptoms
The gender imagery of archaeological reports in the popular press is
often appalling, a written symptom of the BIG problem. Bedlow's
account of the Bronze Age burial at Irthlingborough with the remains of
slaughtered bulls is particularly telling (Daily Telegraph 14 October
1991):
Instead of taking a bottle of wine to the rave-up some 4,000 years
ago on the banks of the river Nene .... Bronze Age man [sic] was asked
to bring his own beef, on the hoof. ... That meant leading young,
sexually active bulls to the party. ... 'What they did to the bulls
before they were eaten is pure conjecture, Women would have been there
to play their part in the feasting.' ... It is thought that cattle
skulls were part of an ancient British burial ritual with sexual
overtones.
The collective terms are male, the beasts are male, the overtones are
all sexual and the impression left by the article is that the female
role in the feasting may well have been sexual. Indeed, the title of the
piece is '20,000 went to Bronze Age Orgy'! The title and
language are presumably used to draw the readers' attention and
hold their interest. It is no comfort to know that such an
unsubstantiated view of gender roles in a Bronze Age ritual has reached
a wider audience than would a more considered one! One further example
just reads as nonsense (Highfield 1992: 12):
The first tool-making is thought to have started about 2.5 million
years ago. Primitive implements not only helped Neanderthals and early
man [sic] to remove more flesh from a carcass and allow larger shares
all round; but also, by helping females to look after their young, gave
everybody more access to sex.
This comes from a section entitled 'Why toolmakers looked so
happy'! Sex is again being used to make archaeological stories
'interesting'!! Happily, most modern academic writing is more
circumspect, but there are still three basic issues: gender-specific
terms, the means of interpretation of gender roles and the tone of the
language.
Many authors and publishers use language very carefully to avoid
specifying the sex of [TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE 2 OMITTED] the scavenger,
gatherer, toolmaker. A 1992 Scientific American article has phrases such
as 'The earliest hominids probably scavenged and took small prey
with their hands ... they began to use tools to butcher large
carcasses' (Blumenschine & Cavallo 1992: 94). Elsewhere
'mankind' and 'man' are used to refer generically to
both sexes according to a dictionary definition of 'man'.
However, there are two contradictory dictionary definitions of the word
'man' as on the one hand 'an adult human male, especially
as distinct from a woman or boy' and on the other hand 'human
beings in general: the human race' (Concise Oxford Dictionary 1990:
719). The 'males only' definition inevitably spills over on to
the 'neutral' generic usage of 'man' in compound
phrases. In addition, phrases such as 'man the hunter' are
still used even when the purpose is to challenge the assumptions made,
because to avoid these terms when discussing their origins and Validity
would be to alter history and deny that they were ever used (see also
Chippindale 1991: 103). The order of words can imply importance too:
Bender (1978) used the term gatherer-hunter rather than hunter-gatherer
to point out that gathering was probably a more important part of
subsistence than hunting.
TABLE 1. Gender portrayal in the nine hominid illustrations by
Maurice Wilson in Andrews & Stringer (1989: 29, 31, 35, 37, 39,
41, 43, 45, 46) and scematically represented in FIGURE 1. The
assignment of male/female was made on the basis of genitalia,
breasts and beards. C denotes child.
scene FIGURE 1 female male either sex total
page
29 d 1 1 1C 3
31 a 0 1 9 10
35 g 4 3 3C 10
37 b 0 2 0 2
39 e 1 2 0 3
41 c 1 1+1C 0 3
43 h 1 7 3 11
45 f 1 1 2C+2 6
47 l 1 5 1C+1 8
totals 10 24 7C+15 56
p. 35. The far right figure has been interpreted as female on the
grounds of full size but no facial hair.
p. 43. The first figure in the water has been interpreted as either
sex, as have the two beardless smaller figures in the top of the
scene as they could be women or children.
p. 45. There are two very small figures leaving the caves in the
background.
p. 47. The artist has been interpreted as either sex because the
hair/beard is unclear.
Moving on to the issue of written interpretations of gender roles,
Conkey & Spector (1984) showed how contemporary expectations colour
interpretations with no explicit discussion or supporting arguments.
Unsubstantiated statements and disparity of treatment persist. Andrews
& Stringer (1989) suggest the nuclear family existed in the
Palaeolithic: for the Zhoukoudian cave - 'they represent the
remains of hunters and their families' (1989: 38), and as the
caption$to an illustration of a female, child and man [ILLUSTRATION FOR
FIGURE 1C OMITTED] 'an African family of about 200,000 years
ago' (1989: 40). Yet noone has ever excavated proof of a Lower
Palaeolithic nuclear family! Interpreting graves and grave-goods can be
problematic (e.g. Parker Pearson 1982; Ucko 1969), but where indicators
of wealth and status are defined, they are not interpreted equally for
both sexes. Pearson et el. (1989) chose to use the number of non-ceramic
grave good types and the size of the grave pit as measures of wealth.
When some female graves are, by their chosen measures, wealthier than
male graves, the assessment is 'females are not inferior to males
on present evidence' (Pearson et el. 1989: 18-22).
No measure can overturn the assumption that the males
'should' show more wealth!
Expectations of relative gender status are again the crux in a last
example where female status is defined as the wealthiest using the
grave-goods and skeletal evidence, but explained (and isolated) as an
environmental anomaly and discussed in a surprised tone (Higham &
Bannanurag 1990: 55):
A society of 'Big Women'
This in essence is the 'Big Man' system. But at Khok Phanom
Di we find that the later generations were dominated numerically, and in
terms of wealth, by women. We believe that a closer examination of the
environment and the graves holds the key to understanding this most
unusual situation.
It is scarcely surprising that despite warnings against gender bias
from the present, and the careful use of terms (e.g. the replacement of
'early man' by 'human evolution' or 'early
hominids'), student essays often reflect the unsubstantiated gender
roles pervading so much of their sources for the human past. Lectures
may discuss Palaeolithic people, artefacts and tools, gathering and
scavenging - but the essays come back with Stone Age man using weapons
and hunting. The BIG problem is endemic and self-propagating.
To follow again the medical analogy, there are various possible
responses, ranging from treating the symptoms to antidotes, vaccines,
surgery or attacking the vectors of disease. Prevention is better than
cure, and the simple solutions may prove the most cost-effective. The
first step has to be isolating cause and effect, and understanding the
life-cycle of the disease.
Spread of the disease
Time plays an important part in the spread of the BIG problem due to
the reliance on and re-use of material and images. We have seen how
little has changed since the modern appreciation of gender issues.
Complacency over dated sources continues to spread the disease.
The illustrations by Maurice Wilson studied above were mostly painted
in the 1950s, yet they feature in a 1989 book and the gender roles they
depict influence perceptions today. Individual illustrations have been
re-used since the 1950s. The Neanderthal scene, dated 1950, has often
been reproduced, e.g. for a newspaper article in 1992, 42 years after it
was painted (Highfield 1992: 12). Archaeologists would not use written
sources from 40 years ago as uncritically as they do the illustrations.
A more modern set (1990) of visually striking illustrations by Benoit
Clarys has been produced as postcards and they will influence
present-day perceptions; I have seen them in four European countries and
on display in some museums. Analysis of gender representations by number
(TABLE 3) and by activity (TABLE 4) allows comparisons with
Wilson's illustrations (the four scenes are schematized in
[ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 2 OMITTED]). The ratio of recognizable images
of women to men is only 1:7, and the females are less active than the
males. Again the key reason is the selection of an activity deemed
important; the four scenes portrayed are of different types of hunting,
presented as a male activity. Progress has not been made in visual
gender representations!
Written academic sources show an increased modern avoidance of
gender-specific terms, e.g. 'modern humans' not 'modern
man' (Bar-Yosef & Vandermeersch 1993), and some books have
chosen to avoid the use of 'man' as a generic term for humans
of both sexes (e.g. Stringer & Gamble 1993). Elsewhere
'mankind' survives (Trinkaus & Shipman 1993). There is
some improvement here; some journals and publishers are aware of the
problem, although they weigh it against freedom of expression (see Evans
1990 and Chippindale 1991).
Cause and effect
The BIG problem is caused by three factors:
1 the present notions of gender roles affecting interpretations being
made today;
2 past notions of gender roles as they have affected previous
interpretations;
3 the lack of rigour in archaeological theory governing interpretive
frameworks.
Since the real gender bias lies with society rather than archaeology,
is archaeology to blame? Archaeologists inherit data excavated and
interpreted by the previous generation. If women did not achieve full
suffrage in Britain until 1929, it is not surprising that earlier
accounts reflect a society in which women had less power and status than
men. Archaeology and history are used to legitimize the present. Gender
bias should be taken seriously, not just for the sake of the past, but
for the political use that is made of the past in the present.
When interpreting the past we need to:
1 recognize explicitly our own notions of gender;
2 when reading, allow for the authors' contemporary notions of
gender;
3 be clear about our own interpretive processes linking gender and
behaviour/status.
These are by no means easy. In looking at the archaeology of gender
the first requirement is to identify biological sex, before analysing
the social construct of gender (Epstein 1988: 5-7; Gilchrist 1991). We
can bring clearer definitions of male/female via scientific techniques
(e.g. DNA on a cemetery population) to look at genetic relationships of
offspring and parents, but this evidence alone cannot provide an
understanding of male/female relationships. In particular, biological
notions of gender roles are allowed to 'explain' too much,
given that biologists and others debate the extent to which biological
sex or social learning create differences (Epstein 1988; Haraway 1989;
MacCormack & Strathern 1980; Ortner & Whitehead 1981; Seward
& Seward 1980; Shaver & Hendrick 1987). The means of observing
women in prehistory include ethnographic generalizations, skeletal and
mortuary studies, texts, art and mythology, physiology and comparative
zoology (Hayden 1992): my assessment has focussed more narrowly on
solely archaeological criteria. There are very limited circumstances
where archaeology can provide direct evidence of biological sex and
gender roles in prehistory. These are summarized below:
1 Skeletons
These can only be sexed in older individuals with sufficient
preservation to define sex via morphology of pelvis, skull, or long
bones (Workshop of European Anthropologists 1980), or some other tests
such as citrate presence (unreliable) or DNA analysis. There are periods
and regions in pro-history where the prevailing mortuary practices, or
lack of any, make the survival of whole skeletons very rare. On the
positive side, it is possible to look at nutritional differences,
genetic inheritance, trauma or other pathological evidence (e.g. Bush
& Zvelebil 1991 especially Meiklejohn & Zvelebil 1991; Holiman
1992; Roberts et al. 1989, especially Henderson 1989).
2 Contexts
The treatment of sexed skeletons and any objects placed with bodies
should offer useful [TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE 3 OMITTED] information.
Again, there are periods and regions where the funerary rite gives poor
sexing information and where no grave goods are used; where evidence
occurs there follows the classic problem of whether what goes into a
grave, accurately reflects life (Parker Pearson 1982).
3 Anthropomorphic images
Any differences in the means of depiction, activities depicted,
frequency or distribution between the sexes could be useful. Again, the
images may not be direct reflections of activities in life, and there is
the problem of how to sex images. Beards, breasts and genitals, the only
means of independently establishing male and female images, are often
unclear or masked by clothes. Where long hair, clothes or weapons are
utilized as the sexing evidence, the interpretations yet again reflect
contemporary social attitudes to gender. If genitalia are shown, it is
sometimes easier to identify male figures than female. Much imagery is
stylized so that the intellectual leap to understand that a scratched
triangle indicates a vulva may require a chronological and regional
knowledge. If the sex is not indicated, it could imply this is not an
important issue in the image context, and in some instances there may be
deliberate sexual ambiguity (e.g. Coles 1990; Kehoe 1991). These points
are of great interest in understanding gender roles and gender identity.
There is a dearth of evidence directly relating gender to objects or
activities, so the basis of any comments about gender should be
explicitly stated (see below). Even so, the caveats in a report are
unlikely to be repeated in the general or popular accounts which are
most read and provide the first references for students. By the time
students are reading the reports, they already have expectations. The
disease of gender bias is self-propagating.
male
1 holds handaxe 1 holds retouched stone tool 1 holds spear near dead
elephant 2 hold spears while searching 2 using spears and spearthrowers
TABLE 4. Activities for each figure whose gender was interpretable
according to TABLE 3.
Countermeasures
It is poor archaeological methodology that allows contemporary social
bias to be so uncritically reflected back on the past. Prevention is
better than cure but time, costs and perceptions affect the
countermeasures' success, This section analyses and advocates
responses to the BIG problem.
Antidotes - womens' books
There are now books focussing on women (Allason-Jones 1989; Boxer
& Quataert 1987; Ehrenberg 1989; Fell 1984; Klapisch-Zuber 1992).
Many females, and males, have eagerly read these as they redress the
imbalance. They are antidotes to the BIG problem, applied after the
event and neutralizing the symptoms. Books were produced with titles
such as The emergence of man (Pfeiffer 1970), Man the tool-maker (Oakley
1972); hunting or food-sharing and carrying were put forward as factors
in human evolution. On becoming human (Tanner 1981) was written as one
of many measured replies, Man the hunter (Lee & De Vore 1968) was a
useful conference volume whose title phrase became a cliche and
indirectly led to Woman the gatherer (Dahlberg 1981: xi), now also a
cliche.
Antibiotics - breaking down and attacking the disease by analysis
Conkey & Spector (1984) offered a critique of research choices
and values. Gilchrist (1991) and Engelstadt (1991) have both presented
feminist critiques of gender in archaeology. This kind of response
attacks not just the symptoms of the disease, but analyses the
components and build-up of the problem. Recent research has focussed on
the interaction of the gendering of archaeology as it is written and as
it is practised (Gero 1985; Claassen in press; Smith in press; Smith
& Du Cros in press). There are terms to describe different aspects
of gender (e.g. gender role, identity and ideology), and examples of the
problems arising from androcentric interpretations (e.g. Conkey &
Spector 1984: 15; Wylie 1991). More concern is also being shown by
organizations such as the Institute of Field Archaeologists (1991), and
by conferences such as the second 'Women in archaeology'
conference in Australia in July 1993. Analysing gender bias in field
data collection, interpretation and research question selection (e.g
Bell et al. 1993; Gero 1985; Gero & Conkey 1984; Ovrevik 1991)
highlights weak theoretical links and naive assumptions. More
specific-case-studies of interpretation and analysis include Rice (1981)
and Gilchrist (1993). These responses are antibiotics attacking the
causes of the BIG problem via analysis.
There is a problem with some feminist critiques: there is no reason
to suppose that women of the past million years shared the cultural
values of women in 1990s western societies. We will simply continue to
create the present in the past if we take this view, repeating the bias
pitfalls, but with the other sex emphasized. Biases in gender issues
need to be analytically countered rather than emotively rejected. In a
seminar discussion on hunter-gatherer societies (in which females are
more often the plant-collectors - a nutritionally and quantitatively
important aspect of the economy), some female students pointed out that
women could hunt; they thought to see women as plant-gatherers was
sexist. In arguing this, they denied some good reasons for this
generalized strategy; women breast-feeding children cannot leave
offspring for long periods, children can more easily contribute to their
own food-getting requirements if they collect static and usually
non-threatening plant resources, on a hunt children may not be able to
be kept quiet, to move quickly enough, or may slow down an adult too
much if they are carried. The age of children can affect a woman's
role in the food-getting strategy; the children may be left in the care
of elders of both sexes while younger and fitter community members go
off on longer trips. The variations are endless, based on age and sex
divisions within the community as a complex interweave of factors. To
say that such ideas are sexist is to miss the point of sexual dimorphism
as an evolutionary strategy and to be biased by our own cultural
experience of the status of activities. The female students wanted women
to be seen as hunters because this was the task they valued more. I
would argue that these female students over-valued the hunting
contribution and, in doing so, tried to deny generalized strategies
which are well documented (e.g. Hayden et al. 1986).
Clearly, what is needed are methods by which preconceived notions of
gender roles and status can separate evidence from interpretation, and
distinguish evidence of different roles from evidence of value. The rest
of this paper offers three approaches to challenge all gender
interpretations.
The sex-change operation - a mental gender switch
Unlike the medical analogue, this theoretical device is quick,
painless and reversible, so it could be one of the most effective
measures against gender bias. When an archaeologist makes an
interpretive statement$about a person or group of one sex and their
relationship to objects or society, they mentally switch the sex around
and examine whether their interpretation changes. If it does, then
further justification for the interpretation is needed. If the
difference cannot be justified, then it cannot be made!
A short example from a conference shows the value of a gender switch.
A study of skeletal data showed that evidence for bone breaks and
depressed skull fractures were more frequent in female skeletal remains.
The presented interpretation was that women had lesser status and were
dominated, and maltreated, by males. In subsequent conversation, the
mental sex-change operation was suggested. There is now a group of males
with more frequent breakages and depressed skull fractures, and the
hypothetical new interpretation was frankly stated - the men were
warriors and the traumas were the results of fighting! The type of
archaeological evidence has not changed, but cultural notions of male
and female directed the differing interpretations. In this example, the
palaeopathologist then offered further justification for the original
interpretation. The gender switch forced an awareness of other
possibilities and the citing of further information. It served to
challenge$preconceptions.
Hygiene education - destroying the breeding grounds
Terminology has a large role to play in re-educating our thoughts. As
Spender (1980) has pointed out, language is 'man made'. One of
the worst language problems is the use of the term 'man' as a
generic term for both sexes within our species. How can one word convey
either 'men and women' or 'men, not women'? The two
current meanings in the English language are contradictory and form a
persuasive argument for changing the accepted English usage of this
word. We make language, and word meanings can become obsolete.
Alternatives to the modern use of the generic term 'man'
exist, and they are less confusing. For 'early man' there are
'early hominids', 'human origins' or 'human
evolution'; for 'modern man', 'modern humans';
for 'Neolithic man' etc., 'people',
'folk', 'communities' or 'society', and
for 'man', 'humankind', 'humanity' or
'people'. With the increasing awareness of gender issues in
children's publications (e.g. Levick & Spencer 1993), the
academic world already seems slow to address gender-biased language.
However, the journal Man is, thankfully, changing its name. I am not
advocating the rewording of historical quotes (Chippindale 1991: 103),
nor compulsion. The use of terms should be a matter of choice, but
choices can be judged.
I would also suggest that many of our terms for social status, or
social relationships, are so laden with present-day perceptions of those
terms that we should find alternatives. Archaeologists would not call a
religious building a 'church' unless they thought the building
was being used to practise Christianity. 'Shrine' or
'temple' would be more appropriate terms because they would
not imply a particular religion, or character of ritual usage. In the
same way we should seek to use neutral terms for some social and gender
relationships, e.g. 'Big men' could become 'charismatic
leaders' or 'big' leaders. Many power terms have
gender-specified endings. 'Chieftain/chieftess' indicate
gender, but the term 'chieftess' can imply that the woman was
important because of her relationship to a chieftain, not in her own
right. The same holds true for 'Princess', and
'Queen'. Such terms are not gender equivalents to
'Prince' and 'King'. 'Ascribed' or
'achieved' leaders might convey a more open attitude to those
societies where women had political power. Other words such as
'principals' for small-state leaders might be appropriate.
'Wife' and 'husband' have so many cultural
implications in our own society that they cannot be used in a fresh way
in prehistory. Where female-male relationships are being discussed, it
may be wiser to use more neutral terms such as 'consort',
which refers to either sex and implies no concept of which sex might be
seen as the more important, nor any notion of the durability or number
of relationships an individual might have.
Many of the words which are gender-biased are so common in our own
social milieu that it is sometimes difficult to 'see' the way
we use gender-specific terms. We are as susceptible to cultural
indoctrination as anyone else. However, there are simple means to
develop a self-critical approach. Word-processors could be used to
examine the use of he/she, his/her, male/ female, man etc. via
'search' commands. Once identified systematically, such terms
could be individually examined to assess whether the text is a true
gender reflection, or whether there is a pattern of difference between
male/female nouns and the style of verbs or statements. This would
increase awareness of the problem and be a check on any unintentional
bias.
Visual sources - whether they involve the re-use of images or the
creation of new models - could be assessed as individual pieces and, as
importantly, as part of any set of images. The number of each sex, their
tasks and relative position and facial expression could all be analysed.
It would be a step forward to show where interpretations rather than
facts are being portrayed. A child skeleton on display at Avebury museum
is labelled 'Charlie or Charlotte', and at this museum there
is also a figure (male) with two opposing versions of dress and
sophistication. Both are examples of simple but effective devices to
show questions and to engage the viewer in the interpreting process. By
such means, the visual depictions of gender roles could be presented as
a matter for debate.
Vaccines - improving resistance by theory
Lastly, but by no means least, the underlying theoretical problem is
one of separating evidence from interpretation and in applying devices
to measure wealth, status and activities independently of our
expectations. This is where some theory developments in gender
archaeology would strengthen the whole discipline. In particular,
archaeologists of both sexes should realize that a difference in gender
roles is not synonymous with inequality. As archaeologists we should
look at roles and indicate differences where they occur, but also
realize that to attempt to give the societies' perceptions of the
status of these roles is a further step. It is in the perceived status
of different gender roles that bias can also occur against women. Some
of the reactions against androcentric interpretations are biasing the
evidence in favour of how some women would like to see the past. Like it
or not, there are fundamental biological and behavioural differences in
many societies between the sexes and at different age stages. To deny or
neglect that this might also have occurred in the past is to subvert the
discipline and undervalue the role of two sexes. It is the interaction
between sexes and the social creation of gender roles which are the
basic social division of human society. If this is not a fundamental
concern of archaeology, there is little point to the more often studied
technological, ritual, and political interactions within society. Gender
should be considered as an aspect of many of the key developments and
interpretations of society, but this is only recently beginning to
happen (e. g. the transition to farming, Jennbert in press;
metal-working, Barrett 1989; and households, Tringham 1991).
Conclusion - physician, heal thyself
The relationship between gender groups is ultimately what propagates
society, and our species. Gender studies are thus perVasive and should
be critical aspects of archaeological research. It is not sufficient to
say that now we know of the BIG problem it will disappear. The recent
examples quoted show this is not the case. Many of the gender concepts
we have are a product of personal upbringing, popular accounts and dated
academic discussion. These influences cannot suddenly be overturned by a
few academic articles, even if we accept them. To argue against accepted
cultural norms requires self-criticism at a conscious level in order to
re-frame concepts and the discipline. The BIG problem exists because of
fundamental flaws in archaeology. The separation of evidence from
interpretation; the distinction of difference from relative status; the
use of clear and neutral terms and the questioning of our preconceptions
are all reasoned objectives for the discipline; not dismissable as
fashionable PC hype. The theoretical devices I have put forward are aids
to self-criticism within archaeology. Physician heal thyself!
Finally, interpretations of genders need justification but they do
need to be made. One neuter category does not give a viable past any
more than does an imbalance between genders. At the moment the
archaeological past has too few women to form a viable population. Time,
ladies and gentlemen please, for the creation of a viable population of
the archaeological past to save our own engendered species.
Acknowledgements. This paper had a long gestation period; thanks go
to Sue Rouillard at Exeter for producing the illustrations, Jackie
Mulville, Barbara Brayshay, Andrew Chamberlain, Robin Dennell, Marek
Zvelebil and Bert Sinclair for discussion, Laurajane Smith and Stephanie
Moser for references. Its present format owes much to my experiences of
teaching gender issues to students and to the person who referred to
gender issues as 'babble'. My purpose in writing this paper
with so many examples was to clarify the issues of our engendered past
for these students and sceptics.
References
ALLASON-JONES, L. 1989. Women in Roman Britain. London: British
Museum Publications.
ANDREWS, P. & C. STRINGER. 1989. Human evolution: an illustrated
guide, with paintings by Maurice Wilson. London: British Museum (Natural
History).
BARRETT, J.C. 1989. Food, gender and metal: questions of social
reproduction, in M.-L. Stig Sorensen & R. Thomas (ed.), The Bronze
Age-Iron Age transition in Europe: 304-20. Oxford: British
Archaeological Reports. International series S483(ii).
BAR-YOSEF, O. & B. VANDERMEERSCH. 1993. Modern humans in the
Levant, Scientific American 264(4): 64-70.
BEDLOW, R. 1991. 20,000 went to Bronze Age orgy, The Daily Telegraph
14 October.
BELL, D., P. CAPLAN & W.J. KARIM (ed.). 1993. Gendered fields:
women, men and ethnography. London: Routledge.
BENDER, B. 1978. Gatherer-hunter to farmer: a social perspective,
World Archaeology 10: 204-22.
BLUMENSCHINE, R.J. & J.A. CAVALLO, 1992, Scavenging and human
evolution, Scientific American 267(4): 90-96.
BOXER, M,J. & J.H. QUATAERT (Ed.). 1987, Connecting spheres:
women in the western world, 1500 to the present. New York (NY): Oxford
University Press.
BURTT, F. 1987. 'Man the hunter': bias in children's
archaeology books, Archaeological Review from Cambridge 6(2): 157-74.
BUSH, H. & M, ZVELEBIL (ed.). 1991. Health in past societies:
biocultural interpretation of human skeletal remains in archaeological
contexts. Oxford: British Archaeological Reports. International series
S567.
CHIPPINDALE, C. 1991. Sexist language in archaeological discourse: a
reply, Archaeological Review from Cambridge 10(1): 102-4.
CLAASSEN, C. (ed,). 1992. Exploring gender through archaeology.
Madison (WI): Prehistory Press.
In press. Feminism and CRM. Boone (NC): Appalachian State University.
COLES, B. 1990. Anthropomorphic wooden figurines from Britain and
Ireland, Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society 56: 315-33.
Concise Oxford Dictionary of current English. 1990. 8th edition.
Oxford: Clarendon Press.
CONKEY, M.W. & J. SPECTOR. 1984. Archaeology and the study of
gender, in M.B. Schiffer (ed.), Advances in archaeological method and
theory 7: 1-38. London: Academic Press.
DAHLBERG, F. (ed.). 1981. Woman the gatherer. New Haven (CT): Yale
University Press.
DUNBAR, R. & R. FOLEY. 1989. Beyond the bones of contention, New
Scientist 124 (1686) (14 October 1989): 37-40.
EHRENBERG, M. 1989. Women in prehistory, London: British Museum.
ENGELSTAD, E. 1991. Images of power and contradiction: feminist
theory and post-processual archaeology, Antiquity 65: 502-14.
EPSTEIN, C.F. 1988. Deceptive distinctions: sex, gender, and the
social order. New Haven (CT): Yale University Press.
EVANS, K. 1990. Sexist language in archaeological discourse,
Archaeological Review from Cambridge 9(2): 252-61.
FELL, C. 1984. Women in Anglo-Saxon England. London: British Museum
Publications.
GERO, J.M. 1985. Socio-politics and the woman-at-home ideology,
American Antiquity 50: 342-50.
GERO, J.M. & M.W. CONKEY (ed). 1991. Engendering archaeology:
women and prehistory. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
GILCHRIST, R. 1991. Women's archaeology? Political feminism,
gender theory and historical revision, Antiquity 65: 495-501.
1993. Gender and material culture. London: Routledge.
GOULD, S.J. 1989. Wonderful life: the Burgess Shale and the nature of
history. London: Penguin.
HARAWAY, D. (Ed.). 1989. Primate visions: gender, race and nature in
the world of modern science. New York (NY): Routledge.
HAYDEN, B. 1986. Ecological determinants of womens' status among
hunter-gatherers, Human Evolution 1: 449-73.
1992. Observing prehistoric women, in Claassen (ed.): 33-47.
HENDERSON, J. 1989. Pagan Saxon cemeteries: a study of the problem of
sexing by grave goods and bones, in Roberts et al. (ed.): 77-83.
HIGHAM, C. & R. BANNANURAG. 1990. The princess and the pots, New
Scientist 126(1718) (26 May): 50-55.
HIGHFIELD, R. 1992. Why toolmakers looked so happy, Daily Telegraph
15 June: 12.
HOLLIMAN, S.E. 1992. Health consequences of sexual division of labor
among native Americans: the Chumash of California and the Arikara of the
Northern Plains, in Claassen (ed.): 81-88.
INSTITUTE OF FIELD ARCHAEOLOGISTS. 1991. Women in British archaeology
- the equal opportunities in archaeology working party report 1991, The
Field Archaeologist 15: 280-82.
JENNBERT, K. In press. 'From the inside': a contribution to
the debate about the introduction of agriculture, in L. Domanska, R.
Dennell & M. Zvelebil (ed.), The transition to farming in the Baltic
region. Oxford: Oxbow.
JONES, S. & S. PAY. 1990. The legacy of Eve, in P. Gathercole
& D. Lowenthal (ed.), The politics of the past: 160-71. London:
Unwin Hyman.
KEHOE, A.B. 1991. No possible, probable shadow of doubt, Antiquity
65: 129-31.
KLAPISCH-ZUBER, C. (ed.). 1992. A history of women in the West 2:
Silences of the Middle Ages. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.
LAMBERT, D. 1987. The Cambridge guide to prehistoric man, Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
LEE, R.B. & I. DE VORE (Ed.). 1968. Man the hunter. Chicago (IL):
Aldine.
LEVICK, C. & B. SPENCER. 1993, Windows on the world: prehistory.
London: Dorling Kindersley.
MACCORMACK, C. & M. STRATHERN (ed.). 1980. Nature, culture and
gender, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
MEIKLEJOHN, C. & M. ZVELEBIL. 1991. Health status of European
populations at the agricultural transition and the implications for the
adoption of farming, in Bush & Zvelebil (ed.): 129-45.
MOSER, S. 1992a. The visual language of archaeology: a case study of
the Neanderthals, Antiquity 66: 83144.
1992b. Visions of the Australian Pleistocene: prehistoric life at
Lake Mungo and Kutikina, Australian Archaeology 5: 1-10.
In press. Gender stereotyping in pictorial reconstructions of human
origins, in Smith & Du Cros (ed.).
OAKLEY, K. 1972. Man the toolmaker. 6th edition. London: British
Museum (Natural History).
ORTNER, S.B. & H. WHITEHEAD (ed.). 1981. Sexual meanings: the
cultural construction of gender and sexuality. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press
OVREVIK, S.E. 1991. Feminist research on archaeology in Norway,
Kvinner i Arkeologi i Norge 11: 27-63.
PARKER PEARSON, M. 1982. Mortuary practices, societies and ideology:
an ethnoarchaeological study, in I. Hodder (ed.), Symbolic and
structural archaeology: 99-114. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
PEARSON, R., J.-W. LEE, W. KOH & A. UNDERHILL. 1989, Social
ranking in the kingdom of Old Silla, Korea: analysis of burials, Journal
of Anthropological Archaeology 8: 1-50.
PFEIFFER, J.E. 1970. The emergence of man. London: Nelson.
RICE, P. 1981. Prehistoric venuses: symbols of motherhood or
womanhood, Journal of Anthropological Research 37: 402-14.
ROBERTS, C., F. LEE & J. BINTLIFF (ed.). 1989, Burial
archaeology: current research. methods and developments. Oxford: British
Archaeological Reports. British series 211.
SEWARD, J. & G.H. SEWARD. 1980. Sex differences: mental and
temperamental. Lexington (KY): Lexington Books.
SHAVER, P. & C. HENDRICK (ed.). 1987, Sex and gender. Newbury
Park (CA): Sage.
SMITH, L. In press. Cultural resource management and the rise of
feminist expression in Australian archaeology, in Claassen (ed.).
SMITH, L. & H. DU CROS. In press. Women in archaeology: a
feminist critique. Canberra: Australian National University Press.
SPENDER, D. 1980. Man made language. London: Routledge & Kegan
Paul.
STRINGER, C. & C. GAMBLE. 1993. In search of the Neanderthals.
London: Thames & Hudson.
TANNER, N. 1981. On becoming human. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
TATTERSALL, I. 1992. Evolution comes to life, Scientific American
267(2): 62-9.
TRINGHAM, R.E. 1991. Households with faces: the challenge of gender
in prehistoric architectural remains, in Gero & Conkey (ed.):
93-131.
TRINKAUS, E. 1983. The Shanidar Neandertals. New York (NY): Academic
Press.
TRINKAUS, E. & P. SHIPMAN. 1993. The Neandertals, changing the
image of mankind. London: Jonathan Cape.
UCKO, P. 1969. Ethnography and archaeological interpretation of
funerary remains, World Archaeology 1: 262-80.
WALCOTT, C.D. 1912. Middle Cambrian Branchiapoda, Malacostraca,
Trilobita and Merostomata: Cambrian geology and paleontology, II,
Smithsonian Miscellaneous Collections 57: 145-228.
WYLIE, A. 1991. Gender theory and the archaeological record: why is
there no archaeology of gender, in Gero & Conkey (ed.): 31-54
WORKSHOP OF EUROPEAN ANTHROPOLOGISTS. 1980. Recommendations for age
and sex diagnoses of skeletons, Journal of Human Evolution 9: 517-49.